Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective: Amygdala volume has been associated with drug craving in cocaine addicts, and amygdala volume reduction is observed in some alcohol-dependent subjects. This study sought an association in alcohol-dependent subjects between volumes of reward-related brain regions, alcohol craving, and the risk of relapse. Method: Besides alcohol craving, the authors assessed amygdala, hippocampus, and ventral striatum volumes in 51 alcohol-dependent subjects and 52 age- and education-matched healthy comparison subjects after detoxification. After imaging and clinical assessment, patients were followed for 6 months and alcohol intake was recorded. Results: Alcohol-dependent subjects showed reduced amygdala, hippocampus, and ventral striatum volumes and reported stronger craving in relation to healthy comparison subjects. However, only amygdala volume and craving differentiated between subsequent relapsers and abstainers. A significant decrease of amygdala volume in alcohol-dependent subjects was associated with increased alcohol craving before imaging and an increased alcohol intake during the 6-month follow-up period. Conclusions: These findings suggest a relationship between amygdala volume reduction, alcohol craving, and prospective relapse into alcohol consumption.
Alcohol dependence is a chronic relapsing disorder and second among the 10 most common medical causes of total years lost to disability (1) . There are biological, genetic, and environmental factors contributing to the high rates of relapse. During the development of alcohol dependence, alterations of the brain reward system in cortical and limbic structures such as the amygdala, hippocampus, and ventral striatum have been observed and may convey the vulnerability for relapse in recovering alcohol-dependent subjects (2) .
The amygdala is involved in the attachment of emotional valence to events (i.e., reward and punishment) (3) and is in this way crucial for decision making and learning (e.g., reference 4 ). Furthermore, individuals with amygdala lesions fail to develop classically conditioned fear responses. Cocaine-dependent subjects who were also abusing alcohol displayed amygdala volume reductions that were connected to baseline measures of drug craving (5) .
Amygdala volume reduction is an important anatomical correlate of alcohol use disorders and has previously been observed in individuals with alcohol dependence and in their offspring (68) . The hippocampus and ventral striatum have also been reported to be similarly altered in heavy drinkers (9), in adolescents with alcohol use disorders (10, 11), and in adult alcoholics (1214) . Animal experiments implicate the amygdala, hippocampus, and nucleus accumbens in conditioned reinstatement of alcohol-seeking (2) . Human measures of substance seeking, namely craving, have been observed to be associated with amygdala volume reduction in subjects dependent on cocaine and abusing alcohol (5) . All three brain regions have been shown to be involved in various aspects of emotional memory function and in fear conditioning (1518) . Furthermore, all three brain regions have been shown to be important for the assessment of salience measures and valence (i.e., reward and aversion) (3, 19) that are important for decision making and learning (e.g., reference 4 ). These observations raise the prospect that differences in brain volumes that process rewarding/aversive stimuli and are important for memory may be related to the relapse risk, in part, on the basis of increased alcohol craving.
We specifically assessed in alcohol-dependent subjects: 1) differences from healthy comparison subjects in the volume of the amygdala, hippocampus, and ventral striatum; 2) differences in alcohol craving; and 3) the association of these variables and subsequent relapse.

