Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective: The limbic system is thought to underlie dysfunctional affective and cognitive processes in individuals with depression. Neuroanatomical studies of subjects with depression have often examined hippocampal and amygdalar structures, since they are two key structures of the limbic system. Research has often but not always found reduced hippocampal volume in patients with major depression. The purpose of the present study was to examine differences in hippocampal and amygdalar volumes in patients with depression subtypes relative to healthy comparison subjects. Method: Participants were 1) patients with major depression with psychosis, 2) patients with major depression without psychosis, and 3) healthy comparison subjects. To examine hippocampal and amygdalar volumes, all participants underwent structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). The authors further examined the effects of clinical and chronicity data on these two brain structures. Results: After age, gender, and total brain volume were controlled, depressed patients with psychosis had a significantly smaller mean amygdala volume relative to depressed patients without psychosis and healthy comparison subjects. There were no differences between depressed patients without psychosis and healthy comparison subjects. Correlational analyses suggested that age of depression onset was strongly associated with amygdala volume. No group differences in hippocampal volume were found. Conclusions: There were no differences between depressed patients and healthy comparison subjects in hippocampal volume. However, psychotic but not nonpsychotic depression was associated with reduced amygdala volume. Reduced amygdala volume was not associated with severity of depression or severity of psychosis but was associated with age at onset of depression. Smaller amygdala volume may be a risk factor for later development of psychotic depression. In addition, chronicity of depression and depression subtype might be two important factors associated with hippocampal and amygdalar volumes in depression.
Many studies have attempted to identify and clarify the roles of specific brain regions and networks in the pathogenesis of cognitive and affective symptoms of depression. Particular emphasis has been placed on the limbic system, specifically the hippocampus and amygdala, which are connected to other cortical areas such as the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and hypothalamus (1) . The limbic system controls emotion and mood regulation. Specifically, hippocampal formation plays a critical role in the regulation of motivation and emotion, and the amygdala is vital to the interpretation of emotional and motivational experiences (1) . The hippocampus and amygdala are two brain regions that are thought to underlie dysfunctional affective and cognitive processes in depression.
Although the hippocampus is one of the most studied structures in affective disorders research, data are inconsistent as to whether depressed individuals have smaller hippocampal volume compared with nondepressed individuals. Some researchers have reported decreased hippocampal volume in depressed patients relative to comparison subjects (2, 3), while others have found no group differences (4, 5) . Two recent reviews examined the extant literature on hippocampal volume. A meta-analysis conducted by Videbech and Ravnkilde (6) confirmed hippocampal volume loss in patients with depression and found a significant correlation between the number of depressive episodes and right hippocampal volume. Similarly, Campbell and MacQueen (7) reported decreased volume of the hippocampus in depressed subjects but no alterations in amygdala volume. Thus, both reviews suggested smaller hippocampal volume in patients with recurrent depression relative to age- and sex-matched comparison groups.
Few studies using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) have examined hippocampal volume in subjects with major depression with psychosis. Kim et al. (8) examined the amygdala-hippocampal complex in older depressed subjects and found no differences between depressed subjects with or without psychosis. However, they did not include a healthy comparison group. More recently, Velakoulis et al. (9) examined various types and stages of psychosis. Their sample of affective patients with psychosis (N=34), which included patients with bipolar and unipolar disorders, did not demonstrate hippocampal reduction in patients with depression relative to comparison subjects. Hence, although hippocampal volume reduction has been noted in nonpsychotic depression—albeit inconsistently—the few available studies have suggested that a comparable volume reduction may not be present in psychotic depression.
Several investigations have examined the amygdala in depressed patients, with mixed results. Increased amygdala volume has been observed in first-episode major depression (10) and dysthymia (11) . However, amygdala atrophy has been reported in female but not male depressed patients (12) and in chronic depression (13), and it has been suggested that depressed individuals have a smaller core amygdala nucleus relative to individuals who have never been diagnosed as depressed (12, 13) . Furthermore, smaller amygdala volume, but intact hippocampal volume, has been reported in pediatric depression (14) . A recent study found that patients with first-episode psychosis (including schizophrenia, psychotic depression, and other psychotic disorders) had a larger mean amygdala volume relative to healthy comparison subjects (9) . In contrast, the study did not find any amygdala reduction in patients with first-episode schizophrenia, chronic schizophrenia, or chronic affective psychosis. Other studies did not find altered amygdala volume in nonpsychotic depression (15, 16) or in geriatric psychotic depression (8) .
Demographic and clinical variables may be crucial in determining which patients are at greatest risk for reduced hippocampal volume. A number of variables have been found to be correlated with hippocampal volumetric changes, including age, gender (17), recurrence (18), duration of illness (19), refractoriness (16), history of abuse (3), age of depression onset (7), and cortisol dysregulation (19) . However, many brain volume studies have not examined the relationship of these clinical variables with volume reduction. Furthermore, most studies have not differentiated subtypes of depression (e.g., psychotic depression), although differing results have been found in subjects with unipolar depression relative to subjects with bipolar depression. These variables may play important roles in the mixed results regarding hippocampal and amygdalar volumes in unipolar major depression.
The primary objective of the present study was to compare hippocampal and amygdalar volumes in patients with psychotic and nonpsychotic depression and in healthy comparison subjects. To our knowledge, none of the extant structural imaging studies has examined psychotic depression in nonelderly subjects. Based on our previous studies that examined cognition in depression (20, 21), which found significant memory impairment in depressed patients with but not without psychosis, we predicted that hippocampal volume differences would be present in patients with major depression with psychosis, relative to comparison subjects, but not in patients with major depression without psychosis.

