Site maintenance Wednesday, November 13th, 2024. Please note that access to some content and account information will be unavailable on this date.
Skip to main content
Full access
Editorials
Published Online: 1 October 2022

Can You Feel the Burn? Using Neuroimaging to Illuminate the Mechanisms of Mindfulness Interventions for Pain

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry
Chronic pain is one of the leading causes of disability and morbidity worldwide (1), and there is a real clinical need for non-opioid-based treatment options. As such, there is a growing body of research investigating mindfulness interventions for pain. There have been two recent meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials in pain, both with modest positive conclusions, although both also commented on the heterogeneity and low quality of many of the trials included. A meta-analysis focusing on mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) interventions for patients with chronic pain (2) found evidence for a small effect size, and another on patients with acute pain (3) concluded that there is weak to moderate evidence for mindfulness improving pain tolerance or threshold, but no good-quality evidence for reducing pain severity or pain-related distress.
Measuring pain, and the components of pain targeted by an intervention, is a challenging task. Pain research typically distinguishes between the sensory component of pain (i.e., the intensity of pain) and the more affective component of pain (i.e., the unpleasantness of pain). Neatly differentiating these components is not always easy (4), especially as we might expect a complex and dynamic relationship between these features of pain, but it is clear that there are numerous emotional, motivational, and cognitive processes involved in the experience of pain beyond the sensory assessment of intensity, including anticipation, anxiety, and catastrophizing (5). Neither of the above-mentioned meta-analyses provides direct evidence regarding which aspects of pain might be targeted by mindfulness interventions, but the latter (3) suggests that mindfulness appears to have more of an effect on cognitive appraisal and behavioral responses to pain.
As of yet, there have been limited studies investigating the neural effects of mindfulness on pain to provide insight into this question. Recent studies suggest that early reports of mindfulness changing brain structure are unlikely to be true (6), but there remains some indication, from the functional MRI (fMRI) evidence available, that mindfulness may help people uncouple the sensory experience of pain from their emotional response to pain (7).
The study by Wielgosz et al. in this issue (8) provides valuable insight by using a novel neuroimaging approach—machine-learning-derived signatures of pain—to unpack which aspects of pain may be targeted by mindfulness interventions. The authors looked at two pain signatures—the neural pain signature (NPS) and the stimulus intensity independent pain signature–1 (SIIPS1). For each, a single numerical value is calculated by looking at activation and deactivation in voxels across the brain, and weighting activity at each individual voxel by how it matches the pattern of the neural signature. The NPS is described as capturing “stimulus-dependent” aspects of pain—i.e., intensity of pain—while the SIIPS1 is described as a complementary signature, trained specifically on “stimulus-independent” aspects of pain that aren’t captured by the NPS, including psychological components such as sense of control or expectation. Both incorporate neural activity across the brain; specific details can be found in the article’s supplementary materials.
Participants naive to mindfulness underwent fMRI during a thermal pain test, before and after a mindfulness intervention, and per-participant neural responses were computed for both signatures. The mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) intervention was a standard, validated course, aligning with the most commonly studied mindfulness interventions in randomized controlled trials. It was compared to an active control intervention—a health enhancement program (HEP)—specifically designed to match the length, structure, and content of MBSR, with the inclusion of group support, exercise, and other nonspecific therapeutic effects, but without the mindfulness component. For example, the “purpose of walking in MBSR is to cultivate awareness in movement, whereas the purpose of walking in HEP is the cardiovascular benefits of the physical activity for cardiovascular training” (9). This active control intervention is a strength of the study, ensuring that conclusions can be drawn about the specific benefit of MBSR.
Comparison of both groups, as well as a waiting list group, found that both intervention groups showed significantly reduced subjective reports of pain unpleasantness after the intervention, and both intervention groups had marginal reductions in SIIPS1 response compared to the waiting list group (where SIIPS1 response increased nonsignificantly), but only the mindfulness intervention significantly reduced NPS response (with a reasonable effect size, >0.4) compared to HEP and the waiting list condition.
Wielgosz et al. also did a cross-sectional comparison of all mindfulness-naive participants to long-term meditators, who had at least 3 years of formal meditation experience, including multiple intensive retreats, and had an ongoing daily practice. This cross-sectional analysis found that while long-term meditators had lower subjective reports of both pain intensity and unpleasantness compared to mindfulness-naive participants, there were no differences in either neural signature. The authors also found that among long-term meditators, SIIPS1 response and pain intensity and unpleasantness ratings were lower for those with greater retreat hours.
While Wielgosz et al. argue that their findings are in line with their predictions, as a focus on physical sensation is common in early stages of mindfulness, it is perhaps surprising that the most compelling (and the only statistically significant [p<0.5]) finding for change after the mindfulness intervention was in the neural signature related to pain intensity rather than cognitive/affective aspects. This appears to be at odds with the indications from both the acute pain meta-analysis (3) and the review of initial neuroimaging studies (7), both of which suggested that mindfulness targets cognitive/affective appraisals. It is also interesting that this neural change occurred alongside the self-report measures indicating a reduction in unpleasantness but not intensity. When this trial was registered (ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT01057368), MRI blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal was the primary outcome, but no specific directional hypotheses were given, so clear confirmatory analyses in follow-up studies would provide greater confidence in a definitive conclusion.