Method

Subjects and Instruments

Fifty-one alcohol-dependent patients (34 men and 17 women) who maintained abstinence for 1 week before scanning with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) were included. They were group matched to 52 healthy comparison subjects (32 men and 20 women) on the basis of age and education and did not differ significantly for gender distribution (χ 2 =0.29, df, 1, p=0.59). All subjects were European and right-handed and provided written informed consent according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The Ethics Committee of the University of Heidelberg, Germany, approved the study.
The mean age of the patients was 41 years (SD=7, range=26–57) and 40 years (SD=8 years, range=26–63) for the comparison subjects. Standardized clinical assessment with the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition With Psychotic Screen (20) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (21) was performed to exclude other active and lifetime axis I psychiatric disorders (and axis II disorders in healthy volunteers). Alcohol-dependent subjects fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for alcohol dependence according to ICD-10 and DSM-IV. The severity of alcohol dependence was assessed with the Alcohol Dependence Scale (22), and the amount of lifetime alcohol intake was measured by the Lifetime Drinking History (23) ( Table 1 ). The patients had no previous substance dependence or current substance abuse other than alcohol dependence (confirmed with random urine drug testing). The severity of depression was assessed with the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (24) and the German version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (25) . Anxiety was measured with the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (26) and the severity of nicotine dependence by the Fagerström test (27) . Exclusion criteria were hepatic impairment (e.g., liver cirrhosis) and clinically manifest neurological disorders (e.g., Wernicke-Korsakoff’s syndrome or alcoholic neuropathy) as determined by a senior neurologist.
Alcohol-dependent subjects participated in a 3-week inpatient detoxification program and were abstinent for at least 5 days (median=17 days, SD=14, range=5–96) before MRI was performed (control for the days of abstinence did not significantly correlate with any of the assessed brain volumes: all R<0.23, all p>0.12). Random breath and urine testing excluded current substance or alcohol abuse. All subjects were medication free (including benzodiazepines, chlormethiazole, acamprosate, naltrexone, etc.) for at least 1 week before testing.
Before scanning, craving for alcohol was measured with a visual analogue craving scale (1–100). After discharge, the patients were seen biweekly for another 6 months, and alcohol consumption was recorded with the Form 90 (28) . In accordance with standard clinical trials (29), relapse was defined as a consumption of more than 60 grams of alcohol in men or more than 40 grams of alcohol in women during the assessment period. By using these criteria, 31 patients relapsed and 17 patients stayed abstinent. For three patients, we could not clarify the relapse status. Random alcohol blood and breath tests were performed, levels of carbohydrate-deficient transferrin and gamma-glutamyltransferase were evaluated, and relatives or members of their self-help group were contacted to verify the patients’ abstinence. Besides alcohol craving, no other alcohol-related factors such as the amount of alcohol consumption over the lifetime, duration of alcohol dependence, age of onset, and number of cigarettes smoked per day were associated with the volume of the assessed brain regions (all R<–0.22, all p>0.13).

Neuroimaging

MRI image acquisition

Scanning was performed by using a 1.5 T clinical whole-body MRI (Magnetom VISION, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) equipped with a standard quadrature head coil. The automatic Siemens Multi-Angle Projection shim was used for shimming. A morphological three-dimensional T 1 -weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient echo image data set (1×1×1 mm=voxel size, field of view=256 mm, 162 slices, TR=11.4 msec, TE=4.4 msec, α=12°) covering the whole head was acquired for anatomical study.

Morphometric analysis

Gray-white matter segmentation was performed on each T 1 -weighted, positionally normalized, three-dimensional coronal scan by using a semiautomated intensity contour mapping algorithm for cerebral exterior definition and signal intensity histogram distributions for demarcation of gray-white matter borders (30) .
The segmentation method applied followed prior applications (5) : all structures were segmented by using a contour line and manual editing. To distinguish the amygdala and hippocampus, sulci lines were drawn in the sagittal views, and the anterior tip of the hippocampus was separated from the ventral and posterior border of the amygdala. Using lateral and sagittal views, one can distinguish and trace this border of the amygdala, which was usually enhanced by the anterior end of the temporal horn of the inferior lateral ventricle. In cross-reference, corresponding axial views helped to identify this border. In the coronal view, the saw-tooth pattern of the hippocampus was identified and traced. The ventral striatum was identified at the inferior junction between the head of the caudate and the putamen. It was delimited superiorly by a line connecting the inferior corner of the lateral ventricle and the most inferior point of the internal capsule abutting the ventral striatum and laterally by a vertical line passing from the latter point. The total volume resulted from the addition of the left and right volumes.
Segmentation was performed by two bachelor’s level magnetic resonance technicians who were blind to subject diagnosis and group status and with a randomly ordered sequence of subjects. Intrarater and interrater reliabilities were assessed through percent common voxel assignments (31) . Reliabilities were all between 85.9 and 90.1. These values are consistent with intrarater and interrater reliabilities reported in previous studies (e.g., references 5 and 31 ).
To rule out gross volumetric effects, total head circumference was calculated for all subjects. Group differences were not significant for this measure (all p>0.05).
For topological analysis, subsequent relapsing and abstaining alcohol-dependent subjects were registered to a reference brain that was separate from either cohort. Amygdala and hippocampus probabilities for each group were calculated on a voxel-by-voxel basis with the aligned data. Iso-surfaces for the probability 0.5 were created for each group separately (5) . Three-dimensional visualization of these surfaces was used to search for systematic differences in the topology of the amygdala between groups.