Method

Participants

Participants were either recruited from outpatient facilities at Stanford University or self-referred via online and print advertisements. Twenty-three patients with psychotic depression and 19 patients with nonpsychotic depression participated in the study and provided usable data. Subjects with major depression with psychosis were diagnosed with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) (22) . The diagnoses were confirmed by consultation with the subjects’ treating psychiatrists. Subjects with major depression without psychosis were also assessed using the SCID.
Depressed patients with and without psychosis were required to have a minimum score of 18 on the 21-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) (23) and a minimum score of 7 on the Thase Core Endogenomorphic Scale (24), an assessment scale derived from the HAM-D. The latter was administered to ensure that patients with major depression without psychosis demonstrated considerable endogenous symptoms. Subjects with major depression with psychosis were also required to have a minimum score of 5 on the positive symptom subscale of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS [25] ), which consists of the following four items: conceptual disorganization, suspiciousness, hallucinations, and unusual thought content. A score of 4 indicates no positive symptoms. All patients met DSM-IV criteria for a current unipolar major depressive episode, with or without psychotic features. Subjects with major depression without psychosis had no history of psychotic symptoms. Participants were allowed to continue their psychiatric medication but were required to maintain a stable medication regimen for at least 1 week prior to the start of the study.
Twenty-two healthy comparison subjects were recruited through online and print advertisements. Comparison subjects were required to have a score of less than 6 on the HAM-D and have no psychotic symptoms. Additionally, they were required to have no current or past axis I psychiatric disorders according to SCID criteria.
Exclusion criteria for all three groups were major medical illnesses, history of seizures or major head trauma, abnormal clinical laboratory tests, pregnant or lactating women, and individuals under age 18. Participants were excluded if they had unstable or untreated hypertension, cardiovascular disease, or endocrine disorders. Subjects being treated with steroids or hormone replacement therapy were also not included. Additionally, patients who were actively suicidal or met DSM-IV criteria for obsessive-compulsive disorder or bipolar disorder were excluded as well as those patients with a history of substance abuse or electroconvulsive therapy within the 6 months prior to the start of the study. All participants were compensated $250.

Procedure

After complete description of the study was given, written informed consent was obtained. Eligibility screening procedures included the following: SCID, HAM-D, BPRS, clinical laboratory tests (with complete blood count examination), comprehensive metabolic panel, urine drug screening, and urine screening for pregnancy in female subjects. If participants met inclusion criteria at the eligibility screening, they were asked to return for baseline procedures. All eligible participants underwent structural and functional MRI. In addition, all eligible subjects participated in neuropsychological testing, psychological interviews, and blood sampling for 2 days (these findings are reported elsewhere [ 20, 26 ]).