There is also the null finding of no differences in neural signature in long-term meditators compared to mindfulness-naive participants. Again, Wielgosz et al. provide a potential explanation, positing that the lack of differences in neural signatures may be because of different baseline characteristics in those who seek out meditation practices, that then became normalized by meditation. Prior use of psychiatric medication was an exclusion, as was a psychiatric diagnosis in the past year, but this leaves significant scope for variability in undiagnosed mental health difficulties (current and historic) as well as psychiatric diagnoses older than a year. Nonetheless, confirmatory follow-up is important to justify this explanation for the lack of group difference.
There is significant interest in identifying neural biomarkers for pain, in part to address the limitations of self-report measures (10). In research, self-report measures may not sensitively discriminate the different components of pain, and they are particularly susceptible to placebo effects. In clinical settings, while self-report measures remain an essential part of patient assessment, there is a push for biomarkers to assist with pain detection in nonverbal patients or patients with barriers to communication, such as babies or persons with dementia. A 2020 Consensus Statement from a presidential task force of the International Association for the Study of Pain (11) highlights the potential benefits of biomarkers for understanding the mechanisms underlying pain and identifying targets for pain interventions, as in the Wielgosz et al. study. The statement also raises concerns about the misuse of neural biomarkers for pain in legal settings (e.g., for assessing the validity of insurance claims or eligibility for disability benefits), the importance of testing neural biomarkers in diverse populations, and the wider societal and ethical implications; these are concerns that researchers involved in biomarker development should be conscious of and consider when publicizing their work.
One of the key strengths of the Wielgosz et al. study is the novel use of independently derived machine learning neural signatures, made available for other researchers to use. These signatures have been shown to be robust against placebo—that is, participants who self-report reductions in pain after placebo treatment do not show reductions in their neural signatures for pain (12); this is particularly beneficial for trials of psychological interventions that cannot be fully blinded, although this study also had a robust active control group. Neural signatures such as these provide benefits for study design and analysis, reducing the likelihood of false positives and the need for substantial multiple-comparison adjustment, by focusing on comparison between one or two single computed values rather than individual voxels across the brain. More broadly, these neural signatures enable greater opportunities for precise reproduction and replication of findings, as well as more direct comparison between populations and interventions.
Comparison between different neural signatures is particularly helpful for teasing apart the target of mindfulness interventions, with the NPS being established as consistently reflecting the magnitude of pain experience and differentiating somatic pain from nonpainful warmth (9). Because neural responses to pain involve activity across numerous regions of the brain, including those involved in fear and cognitive appraisals, it is difficult to be certain that machine learning algorithms don’t identify correlated features of pain or non-pain-specific features of the experience, especially as validation efforts still frequently rely on drawing a relationship between the outcomes of machine learning algorithms and self-report measures. However, established neural signatures that can be applied consistently and readily studied independently go some way toward helping address that challenge and can increase our confidence in interpretation. In future studies, it may be interesting to look at other neural signatures of pain, for example, the pain-analgesic network (13), which was specifically designed to capture analgesic effects in clinical trials; this could be used to compare mindfulness interventions to pharmacological interventions.
Using these neural signatures to explore other interventions for pain, beyond mindfulness, could also provide an insight into the parallels (or lack of) in the targets for these interventions. For example, deep brain stimulation has been used for decades to treat chronic pain, with recent targeting of the anterior cingulate cortex providing more promising outcomes; reports from patients indicate that this is due to a change in their emotional attachment to the pain (14), and it would be interesting to see if this is reflected in the same neural signatures investigated in the Wielgosz et al. study.
Experimental medicine models in healthy populations are important, to explore the mechanisms of an intervention without the confounder of symptom change and without the heterogeneity introduced by complex medical histories. However, ultimately findings need to be translated to clinical populations, to establish their impact and benefit in patients. As Wielgosz et al. note, there are no neural signatures developed and validated for chronic pain specifically—a key priority for future research—and so it is unclear how their work using acute methods of inducing pain would translate to chronic pain. Acute and ongoing pain are vastly different, with reports of significant distinctions in structural and functional MRI findings, as well as metabolite and neurotransmitter function (15), and differences in how this pain is experienced and reported. Clinical experiences of pain are typically associated with reports of lower pain intensity and greater pain unpleasantness, with the reverse true in acute pain induction in experimental settings. This latter difference in experience of pain could provide an alternative explanation for why Wielgosz et al. found the most robust differences in neural signatures associated with pain intensity; in chronic pain, there is significant distress associated with the medical concern, the unknown cause, and the uncertain trajectory of pain, whereas in a controlled experimental setting, these elements are less pertinent. This may mean that acutely induced pain provides less scope for improvement in cognitive/emotional responses. Chronic pain is also frequently associated with psychiatric comorbidity, such as depression, which is characterized by substantive differences in emotional cognition, so it would be interesting to translate these findings into a sample with well-characterized psychiatric symptoms.
Looking further afield, there are also indications that developing similar neural signatures in other research areas could benefit our understanding of psychiatric interventions for different conditions. A recent meta-analysis (16) concluded that antidepressants and psychotherapy appear to evoke neural changes in different regions of the brain, with antidepressants primarily targeting limbic regions such as the amygdala and psychotherapy primarily targeting regions such as the medial prefrontal cortex. If this clear distinction is borne out in future research, and/or refined further to specify relationships between features of interventions and neural pathways, perhaps neural signatures could be similarly developed to help evaluate and compare novel across- and within-class treatments for depression.