Statistical Analysis

For a comparison of brain volumes between healthy comparison subjects, relapsers, and abstainers, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used with sex and morphometry rater as covariates and the post hoc test was corrected with the least significant difference. Since craving data were not normally distributed, the Mann-Whitney U test was used for the comparison between relapsers, abstainers, and comparison subjects, and the significance threshold was Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons (p=0.05/3=0.0166). Amygdala volume and craving were correlated using Spearman’s rho (p<0.05, two-tailed).

Results

Group Comparison

Alcohol-dependent patients displayed significantly more liver dysfunction and negative mood states in relation to healthy comparison subjects ( Table 1 ); however, the groups of subsequently relapsing versus abstaining patients did not differ significantly from each other with respect to these clinical variables.

Volumes

In alcohol-dependent subjects in relation to healthy comparison subjects, we observed a significant volume reduction in the hippocampus (T=2.49, p<0.02) and the ventral striatum (T=2.87, p=0.005) and a nonsignificant reduction in the amygdala (T=1.45, p=0.15). Considering the relapse status during the 6-month follow-up period, a MANOVA revealed a significant effect for the group factor (comparison subjects, relapsers, and abstainers) (Wilks’s lambda=3.57, df=6, p=0.002), with significant effects for all three regions (the amygdala: p=0.007, the hippocampus: p=0.008, and the ventral striatum: p=0.003). This result remained statistically significant even when control was added for total gray matter volume (Wilks’s lambda=2.43, df=6, p<0.03), with significant effects for all three brain regions: the amygdala: p<0.03, the hippocampus: p<0.05, and the ventral striatum: p<0.04). Post hoc tests revealed that the amygdala volume was significantly reduced in relapsers compared to both abstainers (p<0.05) and comparison subjects (p=0.002), but there was no significant difference between abstainers and comparison subjects (p=0.99) ( Figure 1 ). Ventral striatal volume was reduced in relapsers (p=0.004) and abstainers (p<0.02) in relation to healthy comparison subjects, but no significant difference was observed between abstainers and relapsers (p=0.94) ( Figure 1 ). For the hippocampal volume, we found a significant difference between relapsers and comparison subjects (p=0.004) but no significant differences between abstainers and comparison subjects (p=0.26) and abstainers and relapsers (p=0.25) ( Figure 1 ).
Figure 1. Total Amygdala, Hippocampus, and Ventral Striatum Volumes (left plus right) and Alcohol Craving for Healthy Comparison Subjects, All Alcohol-Dependent Patients, and the Subgroups of Subsequent Relapsing and Abstaining Patients
*p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

Topological data

The average surface of the amygdala for the relapsers lay slightly above and in front of the surface of the abstainers in a continuous fashion from an anterior-inferior position to an anterior-superior position. The larger extent of the amygdala observed in the abstainers was observed along both the inferior and posterior-superior aspects of the amygdala. The maximum extent differences between relapsing and abstaining alcohol-dependent subjects at the back of the amygdala were 3 mm to 4 mm. This area represents regions in which abstinent individuals have amygdala tissue but the relapsing group does not. Using the atlas of Mai et al. (32) for reference, these pattern differences encompass nuclei from the basolateral group (i.e., lateral nucleus) in the portion of the amygdala adjacent to the subiculum. Although no absolute group difference in hippocampal volume between abstainers and relapsers was found, findings within the hippocampus (i.e., subiculum) parallel those with the amygdala: the hippocampus of subsequent abstinent alcohol-dependent subjects extended about 3 mm to 4 mm beyond those of subsequent relapsing alcohol-dependent subjects along its inferior and posterior-superior aspects, encompassing components of the subiculum.

Craving

Alcohol-dependent subjects reported a significantly stronger craving for alcohol than healthy comparison subjects (Mann-Whitney U=636, p<0.0001) ( Figure 1 ). Dividing alcohol-dependent patients into subsequent relapsers and abstainers, subsequent relapsers showed a significantly stronger craving compared to subsequent abstainers (Mann-Whitney U=123.5, p=0.003) and to comparison subjects (Mann-Whitney U=230, p<0.0001). There was no difference between subsequently abstaining patients and healthy comparison subjects (Mann-Whitney U=368, p=0.38) ( Figure 1 ).