MRI Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

Structural images were acquired on a 3T GE Signa scanner, using a standard GE whole-head coil. The scanner runs on an LX platform, with gradients in “Mini-CRM” configuration (35 mT/m, SR 190 mT/m/second), and has a Magnex 3T 80-cm magnet. The following three-dimensional, volumetric radio frequency, spoiled-gradient echo parameters were used: TR=35 msec, echo time=6 msec, flip angle=45°. The field of view was 240×240 mm 2, and the acquisition matrix size was 256×192. One hundred twenty-four scan locations were acquired at 1.5-mm slice thickness. MRI scans were imported into BrainImage software for semiautomated whole-brain segmentation and quantification in the coronal plane (27, 28) .
The amygdala and hippocampus were manually delineated in the coronal plane for each participant according to previously described methods (29), using grayscale volume stacks derived from whole-brain analysis. These data sets were standardized in their orientation, parallel to the axial plane passing through the anterior and posterior commissures, using a six-parameter rigid-body transformation. A Talairach-based grid (30) was subsequently fit to each data set in order to provide an additional method for subdividing regions of interest. The resolution of the standardized coronal data sets was then increased from 256×256 to 512×512, using a bicubic interpolation algorithm to increase visualization accuracy. The gray and white matter constituent volumes of the resulting amygdala and hippocampus regions of interest were quantified by projecting these regions of interest (derived from a nonsegmented grayscale image) on to the segmented gray and white matter fraction stacks of each subject. The reliability of the hippocampus and amygdala regions of interest was established by achieving an intraclass correlation coefficient of  >0.90.
The anterior border of the amygdala was defined as the coronal plane where the anterior commissure first crossed the midline of the brain and where the amygdala was clearly present. The entorhinal sulcus and a white matter tract comprised the superior and inferior borders, respectively. Medial and lateral borders were defined by CSF in the medial margin of the temporal lobe and a lateral white matter tract. The posterior border of the amygdala was defined by the appearance of the hippocampus.
The anterior border of the hippocampus was defined by the alveus or the hippocampal sulcus. The alveus marked the superior border, and white matter defined the inferior border. The temporal horn demarcated the lateral border, and the ambient cistern demarcated the medial border. Measurement of the hippocampus proceeded posteriorly until the corpus callosum first fused with the fornix. Anterior and posterior subdivision of the hippocampus were achieved using the Talairach E3/F coronal plane (30), which was located approximately midway along the length of the hippocampus.

Statistical Analyses

For all statistical tests, a two-tailed alpha of 0.05 was chosen as the threshold for statistical significance. Initial data analyses consisted of univariate analysis of variance and chi square analyses examining basic demographic differences between the three groups. Univariate analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was then used to detect group differences in hippocampal and amygdala volumes. A custom model was established based on literature suggesting that age and gender are variables that may contribute to volume differences in brain regions. Independent factors were gender and diagnostic group, and covariables were total brain volume and age. In addition to the full model examining the interaction of group-by-gender, we added the group-by-age interaction to our model. Group-effect differences were then examined using simple-effects ANCOVAs.
Next, we examined the effects of clinical and chronicity variables on brain volume. Clinical variables included the total scores for the following assessments: HAM-D, BPRS, and the BPRS positive symptom subscale. Unfortunately, chronicity data were only available for approximately one-half of our sample, equally distributed across the major depression with psychosis and major depression without psychosis groups. Post hoc analyses comparing the depressed groups were conducted using available chronicity data, including age at onset, number of past major depressive episodes, length of current major depressive episode, and number of patients with recurrent depression.

Results

Demographics

Results showed no group differences in age, education, or gender ( Table 1 ). There were significant differences in total HAM-D (F=486.3, df=2, 61, p<0.001) and BPRS (F=213.7, df=2, 61, p<0.001) scores. In both assessments, patients with major depression with psychosis had higher scores relative to patients with major depression without psychosis, and depressed patients without psychosis had higher scores relative to healthy comparison subjects. There were also differences in BPRS positive symptom subscale scores (F=92.2, df=2, 61, p<0.001), with depressed patients with psychosis scoring significantly higher than depressed patients without psychosis and healthy comparison subjects. No differences were found between depressed patients without psychosis and healthy comparison subjects. There were no differences between the two depressed groups for several variables, including age at onset, number of past depressive episodes, length of current major depressive episode, and recurrence ( Table 1 ). These results suggest that, relative to depressed patients without psychosis, depressed patients with psychosis were more severely ill at the time of scanning but did not have a more chronic course of depression. In addition, differences in medication use were found between the two depressed groups ( Table 2 ). The number of depressed patients with psychosis who were taking antipsychotics (χ 2 =21.4, df=1, p<0.001) and anxiolytics (χ 2 =5.5, df=1, p=0.019) was higher than that of depressed patients without psychosis. No group differences were found for antidepressant or mood stabilizer use.

