Skip to main content
Full access
Editorial
Published Online: 1 March 2002

Considering Disruptive Behaviors

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry
Problems with oppositional, aggressive, and disruptive behaviors have long been of concern to mental health professionals; they continue to be among the most common problems in children referred for mental health intervention (1). Such behaviors are the source of much distress to the child as well as to the family and society, can be associated with a host of other difficulties (1), and, as the classic work of Robins and colleagues has shown (2, 3), are highly persistent. Despite the clear importance of these difficulties, their classification has been somewhat controversial, with the international (ICD) and American (DSM) systems taking somewhat different, if overlapping, approaches (4). DSM-IV draws a distinction between conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder, whereas ICD-10 defines conduct disorder as an amalgam of the two DSM-IV disorders. Further difficulties are raised by the complex question of comorbidity—the strong association of problems in conduct with attentional and other problems (5). Two articles in this issue address aspects of disruptive behavior disorders in children.
The review article by Keenan and Wakschlag is concerned with whether valid diagnoses of disruptive behavior disorders can be made in preschool children. They note that such problems are common reasons for referral to mental health clinics in this age group, and yet, somewhat paradoxically, there has been some controversy about how validly these diagnoses can be made in these children. This controversy reflects several factors, including a concern for premature (and possibly inappropriate) labeling, difficulties in dealing with developmental aspects of disruptive behavior, and potential difficulties in the applicability of the DSM-IV criteria for conditions that, in some respects, are clearly more oriented toward older children and adolescents. Keenan and Wakschlag propose a developmental approach and suggest that the degree of interference with normal developmental functioning can help distinguish the clinical condition of oppositional defiant disorder from more normative developmental issues involving oppositionality and increasing autonomy. For conduct disorder, some of the same developmental considerations apply. For example, learning to deal with aggression and its expression is a normative developmental task. A second issue in validity of the conduct disorder diagnosis is the idea that a child with the disorder must understand and thus knowingly violate societal rules. As the authors note, otherwise typically developing children have some basic understanding of rule violation as early as 3 years of age. On the other hand, studies of moral development suggest that major changes in this area occur throughout childhood. For preschool children, there is at least some evidence for the validity of these conditions, although interpretation of the data is compromised to some extent by the relative dearth of studies, the difficulties in diagnosis (e.g., given the orientation of the DSM-IV criteria to somewhat older children), and the tendency to lump oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder together in research and clinical reports. It is important to note that early-onset conduct problems may be even more persistent than later-onset ones (6, 7). Keenan and Wakschlag suggest that issues of assessment in children in this age group need to be carefully considered, particularly since young children with conduct problems seem to be at greater risk for persistent and more widespread difficulties (6). Similarly, additional work (8) has supported the effectiveness of an array of intervention techniques, including parent-focused, youth-focused, and system-based interventions, thus making early identification even more relevant.
In the second of this issue’s articles on disruptive behavior disorders, Wakefield and colleagues address a somewhat different problem—whether the diagnostic criteria for conduct disorder should consider social context. Their study found that, in a contrived experimental situation, mental health professional can incorporate consideration of social context into their clinical judgment of conduct disorder. This issue is important given DSM-IV’s text discussion of conduct disorder, which states that a diagnosis of the disorder should not be made if the symptoms are simply a reaction to a negative environment. As Wakefield and colleagues point out, the actual DSM-IV criteria for conduct disorder do not incorporate this exclusion. Thus clinicians who rely solely on the criteria to make a diagnosis (i.e., who fail to read the text) may not take special circumstances into account. The authors note the long history of concern about this problem. I am also reminded of a colleague, the late Dr. Donald Cohen, who, during a meeting of the DSM-IV childhood disorders work group, suggested only half facetiously that the word “THINK” be printed in large, boldface type at the head of each page of DSM-IV! As Wakefield and colleagues report, clinicians’ judgment that a child needs treatment as a result of a disordered environment does not necessarily imply that the child has a disorder.
These two articles highlight the advances made in understanding conduct disorder and other disruptive behavior disorders, as well as the significant issues that remain to be addressed. These issues include continuity over time (e.g., with adult disorders), the nature of risk and protective factors, and our understanding of causal factors, including theoretical models for the pathogenesis of these conditions. As Hill (1) suggested, there is a considerable need to broaden our conceptual/theoretical understanding. The articles by Keenan and Wakschlag and Wakefield and colleagues in this issue help further this goal.

Footnote

Address reprint requests to Dr. Volkmar, Yale University Child Study Center, 230 South Frontage Rd., P.O. Box 207900, New Haven, CT 06520-7900; [email protected] (e-mail).

References

1.
Hill J: Biological, psychological, and social processes in conduct disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002; 43:133-164
2.
Robins LN: Deviant Children Grown Up: A Sociological and Psychiatric Study of Sociopathic Personalities. Baltimore, Williams & Wilkins, 1991
3.
Robins LN, Tipp J, Pryzbeck T: Antisocial personality, in Psychiatric Disorder in America: The Epidemiological Catchment Area Study. Edited by Robins LN, Regier D. New York, Free Press, 1991, pp 258-290
4.
Angold A, Costello EJ: The epidemiology of disorders of conduct: nosological issues and comorbidity, in Conduct Disorders in Childhood and Adolescence. Edited by Hill J, Maughan B. New York, Cambridge University Press, 2000, pp 126-168
5.
Angold A, Costello EJ, Erkanli A: Comorbidity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999; 40:57-87
6.
Moffit T, Caspi A, Dickson N, Silva P, Stanton W: Childhood onset vs. adolescent onset antisocial conduct problems in males: natural history from ages 3 to 18 years. Dev Psychopathol 1996; 8:399-424
7.
Loeber R: The stability of antisocial and delinquent child behaviour: a review. Child Dev 1982; 6:321-357
8.
Weersing VR, Weisz JR: Mechanisms of action in youth psychotherapy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002; 43:3-29

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 349 - 350
PubMed: 11869994

History

Published online: 1 March 2002
Published in print: March 2002

Authors

Details

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share