Skip to main content
Full access
Editorial
Published Online: 1 June 2009

Targeting Cognition in Schizophrenia Research: From Etiology to Treatment

Over the past 20 years, a plethora of resources have been aimed at characterizing and treating the cognitive impairments of schizophrenia. This activity is reflected in the exponential increase in scientific publications related to cognition in schizophrenia (1) . As a result of these efforts, cognitive impairment is now considered a core symptom of the disorder. Of crucial importance is the influence of cognitive impairment in predicting patients’ functional adaptation. Cognitive impairment appears to be a major predictive factor in determining a patient’s ability to cope successfully with everyday activities, including vocation, social networks, and living independently (2) . For this reason, efforts are being made to develop treatments aimed at enhancing cognition, as well as to identify the underlying etiopathogenic mechanisms of cognitive impairment. In this issue of the Journal, two articles address this trend in schizophrenia research.
In a pragmatic and clinically oriented open-label trial, Davidson et al. (3) tested the cognition-enhancing properties of four second-generation antipsychotics—amisulpride, olanzapine, quetiapine, and ziprasidone—as well as the most commonly prescribed first-generation antipsychotic—haloperidol, administered at low doses. This trial enabled the researchers to test the efficacy of haloperidol on cognitive deficits without the motor and speed impairments in cognitive performance that it often causes at higher doses. The study found enhanced cognitive performance after 6 months of treatment and no overall difference in the degree of improvement among the five drugs. While these results are consistent with those of the Clinical Trials of Antipsychotic Intervention Effectiveness (CATIE) in chronically ill patients (4), this study used haloperidol instead of perphenazine and extended the findings to patients with first-episode schizophrenia.
The investigators present convincing arguments that neither the use of an open-label design nor study dropouts biased the results. Despite the low doses of haloperidol, the use of a test battery heavily weighted toward tests of speed may have influenced the results. Slower responses are scored as cognitive deficiencies in executive function and working memory. An editorial published in the February 2008 issue of the Journal highlighting the results of the Measurement and Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (MATRICS) collaborative project spoke to the need for standardized cognitive tests that better distinguish the different facets of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia (5) .
The results of the Davidson et al. study have important implications for the clinical management of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. They also allude to the disadvantages and cost-effectiveness, or lack thereof, of using second-generation antipsychotics for the therapeutic goal of enhancing cognitive performance. As the authors point out, the process of finding a new drug with cognition-enhancing properties would require the development of new compounds focusing on cognitive impairments rather than merely using drugs that have been successful in ameliorating psychotic symptoms.
There are three factors to consider when thinking about these data. The first has to do with practice effects (6) . Certainly, longer intervals (like the 6 months in the Davidson et al. study) may attenuate practice effects but not entirely eliminate them. For instance, verbal learning tests repeated at 2-year intervals demonstrate a practice effect of 0.22 effect size units (the mean difference between the two tests divided by the pooled standard deviation), an effect similar to that observed with drug treatment (7) . Procedural or implicit learning may show effects of earlier exposure to the test that are especially long lived. Second, the different binding profiles of the drugs used in this study, at receptors known to influence neurocognition (including the D 1, D 2, M 1, and 5-HT 2A receptors), might in principle make it seem improbable that all would engender similar degrees of cognitive improvement, yet they do. Third, if one assumes that a cognition-enhancing agent should also enhance cognition in at least some healthy comparison subjects (e.g., in the elderly or in individuals carrying a specific genotype), there is little evidence to suggest that a class of drugs with high D 2 receptor antagonist activity could improve cognition.