References

1.
Rice ASC, Smith BH, Blyth FM: Pain and the global burden of disease. Pain 2016; 157:791–796
2.
Hilton L, Hempel S, Ewing BA, et al: Mindfulness meditation for chronic pain: systematic review and meta-analysis. Ann Behav Med 2017; 51:199–213
3.
Shires A, Sharpe L, Davies JN, et al: The efficacy of mindfulness-based interventions in acute pain: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pain 2020; 161:1698–1707
4.
Talbot K, Madden VJ, Jones SL, et al: The sensory and affective components of pain: are they differentially modifiable dimensions or inseparable aspects of a unitary experience? A systematic review. Br J Anaesth 2019; 123:e263–e272
5.
Tracey I, Mantyh PW: The cerebral signature for pain perception and its modulation. Neuron 2007; 55:377–391
6.
Kral TRA, Davis K, Korponay C, et al: Absence of structural brain changes from mindfulness-based stress reduction: two combined randomized controlled trials. Sci Adv 2022; 8:eabk3316
7.
Marie SR, Talebkhah KS: Neurological evidence of a mind-body connection: mindfulness and pain control. Am J Psychiatry Residents J 2018; 13:2–5
8.
Wielgosz J, Kral TRA, Perlman DM, et al: Neural signatures of pain modulation in short-term and long-term mindfulness training: a randomized active-control trial. Am J Psychiatry 2022; 179:758–767
9.
MacCoon DG, Imel ZE, Rosenkranz MA, et al: The validation of an active control intervention for mindfulness based stress reduction (MBSR). Behav Res Ther 2012; 50:3–12
10.
Tracey I, Woolf CJ, Andrews NA: Composite pain biomarker signatures for objective assessment and effective treatment. Neuron 2019; 101:783–800
11.
Davis KD, Flor H, Greely HT, et al: Brain imaging tests for chronic pain: medical, legal, and ethical issues and recommendations. Nat Rev Neurol 2017; 13:624–638
12.
Zunhammer M, Bingel U, Wager TD, et al: Placebo effects on the neurologic pain signature: a meta-analysis of individual participant functional magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA Neurol 2018; 75:1321–1330
13.
Duff EP, Vennart W, Wise RG, et al: Learning to identify CNS drug action and efficacy using multistudy fMRI data. Sci Transl Med 2015; 7:274ra16
14.
Farrell SM, Green A, Aziz T: The current state of deep brain stimulation for chronic pain and its context in other forms of neuromodulation. Brain Sci 2018; 8:E158
15.
Martucci KT, Mackey SC: Neuroimaging of pain: human evidence and clinical relevance of central nervous system processes and modulation. Anesthesiology 2018; 128:1241–1254
16.
Nord CL, Barrett LF, Lindquist KA, et al: Neural effects of antidepressant medication and psychological treatments: a quantitative synthesis across three meta-analyses. Br J Psychiatry 2021; 219:546–550

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 705 - 707
PubMed: 36181328

History

Accepted: 16 August 2022
Published online: 1 October 2022
Published in print: October 2022

Keywords

  1. Biological Markers
  2. Brain Imaging Techniques
  3. Cognitive Neuroscience
  4. Neurocircuitry
  5. Neuroimaging
  6. Pain

Authors

Details

Amy Gillespie, Ph.D.
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, U.K.
Catherine J. Harmer, D.Phil. [email protected]
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford; and Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, U.K.

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Harmer ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

Dr. Gillespie has served as a consultant for UCB Pharma (formerly Zogenix). Dr. Harmer has served as a consultant for Johnson & Johnson, Lundbeck, P1vital, and Zogenix.

Funding Information

Dr. Harmer is supported by the Oxford Health NIHR Biomedical Research Centre.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share