Association Analysis

Smaller amygdala volume was associated with increased alcohol craving in alcohol-dependent subjects (R=–0.36, p<0.02) but not in comparison subjects (R=–0.27, p<0.06). Separating relapsers and abstainers, a significant association between amygdala volume and craving was found in relapsers (R=–0.38, p<0.04) but not in abstainers (R=–0.17, p=0.51).

Discussion

Our study showed for the first time that amygdala volume reduction, located mainly in the basolateral amygdala, and alcohol craving differentiated between subsequently relapsing and abstaining alcohol-dependent subjects.
The amygdala comprises more than a dozen subnuclei, which by a topological perspective can be categorized into three general divisions: basolateral, corticomedial, and central nuclear/anterior area (33) . The smaller extension of amygdala volume in relapsing compared with abstinent alcohol-dependent subjects was observed along both inferior and posterior-superior aspects of the amygdala encompassing subnuclei from the basolateral group. Studies in rodents have shown that the basolateral amygdala is critical to learning representations linking stimuli to the incentive values of outcomes (4, 34) . In primates, changes in the positive and negative values of cues modulate neural activity in the amygdala, and this modulation occurs fast enough to account for learning (35) . In human studies with functional MRI, it has been shown that amygdala activation is correlated with 1) assessment of positive and negative stimuli (36), 2) expectancies of rewards and losses (19), 3) valuation of reward representations accessible to predictive cues (37), and 4) processing of alcohol-associated cues in alcohol-dependent subjects (38) .
Animals with basolateral amygdala lesions lack response flexibility during unconditioned stimulus devaluation (39) and display an increase in impulsive choice (4) . Alcohol-dependent subjects with amygdala volume alterations display decision-making deficits in a simulated gambling task, suggesting that decision-making deficits in everyday life of abstinent alcohol-dependent subjects may reflect an alteration in brain structure (7) . These factors may contribute to poor treatment outcome in alcohol-dependent subjects with amygdala volume differences.
Our study does not address the question of whether amygdala volume reduction is a cause or consequence of chronic alcohol intake. Overall reductions in brain volume have often been attributed to ethanol’s neurotoxic effects, but other environmental effects and genetic factors may also play a role (40) . Fein and coworkers (7) found amygdala reduction in long-term abstinent alcohol-dependent subjects (on average 7.1 per year) with no association between amygdala volume and duration of abstinence. Also, in our data, results were not affected when we controlled for the duration of abstinence before scanning. This finding may indicate that amygdala volume reduction is a trait rather than a state marker and does not recover with abstinence. Hill (6) and Benegal and coworkers (8) observed amygdala volume reduction in adolescent and young adult offspring from families with a history of alcohol dependence, suggesting that reduced amygdala volume may—at least in part—be present even before excessive alcohol intake starts and may reflect a preexisting condition that predisposes to alcohol dependence and severe patterns of consumption. Assuming that the difference in brain volumes is a trait marker, follow-up studies might compare 1) alcohol-dependent subjects who tried several times to stay abstinent but failed with 2) alcohol-dependent subjects who have been abstinent long-term and healthy comparison subjects.
Volume reductions of the ventral striatum and subiculum region of the hippocampus were found in this study in all alcohol-dependent subjects in relation to healthy comparison subjects, but there was no difference between subsequent relapsing and abstaining patients. This finding emphasizes that volume reduction of the ventral striatum and hippocampus is a common finding in alcohol-dependent subjects, which, however, did not discriminate between subsequent relapsers and abstainers in our study.
In summary, this study suggests that amygdala volume reduction is associated with alcohol craving and an increased prospective relapse risk.