Nevertheless, it is important to appreciate that D 2 antagonists have profound restorative effects on the neurocognitive underpinnings and circuitry of psychiatric symptoms, including abnormal salience in delusions, semantic misprocessing in speech disorganization, and misattribution in hallucinations (810) . Thus, understanding how the first- and second-generation D 2 antagonists succeed and how they fail in reversing specific types of information processing abnormalities in relatively dissociable neural systems may provide an important lead in understanding pathogenesis in schizophrenia.
In a second study reported in this issue, Brown et al. (11), in line with their previous research on the associated risk of schizophrenia and increased serological levels of influenza and toxoplasma antibodies (12, 13), go a step further in trying to demonstrate an etiopathogenic mechanism underlying executive dysfunction in schizophrenia. In a research area where studies focusing on descriptions of impaired cognition have been the rule instead of studies aimed at finding etiological mechanisms, this is one of the first studies to look at prenatal exposure to infection as an etiologic mechanism of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. Using prospective data from a well-characterized population-based birth cohort, Brown et al. found an association between serologically documented maternal infection with influenza and toxoplasmosis and executive dysfunction in people who have schizophrenia. The acquisition of biomarkers documenting the prenatal exposure to infection is doubtless a robust strength supporting the finding, but the small sample of only eight infected schizophrenia patients who underwent executive evaluation (and a small, albeit exquisitely well matched, comparison group) and the lack of an infected comparison group are limitations that future research should address to replicate this finding.
Prenatal exposure to infection with Toxoplasma gondii is associated with immediate and delayed cognitive sequelae in children: microcephaly, low IQ, and bilateral deafness (14) . Given that the parasite that causes the infection cannot cross the placenta, it is assumed that elevated maternal IgG antibody to toxoplasma is the agent that damages the developing fetal brain (13) . How antibody titers selectively disrupt frontal-type executive functions is an unresolved question. Moreover, it is unknown whether higher IgG toxoplasma antibody levels are related to the severity of executive dysfunction as a possible surrogate of the effect of IgG antibody on the brain. Little is known about the consequences of maternal influenza infection in human fetal development. As findings in experimental animals suggest (15), prospective cohort studies should address the question of what the neurodevelopmental consequences are in offspring of mothers who have been exposed to influenza infection during pregnancy. In that respect, a recent study (16) has linked general cognitive abnormalities (as reflected in IQ) in children exposed in utero to influenza who later developed schizophrenia in contradistinction to exposed children who did not develop schizophrenia. Again, future research should aid in distinguishing between cognitive consequences of prenatal exposure to infections, such as toxoplasmosis and influenza, and specifically schizophrenia-related changes. In addition, as may have been expected, cognitive sequelae of exposure to infection in this schizophrenia sample are complex. For instance, premorbid cognitive ability (measured with the Wide Range Achievement Test–3) was higher in the exposed than in the nonexposed case subjects.
To summarize, Brown et al. demonstrated that exposed case subjects had poorer executive function. This is not only interesting in and of itself, it also raises questions about the nature of the pathology, its selectivity or predilection for a presumptively prefrontal neural system (versus the alternative notion that the tests showing impairment were simply those most psychometrically sensitive), and whether a specific etiology shapes the cognitive target. What would be the implications for the treatment of schizophrenia patients who had prenatal exposure to infection? Should they be treated in a different way than those not exposed? Thus, in addition to the search for cognition-enhancing compounds that enhance neural systems with specificity, will drugs need to be developed that are specific to etiology? Economic hard times notwithstanding, these are million-dollar questions that the field faces.