Amygdala Volume

ANCOVAs indicated significant group differences in amygdala volume. After controlling for the effects of age, gender, and total brain volume and the interaction of age and gender by group, we observed that right amygdala volume (F=8.55, df=2, 54, p=0.001) and left amygdala volume (F=5.2, df=2, 54, p=0.009) differentiated the three study groups. Post hoc two-group ANCOVAs revealed that depressed patients with psychosis had smaller right and left amygdala volumes relative to healthy comparison subjects, and they had marginally smaller right and left amygdala volumes relative to depressed patients without psychosis (left amygdala: p=0.10; right amygdala: p=0.08). Depressed patients without psychosis did not differ from healthy comparison subjects in either right or left amygdala volume ( Table 3, Figure 1 ).
Figure 1. Adjusted Residual Volumes in Right and Left Amygdala and Hippocampus in Depressed Patients With and Without Psychosis and Healthy Comparison Subjects
Significant group-by-age interactions were observed in both left (F=4.47, df=2, 54, p=0.016) and right (F=5.92, df=2, 54, p=0.005) amygdala volumes. Two-group ANCOVAs revealed that group-by-age interaction was significant between depressed patients with psychosis and healthy comparison subjects in right (F=15.53, df=1, 38, p<0.001) and left (F=9.90, df=1, 38, p=0.003) amygdala volumes. However, this interaction was not significant between depressed patients with and without psychosis or between depressed patients without psychosis and healthy comparison subjects in either the left or right amygdala. To further understand this interaction, we performed a median split for age and categorized subjects as either young (age <35.5 years) or old (age >35.5 years) ( Figure 2 ). Post hoc comparisons indicated that depressed patients with psychosis who were categorized as young had significantly (p=0.001) smaller right amygdala volume (mean=2.16 [SD=0.38]) relative to young healthy comparison subjects (mean=2.59 [SD=0.39]). Post hoc comparisons also indicated that there was a tendency (p=0.06) for smaller left amygdala volume in younger depressed patients with psychosis (mean=2.50 [SD=0.43]) relative to younger healthy comparison subjects (mean=2.68 [SD=0.43]). There were no significant differences in left or right amygdala volume in older depressed patients with psychosis relative to older healthy comparison subjects.
Figure 2. Amygdala Volume/Total Brain Volume Ratio by Diagnostic Group and Age a
a Median age split: young, <35.5 years; old, >35.5 years.

Hippocampal Volume

We found no significant group differences in left or right hippocampal volume ( Table 3, Figure 1 ). Furthermore, there were no effects of gender (F=2.08, df=1, 54), age (F=0.096, df=1, 54), age-by-group interaction (F=1.20, df=2, 54), or gender-by-group interaction (F=1.97, df=2, 54) for the left hippocampus. Additionally, there were no effects of age (F=0.41, df=1, 54), gender (F=3.51, df=1, 54, p=0.06), age-by-group interaction (F=0.91, df=2, 54), or gender-by-group interaction (F=1.46, df=2, 54) for the right hippocampus. No between-group differences were observed after subdividing the hippocampus into anterior and posterior subregions.