Footnotes

Received Dec. 11, 2007; revision received March 4, 2008, accepted April 7, 2008 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07121877). From the Department of Psychiatry, Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Campus Charité Mitte; the Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany; the Center for Morphometric Analysis, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston; the Clinic for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Wiesbaden, Germany; the Department of Psychology, Technical University of Dresden, Dresden, Germany; the School of Medicine, University of Wales Swansea, U.K.; the Section of Addiction Biology, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College, London, and the Athinoula A. Martinos Center, Department of Radiology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Heinz, Department of Psychiatry, Charité-Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Charité Campus Mitte, Charitéplatz 1, 10117 Berlin, Germany; [email protected] (e-mail).
The authors report no competing interests.
Dr. Heinz is supported by grants from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft number He2597/4-3) and the Bernstein Centre for Computational Neuroscience Berlin (01GQ0411). Further support for this study was provided by grants to Dr. Breiter (numbers 00265, 09467, and 14118) from the National Institute on Drug Abuse and a grant (DABK39-03-C-0098 from the Phenotype Genotype Project in Addiction and Depression) from the Office of National Drug Control Policy—Counterdrug Technology Assessment Center. Further support, in part, was provided to Dr. Breiter by the Massachusetts General Hospital Department of Radiology, the National Center for Research Resources (P41RR14075), and the Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery (MIND) Institute.
The authors thank Derik Hermann and Sabine Klein for assistance with subject recruitment, characterization, and brain scanning.