Footnotes

Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Goldberg, Zucker Hillside Hospital, Lowenstein Research Building, 75-59 263rd St., Glen Oaks, NY 11004; [email protected] (e-mail). Editorial accepted for publication April 2009 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040497).
Dr. Goldberg has served as a consultant for GlaxoSmithKline, Merck, Organon, Pfizer, and Wyeth and receives royalties for use of the Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia in clinical trials. Mr. Gomar reports no competing interests. Dr. Freedman has reviewed this editorial and found no evidence of influence from these relationships.
Mr. Gomar’s position is supported by Benito Menni CASM (Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain), Instituto de Salud Carlos III (FI05/00322), and Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM).

References

1.
Hyman SE, Fenton WS: Medicine: what are the right targets for psychopharmacology? Science 2003; 299:350–351
2.
Bowie CR, Reichenberg A, Patterson TL, Heaton RK, Harvey PD: Determinants of real-world functional performance in schizophrenia subjects: correlations with cognition, functional capacity, and symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:418–425
3.
Davidson M, Galderisi S, Weiser M, Werbeloff N, Fleischhacker WW, Keefe RS, Boter H, Keet IPM, Prelipceanu D, Rybakowski JK, Libiger J, Hummer M, Dollfus S, López-Ibor JJ, Hranov LG, Gaebel W, Peuskens J, Lindefors N, Riecher-Rössler A, Kahn RS: Cognitive effects of antipsychotic drugs in first-episode schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: a randomized, open-label clinical trial (EUFEST). Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:675–682
4.
Keefe RS, Bilder RM, Davis SM, Harvey PD, Palmer BW, Gold JM, Meltzer HY, Green MF, Capuano G, Stroup TS, McEvoy JP, Swartz MS, Rosenheck RA, Perkins DO, Davis CE, Hsiao JK, Lieberman JA: Neurocognitive effects of antipsychotic medications in patients with chronic schizophrenia in the CATIE trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:633–647
5.
Harvey PD, Cornblatt BA: Pharmacological treatment of cognition in schizophrenia: an idea whose method has come (editorial). Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:163–165
6.
Goldberg TE, Goldman RS, Burdick KE, Malhotra AK, Lencz T, Patel RC, Woerner MG, Schooler NR, Kane JM, Robinson DG: Cognitive improvement after treatment with second-generation antipsychotic medications in first-episode schizophrenia: is it a practice effect? Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007; 64:1115–1122
7.
Blasi S, Zehnder AE, Berres M, Taylor KI, Spiegel R, Monsch AU: Norms for change in episodic memory as a prerequisite for the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Neuropsychology 2009; 23:189–200
8.
Ford JM, Mathalon DH: Corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia: can it explain auditory hallucinations? Int J Psychophysiol 2005; 58:179–189
9.
Kapur S, Mizrahi R, Li M: From dopamine to salience to psychosis-linking biology, pharmacology, and phenomenology of psychosis. Schizophr Res 2005; 79:59–68
10.
Kuperberg GR, Ditman T, Kreher DA, Goldberg TE: The cognitive basis of language dysfunction in schizophrenia, in Handbook of Neuropsychology of Mental Illness. Edited by Wood S, Allen A, Pantelis C. Cambridge, England, Cambridge University Press (in press)
11.
Brown AS, Vinogradov S, Kremen WS, Poole JH, Deicken RF, Penner JD, IW McKeague, Kochetkova A, Kern D, Schaefer CA: Prenatal exposure to maternal infection and executive dysfunction in adult schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166: 683–690
12.
Brown AS, Begg MD, Gravenstein S, Schaefer CA, Wyatt RJ, Bresnahan M, Babulas VP, Susser ES: Serologic evidence of prenatal influenza in the etiology of schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:774–780
13.
Brown AS, Schaefer CA, Quesenberry CP Jr, Liu L, Babulas VP, Susser ES: Maternal exposure to toxoplasmosis and risk of schizophrenia in adult offspring. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:767–773
14.
Sever JL, Ellenberg JH, Ley AC, Madden DL, Fuccillo DA, Tzan NR, Edmonds DM: Toxoplasmosis: maternal and pediatric findings in 23,000 pregnancies. Pediatrics 1988; 82:181–192
15.
Shi L, Fatemi SH, Sidwell RW, Patterson PH: Maternal influenza infection causes marked behavioral and pharmacological changes in the offspring. J Neurosci 2003; 23:297–302
16.
Ellman LM, Yolken RH, Buka SL, Torrey EF, Cannon TD: Cognitive functioning prior to the onset of psychosis: the role of fetal exposure to serologically determined influenza infection. Biol Psychiatry (Epub ahead of print, Feb 4, 2009)

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 631 - 634
PubMed: 19487396

History

Published online: 1 June 2009
Published in print: June, 2009

Authors

Details

Terry E. Goldberg, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share