Clinical and Chronicity Data

Correlations between clinical variables were examined separately for depressed patients with and without psychosis. Interestingly, there were significant correlations between the total BPRS score and left (r=–0.526, p=0.021) and right (r=–0.591, p=0.008) amygdala volumes in depressed patients without psychosis, but there were no significant correlations between the total score and amygdala volume in depressed patients with psychosis. Additionally, there were no significant correlations between this clinical variable and hippocampal volume for either depressed group.
Next, we examined—in a post hoc manner—the relationship between chronicity and amygdalar and hippocampal volumes separately for both depressed groups. Significant correlations were observed in depressed patients with psychosis between right amygdala volume and 1) the length of current major depressive episode (r= –0.537, p=0.04), 2) age at first major depressive episode (r=0.849, p<0.001), and 3) the number of past depressive episodes (r=–0.613, p=0.012). In depressed patients with psychosis, time since the first major depressive episode was correlated with left hippocampal volume (r=–0.646, p=0.03), and age at first major depressive episode was significantly correlated with left (r=0.838, p=0.001) and right (r=0.730, p=0.011) hippocampal volumes. This finding suggests a strong relationship between age at onset and hippocampal volume, with younger age at onset associated with smaller hippocampi. There were no significant correlations in depressed patients without psychosis with amygdalar or hippocampal volume.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the largest study to examine amygdalar and hippocampal volume differences separately in nonelderly subjects with major depression with psychosis relative to nonelderly subjects with major depression without psychosis. Although the depressed patients with psychosis in our study were more severely depressed compared with those patients without psychosis, the two depressed groups did not differ on measures of chronicity or age at onset of depression. Contrary to the conclusions of recent meta-analyses (6, 7), we did not find left or right hippocampal volume differences in patients with psychotic or nonpsychotic depression relative to healthy comparison subjects. Our effect sizes suggest that this finding was not simply the result of a lack of statistical power. One meta-analysis (6), which examined nonpsychotic depression, found recurrent chronic depression to be an important variable that influences brain volume. Our samples did not differ on measures of recurrent chronic depression, which may be one reason that we observed no differences in hippocampal volume between the two depressed groups. Alternatively, it is possible that the finding of no group differences in our study, as well as in the data reported by other investigators, may be related to the rating scales used for sample assessment or the sensitivity of the methodology used in quantifying hippocampal size.
Interestingly, our results suggest that psychotic, but not nonpsychotic, depression is associated with reduced amygdala volume. Depressed patients with psychosis had smaller right and left amygdala volumes relative to depressed patients without psychosis and healthy comparison subjects. In addition, the two latter groups did not differ from one another. Importantly, our statistical models took into consideration gender, age, and total brain volume, since it has been suggested that these variables are central in examining amygdalar and hippocampal volumes (31, 32) . The amygdala is vital to the accurate interpretation of emotion, emotional memory, and fear conditioning (33, 34) . Many depressed patients with psychosis have significant anxiety, fear, and apprehension, all of which could be influenced by structural abnormalities in the limbic region. Given the paucity of data pertaining to morphometric findings in psychotic depression, the novel finding in the present study requires replication before being considered a possible phenotypic characteristic of major depression with psychosis. If our data are replicated, further investigation into the functional implications of abnormal amygdala morphology in psychotic depression is warranted.
In order to better understand our findings, we examined whether severity of current psychiatric illness correlated with amygdala volume. Severity of depression and severity of positive symptoms did not correlate with amygdala volume in our two depressed groups. Normal amygdala volume has been seen in patients with chronic schizophrenia (9), which is consistent with our observation that amygdala volume was not related to psychosis per se. These findings suggest that there is a specific aspect of the diagnosis of major depression with psychosis, other than the severity of illness and presence of psychosis, which accounts for the reduction of amygdala volume.
We found a significant group-by-age interaction for amygdala volume. Follow-up analyses suggested that younger age was associated with smaller amygdala volume in depressed patients with psychosis. One possible explanation for this relationship is age at depression onset. In our sample of depressed patients with psychosis who had age at onset data, younger patients had a mean age at onset of 19 years, whereas older patients had a mean age at onset of 34 years. Thus, a smaller amygdala volume may be a risk factor for psychotic depression. However, replication of this effect is required to determine whether age or age of depression onset has a mitigating effect on amygdala volume.