References

1.
Murray CJL, Lopez AD: Evidence-based health policy—lessons from the global burden of disease study. Science 1996; 274:740–743
2.
Dayas CV, Liu X, Simms JA, Weiss F: Distinct patterns of neural activation associated with ethanol seeking: effects of naltrexone. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:979–989
3.
Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio A, Lee G: Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci 1999; 19:5473–5481
4.
Winstanley CA, Theobald DEH, Cardinal RN, Robbins TW: Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. J Neurosci 2004; 24:4718–4722
5.
Makris N, Gasic GP, Seidman L, Goldstein J, Gastfriend D, Albaugh DM, Hodge SM, Ziegler D, Kennedy D, Caviness VS, Tsuang M, Hyman S, Rosen BR, Breiter HC: Decreased absolute amygdala volume in cocaine addicts. Neuron 2004; 44:729–740
6.
Hill SY: Trajectories of alcohol use and electrophysiological and morphological indices of brain development: distinguishing causes from consequences. Ann NY Acad Sci 2004; 1021:245–259
7.
Fein G, Landman B, Tran H, McGillivray S, Finn P, Barakos J, Moon K: Brain atrophy in long-term abstinent alcoholics who demonstrate impairment on a simulated gambling task. Neuroimage 2006; 32:1465–1471
8.
Benegal V, Antony G, Venkatasubramanian G, Jayakumar PN: Gray matter volume abnormalities and externalizing symptoms in subjects at high risk for alcohol dependence. Addict Biol 2007; 12:122–132
9.
Beresford TP, Arciniegas DB, Alfers J, Clapp L, Martin B, Du Y, Liu D, Shen D, Davatzikos C: Hippocampus volume loss due to chronic heavy drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2006; 30:1866–1870
10.
Nagel BJ, Schweinsburg AD, Phan V, Tapert SF: Reduced hippocampal volume among adolescents with alcohol use disorders without psychiatric comorbidity. Psychiatry Res—Neuroimaging 2005; 139:181–190
11.
De Bellis MD, Clark DB, Beers SR, Soloff PH, Boring AM, Hall J, Kersh A, Keshavan MS: Hippocampal volume in adolescent-onset alcohol use disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:737–744
12.
Agartz I, Momenen R, Rawlings RR, Kerich MJ, Hommer DW: Hippocampal volume in patients with alcohol dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:356–363
13.
Sullivan EV, Marsh L, Mathalon DH, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A: Anterior hippocampal volume deficits in nonamnesic, aging chronic alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995; 19:110–122
14.
Sullivan EV, Deshmukh A, De Rosa E, Rosenbloom MJ, Pfefferbaum A: Striatal and forebrain nuclei volumes: contribution to motor function and working memory deficits in alcoholism. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:768–776
15.
Davis M: Neural systems involved in fear-potentiated startle. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1989; 563:165–183
16.
Schwienbacher I, Schnitzler HU, Westbrook RF, Richardson R, Fendt M: Carbachol injections into the nucleus accumbens disrupt acquisition and expression of fear-potentiated startle and freezing in rats. Neuroscience 2006; 140:769–778
17.
Norris EH, Strickland S: Modulation of NR2B-regulated contextual fear in the hippocampus by the tissue plasminogen activator system. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007; 104:13473–13478
18.
Puga F, Barrett D, Bastida C, Gonzalez-Lima F: Functional networks underlying latent inhibition learning in the mouse brain. Neuroimage 2007; 38:171–183
19.
Breiter HC, Aharon I, Kahneman D, Dale A, Shizgal P: Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron 2001; 30:619–639
20.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams J: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition With Psychotic Screen (SCID-I/P W/ PSY screen). New York, Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, 2001
21.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1997
22.
Skinner HA, Horn JL: Alcohol Dependence Scale: Users Guide. Toronto, Addiction Research Foundation, 1984
23.
Skinner HA, Sheu WJ: Reliability of alcohol use indices: the Lifetime Drinking History and the MAST. J Stud Alcohol 1982; 43:1157–1170
24.
Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62
25.
Hautzinger M, Bailer M: Allgemeine Depressions Skala (ADS). Weinheim, Germany, Beltz Verlag, 1993
26.
Spielberger CD: Manual of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, Calif, Consulting Psychologists Press, 1983
27.
Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerström KO: The Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence—a revision of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict 1991; 86:1119–1127
28.
Miller WR, Del Boca FK: Measurement of drinking behavior using the Form 90 family of instruments. J Stud Alcohol Suppl 1994; 12:112–118
29.
Heinz A, Dufeu P, Kuhn S, Dettling M, Graf K, Kurten I, Rommelspacher H, Schmidt LG: Psychopathological and behavioral correlates of dopaminergic sensitivity in alcohol-dependent patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:1123–1128
30.
Makris N, Worth AJ, Sorensen AG, Papadimitriou GM, Wu O, Reese TG, Wedeen VJ, Davis TL, Stakes JW, Caviness VS, Kaplan E, Rosen BR, Pandya DN, Kennedy DN: Morphometry of in vivo human white matter association pathways with diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging. Ann Neurol 1997; 42:951–962
31.
Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, Goldstein JM, Kremen WS, Horton NJ, Makris N, Toomey R, Kennedy D, Caviness VS, Tsuang MT: Left hippocampal volume as a vulnerability indicator for schizophrenia—a magnetic resonance imaging morphometric study of nonpsychotic first-degree relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:839–849
32.
Mai JK, Assheuer J, Paxinos G: Atlas of the Human Brain. San Diego, Academic Press, 1997
33.
Price JL, Russchen RT, Amaral DG: The limbic region, II: the amygdaloid complex, in Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, vol 5. Edited by Hökfeltand T, Björklund A, Swanson LW. Amsterdam, Elsevier, 1987, pp 279–388
34.
Tye KM, Janak PH: Amygdala neurons differentially encode motivation and reinforcement. J Neurosci 2007; 27:3937–3945
35.
Paton JJ, Belova MA, Morrison SE, Salzman CD: The primate amygdala represents the positive and negative value of visual stimuli during learning. Nature 2006; 439:865–870
36.
Breiter HC, Etcoff NL, Whalen PJ, Kennedy WA, Rauch SL, Buckner RL, Strauss MM, Hyman SE, Rosen BR: Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron 1996; 17:875–887
37.
Gottfried JA, O’Doherty J, Dolan RJ: Encoding predictive reward value in human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex. Science 2003; 301:1104–1107
38.
Schneider F, Habel U, Wagner M, Franke P, Salloum JB, Shah NJ, Toni I, Sulzbach C, Hönig K, Maier W, Gaebel W, Zilles K: Subcortical correlates of craving in recently abstinent alcoholic patients. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1075–1083
39.
Koob GF: The neurobiology of addiction: a neuroadaptational view relevant for diagnosis. Addiction 2006; 101(suppl 1):23–30
40.
Gilman JM, Bjork JM, Hommer DW: Parental alcohol use and brain volumes in early- and late-onset alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:607–615

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1179 - 1184
PubMed: 18593776

History

Published online: 1 September 2008
Published in print: September, 2008

Authors

Details

Dieter F. Braus, Prof.
Michael N. Smolka, Prof.
David N. Kennedy, Ph.D.
Verne S. Caviness, M.D., D.Phil.
Steven M. Hodge, Ph.D.
Matthew Albaugh, Ph.D.
David A. Ziegler, Ph.D.
Christian Kissling, Ph.D.
Gunter Schumann, Prof.
Hans C. Breiter, Prof.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share