There are data to support the hypothesis that chronicity of depression contributes to hippocampal volumetric changes in depressed individuals (17), but data pertaining to amygdala volume are limited. The fact that we did not have complete chronicity data from all subjects limited our ability to test this hypothesis. However, with chronicity data from approximately one-half of our depressed subjects, we found that increased right amygdala volume was highly correlated with 1) a later age at depression onset, 2) a shorter duration of current depressive episode, and 3) fewer past major depressive episodes. One explanation for this finding is that smaller amygdala volume may be a risk factor for psychotic depression. If this is the case, it would support the hypothesis that smaller volume of mesial temporal limbic structures is associated with a vulnerability to psychopathology rather than the result of psychopathology. Both animal (35) and human (36, 37) data have suggested that smaller hippocampal volume may be a risk factor for impaired stress response. For example, Lyons et al. (35) found that smaller hippocampal volume in adolescent monkeys predicted greater stress responses in adulthood. Gilbertson et al. (36) reported that smaller hippocampal volume in twins was a risk factor for stress-related psychopathology. In particular, they found that veterans with smaller hippocampal volume were at greater risk for later development of posttraumatic stress disorder after combat. Similarly, Frodl et al. (15) reported that men with first-episode depression had smaller hippocampal volume relative to healthy comparison subjects. Furthermore, a recent study found reduced amygdala volume in pediatric depressed patients relative to healthy comparison subjects, but no hippocampal volume differences were observed (14) . These findings suggest a relationship between age at depression onset and amygdala volume, and thus support the hypothesis that smaller amygdala volume may be a risk factor for psychotic depression.
In the general literature, there are some inconsistencies pertaining to neuroimaging and depression. One concern is that the existing literature on depressed samples may contain a mixture of depressive subtypes, and this may confound results. Our study suggests a specific relationship between unipolar psychotic depression and reduced amygdala volume. We did not observe a similar finding in depressed patients without psychosis. More studies need to be conducted in order to understand how psychotic and nonpsychotic depression may differ at the neurobiological and morphometric levels. More broadly, our findings highlight the need to distinguish depression subtypes when investigating brain abnormalities in depression.
The association of chronicity and psychotic subtype with limbic volume may be related. Depressed patients with psychosis are often more chronic, with a longer duration of depressive episodes (38) and a greater likelihood of recurrence (39) . Furthermore, the psychosis associated with major depression may be more difficult to recognize than other (nonaffective) psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia. Dubovsky (40) concluded that approximately one-half of the depressed patients who are refractory to antidepressants have psychotic symptoms of which their treating physician is unaware. Hence, available morphometric imaging studies of depression could be confounded by the lack of recognition of psychotic subtypes as well as symptom chronicity. Although depression chronicity has received a lot of attention, only a few studies have examined depressive subtypes. It would seem wise for future neuroimaging studies to systematically address both the chronicity of an individual’s condition as well as the individual’s depressive subtype. Only then can we begin to elucidate the neuroanatomical findings of psychotic depression.
Our study has several limitations. We reported multiple correlational analyses between brain volume and other demographic and chronicity variables. The number of these analyses increased the risk of family-wise type I error. Thus, our findings are in need of replication in order to obtain confirmation. Another limitation of our study is that the majority of the depressed patients (both with and without psychosis) were medicated with antidepressants, and three-quarters of depressed patients with psychosis were medicated with antipsychotic agents. Unfortunately—but out of necessity—the vast majority of studies that have investigated brain volume in depression have included medicated subjects (6) . We examined the effects of medication use on our findings but did not observe significant relationships between medication use and brain volume. Furthermore, normal amygdala volume has been reported in patients with chronic schizophrenia (9), individuals who would be more likely to have reduced amygdala volume if such volume reduction was a direct result of antipsychotic medication. In addition, if the effect was primarily the result of antidepressant medication, we would have expected our depressed patients without psychosis to have shown reduced volume as well. Thus, it appears that amygdala volume in psychotic depression is not likely associated with medication effects. Further studies that specifically address the long-term effects of medication on brain volume would be useful to clarify whether volumetric abnormalities can be attributed to major depression, medication, or both.

Footnotes

Received Aug. 7, 2007; revision received Dec. 21, 2007; accepted Feb. 15, 2008 (doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081257). From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, Calif. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Keller, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, 401 Quarry Rd., Stanford, CA 94305-5723; [email protected] (e-mail).
Dr. Keller has served as a consultant to Corcept Therapeutics. Dr. Solvason has served as a consultant to Neuropace; he has served on speakers bureaus for Forest Laboratories, Eli Lilly, and Cephalon; and he has received grant support from Forest Pharmaceuticals and Neuronetics. Dr. Schatzberg has served as a consultant to Corcept Therapeutics, Eli Lilly, Forest Laboratories, Merck, Neuronetics, Novartis, Pathway Diagnostics, Quintiles, and Wyeth; he is a shareholder with Corcept Pharmaceuticals, Forest, Merck, Neurocrine, and Pfizer; and he holds intellectual property rights on pharmacogenetic use patents on prediction of antidepressant response. Mr. Shen and Drs. Gomez, Garrett, and Reiss report no competing interests.
Supported in part by a grant from the Pritzker Foundation and NIH grant MH-50604 (Dr. Schatzberg).
Dr. Keller and Mr. Shen served as co-first authors.

References

1.
Davidson RJ, Pizzagalli D, Nitschke JB, Putnam K: Depression: perspectives from affective neuroscience. Annu Rev Psychol 2002; 53:545–574
2.
Bremner JD, Narayan M, Anderson ER, Staib LH, Miller HL, Charney DS: Hippocampal volume reduction in major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:115–118
3.
Vythilingam M, Heim C, Newport J, Miller AH, Anderson E, Bronen R, Brummer M, Staib L, Vermetten E, Charney DS, Nemeroff CB, Bremner JD: Childhood trauma associated with smaller hippocampal volume in women with major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:2072–2080
4.
Anand A, Shekhar A: Brain imaging studies in mood and anxiety disorders: special emphasis on the amygdala. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003; 985:370–388
5.
Axelson DA, Doraiswamy PM, McDonald WM, Boyko OB, Tupler LA, Patterson LJ, Nemeroff CB, Ellinwood EH Jr, Krishnan KR: Hypercortisolemia and hippocampal changes in depression. Psychiatry Res 1993; 47:163–173
6.
Videbech P, Ravnkilde B: Hippocampal volume and depression: a meta-analysis of MRI studies. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1957–1966
7.
Campbell S, MacQueen G: An update on regional brain volume differences associated with mood disorders. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2006; 19:25–33
8.
Kim DK, Kim BL, Sohn SE, Lim SW, Na DG, Paik CH, Krishnan KR, Carroll BJ: Candidate neuroanatomic substrates of psychosis in old-aged depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 1999; 23:793–807
9.
Velakoulis D, Wood SJ, Wong MT, McGorry PD, Yung A, Phillips L, Smith D, Brewer W, Proffitt T, Desmond P, Pantelis C: Hippocampal and amygdala volumes according to psychosis stage and diagnosis: a magnetic resonance imaging study of chronic schizophrenia, first-episode psychosis, and ultra-high-risk individuals. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:139–149
10.
Frodl T, Meisenzahl E, Zetzsche T, Bottlender R, Born C, Groll C, Jager M, Leinsinger G, Hahn K, Moller HJ: Enlargement of the amygdala in patients with a first episode of major depression. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 51:708–714
11.
Tebartz van Elst L, Woermann FG, Lemieux L, Trimble MR: Amygdala enlargement in dysthymia: a volumetric study of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:1614–1623
12.
Hastings RS, Parsey RV, Oquendo MA, Arango V, Mann JJ: Volumetric analysis of the prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and hippocampus in major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:952–959
13.
Sheline YI: Neuroanatomical changes associated with unipolar major depression. Neuroscientist 1998; 4:331–334
14.
Rosso IM, Cintron CM, Steingard RJ, Renshaw PF, Young AD, Yurgelun-Todd DA: Amygdala and hippocampus volumes in pediatric major depression. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:21–26
15.
Frodl T, Meisenzahl EM, Zetzsche T, Born C, Groll C, Jager M, Leinsinger G, Bottlender R, Hahn K, Moller HJ: Hippocampal changes in patients with a first episode of major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1112–1118
16.
Mervaala E, Fohr J, Kononen M, Valkonen-Korhonen M, Vainio P, Partanen K, Partanen J, Tiihonen J, Viinamaki H, Karjalainen AK, Lehtonen J: Quantitative MRI of the hippocampus and amygdala in severe depression. Psychol Med 2000; 30:117–125
17.
Sheline YI, Wang PW, Gado MH, Csernansky JG, Vannier MW: Hippocampal atrophy in recurrent major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1996; 93:3908–3913
18.
MacQueen GM, Campbell S, McEwen BS, Macdonald K, Amano S, Joffe RT, Nahmias C, Young LT: Course of illness, hippocampal function, and hippocampal volume in major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2003; 100:1387–1392
19.
Sheline YI, Sanghavi M, Mintun MA, Gado MH: Depression duration but not age predicts hippocampal volume loss in medically healthy women with recurrent major depression. J Neurosci 1999; 19:5034–5043
20.
Gomez RG, Fleming SH, Keller J, Flores B, Kenna H, DeBattista C, Solvason B, Schatzberg AF: The neuropsychological profile of psychotic major depression and its relation to cortisol. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60:472–478
21.
Schatzberg AF, Posener JA, DeBattista C, Kalehzan BM, Rothschild AJ, Shear PK: Neuropsychological deficits in psychotic versus nonpsychotic major depression and no mental illness. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1095–1100
22.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1997
23.
Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62
24.
Thase ME, Hersen M, Bellack AS, Himmelhoch JM, Kupfer DJ: Validation of a Hamilton subscale for endogenomorphic depression. J Affect Disord 1983; 5:267–278
25.
Overall JE, Gorham DE: The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychol Rep 1962; 10:799–812
26.
Keller J, Flores B, Gomez RG, Solvason HB, Kenna H, Williams GH, Schatzberg AF: Cortisol circadian rhythm alterations in psychotic major depression. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 60:275–281
27.
Kates WR, Warsofsky IS, Patwardhan A, Abrams MT, Liu AM, Naidu S, Kaufmann WE, Reiss AL: Automated Talairach atlas-based parcellation and measurement of cerebral lobes in children. Psychiatry Res 1999; 91:11–30
28.
Reiss AL, Hennessey JG, Rubin M, Beach L, Abrams MT, Warsofsky IS, Liu AM, Links JM: Reliability and validity of an algorithm for fuzzy tissue segmentation of MRI. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1998; 22:471–479
29.
Kates WR, Abrams MT, Kaufmann WE, Breiter SN, Reiss AL: Reliability and validity of MRI measurement of the amygdala and hippocampus in children with fragile X syndrome. Psychiatry Res 1997; 75:31–48
30.
Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. Thieme Medical Publishers, New York, 1988
31.
Brierley B, Shaw P, David AS: The human amygdala: a systematic review and meta-analysis of volumetric magnetic resonance imaging. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2002; 39:84–105
32.
Pruessner JC, Collins DL, Pruessner M, Evans AC: Age and gender predict volume decline in the anterior and posterior hippocampus in early adulthood. J Neurosci 2001; 21:194–200
33.
Cahill L, Babinsky R, Markowitsch HJ, McGaugh JL: The amygdala and emotional memory. Nature 1995; 377:295–296
34.
LaBar KS, LeDoux JE, Spencer DD, Phelps EA: Impaired fear conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. J Neurosci 1995; 15:6846–6855
35.
Lyons DM, Parker KJ, Zeitzer JM, Buckmaster CL, Schatzberg AF: Preliminary evidence that hippocampal volumes in monkeys predict stress levels of adrenocorticotropic hormone. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:1171–1174
36.
Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, Kasai K, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK: Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci 2002; 5:1242–1247
37.
Wignall EL, Dickson JM, Vaughan P, Farrow TF, Wilkinson ID, Hunter MD, Woodruff PW: Smaller hippocampal volume in patients with recent-onset posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 56:832–836
38.
Coryell W, Pfohl B, Zimmerman M: The clinical and neuroendocrine features of psychotic depression. J Nerv Ment Dis 1984; 172:521–528
39.
Aronson TA, Shukla S, Gujavarty K, Hoff A, DiBuono M, Khan E: Relapse in delusional depression: a retrospective study of the course of treatment. Compr Psychiatry 1988; 29:12–21
40.
Dubovsky SL: What we don’t know about psychotic depression. Biol Psychiatry 1991; 30:533–536

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 872 - 880
PubMed: 18450931

History

Published online: 1 July 2008
Published in print: July, 2008

Authors

Details

Jennifer Keller, Ph.D.
Rowena G. Gomez, Ph.D.
H. Brent Solvason, M.D., Ph.D.
Alan F. Schatzberg, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share