Skip to main content
Full access
Columns
Published Online: 1 December 2002

Clinical Computing: The Diffusion of Innovations Into Psychiatric Practice

Fifteen years ago, a paper in the Journal of Clinical Psychiatry reported a new technology that assessed suicide risk more sensitively and accurately than experienced clinicians did (1). Three years later, a paper in the American Journal of Psychiatry described an effective new treatment for depression that was virtually free of side effects and was dramatically less expensive than established methods (2). Both approaches harnessed the same underlying technology, and both were published in leading psychiatric journals. One might have expected that by now these discoveries would either be in widespread use or have been refuted. Neither has occurred. These discoveries remain unchallenged, little known, and virtually unused clinically.
Psychiatrists' training is steeped in science, and psychiatric journals are crowded with articles that attempt to quantify the significance of their findings. Science is supposed to shape clinical decisions. And psychiatrists do regularly, rapidly, and dramatically change their behavior under the banner of new research findings. When pharmaceutical companies release new medications, buttressed by a wave of published research studies, specialist lectures, continuing-medical-education compact discs, and personable marketing representatives, psychiatrists often respond energetically, writing hundreds of thousands of new prescriptions within a few years.
Unsurprisingly, given that this is the Clinical Computing column, the assessment and treatment systems I mentioned above are built on computer technology. They are part of a compelling body of literature reporting the efficacy of computers in clinical psychiatry. Among other findings, this literature claims that patients like giving their histories to computers (3), that adolescents are more honest with computers than with clinicians (4), that computer-based assessments can be comparable to the best rating instruments used in research (5), and that computer-assisted treatments are effective and well received (2,6,7). Scores of papers, none convincingly challenged.
Why have computer-based systems not swept into clinical use? Rogers (8), in his seminal Diffusion of Innovations, studied the elements that determine whether an innovation spreads throughout a group. He and other innovation researchers have examined a host of historical successful and unsuccessful innovations and tried to identify the factors that explain the variance. His examples range from the adoption of scurvy prophylaxis by the British Navy to the popular rejection of the Dvorak typewriter-keyboard layout.
Consistently, these researchers have found that one cannot anticipate the reception of an innovation without understanding the conceptual and social framework of the receiving populace. Rogers recounts the experience of Nelida, a health worker in rural Peru who was trying to staunch an epidemic of typhus by getting local families to boil their drinking water. Nelida spent two years trying to convince 185 families; only 11 families adopted the practice. As Nelida found, in order to gain acceptance, it is not enough for an innovation simply to work better than the old approach. Emerson apparently oversimplified in his famous assertion, "build a better mousetrap, and the world will beat a path to your door."
Rogers's framework is helpful in understanding why, as with innovations in mousetrap design, computer-based advances in psychiatric treatment have been slow to catch on. He identifies 11 factors that he believes are the major determinants of the rate of acceptance of a new approach or method. Most of these factors are highly dependent on the social fabric of the community. The intrinsic advantages of the innovation—or at least the perceived advantages—count too. Typically, though, these perceptions are more influenced by the attitudes of neighbors than by an independent review of the evidence. Taken in aggregate, social forces consistently trump unvarnished effectiveness. Well-equipped marketers for change— "agents"—can tip the balance substantially, but they must accurately target these same social pressure points in order to succeed.
Nelida failed because her approach was antithetical to the social and conceptual orientation of the villagers. The villagers eschewed boiling because of a deeply held traditional belief that foods are either hot or cold and that everyone but the sick should drink cold water. Those who violated this norm were viewed as strange and were shunned. Although Nelida repeatedly and insistently told villagers that germs cause disease and that boiling kills germs, this perspective was alien and ultimately less compelling than maintaining social standing in the village.
Although there are many differences between introducing water boiling to a rural Peruvian village and promoting the use of computer technology in contemporary psychiatry, there are parallels as well. The Peruvians never took seriously the evidence presented by Nelida, despite her bringing in a public health physician to give a series of lectures, largely because their preexisting beliefs were so established and so contrary to Nelida's message. Similarly, psychiatrists may be disinclined to freely consider the clinical use of computers, and this reluctance may be due to preexisting beliefs that blunt consideration of the evidence.
Impeding psychiatrists' open consideration of the evidence is a long-held tenet, in place at least since Freud, that the psychiatrist and the patient should share information though speech. Psychiatric outpatient treatment is built on a foundation of two people in a room, talking. The dominance of this image is manifest in many aspects of psychiatric practice. Its force stifles not only new advances but even the use of established skills. For example, physical examinations, which are central to every psychiatrist's training as a physician, are infrequently performed in outpatient psychiatry (9). Clinically, there would seem to be risks associated with this omission (10).
In the late 1970s and early 1980s, a minor flow of publications examined the rate of physical exams performed by psychiatrists on their outpatients. Rates were low—typically less than 10 percent (9,10). At that time, the rate of medication use in the treatment of depression was nearing 40 percent (11). Some of the papers judged the lack of exams to be a malpractice risk. They argued, sensibly, that as physicians who use medications that cause a host of potentially serious side effects, including seizures, delirium, tardive dyskinesia, and drug-induced parkinsonism, physical examinations by psychiatrists should perhaps be standard care. By 1997, the use of medication in the treatment of depression had grown to almost 75 percent (11). Despite this increase, which one might have anticipated would have triggered a swelling debate about physical exams, the minor trickle of papers on physical exams essentially dried up. Although some might argue that a physical examination distorts transference or otherwise interferes with psychiatric treatment, the relatively passive acceptance of omitting the physical exam, especially for patients being treated with medications, seems more likely to reflect socialization than sound clinical judgment.
Unlike papers on physical exams, papers on the effectiveness of computers in psychiatry continue to be published, but they, too, have had little effect on clinical practice. Applying Rogers's model, there are multiple likely reasons for this inertia, including clinicians' dependence on reimbursement by managed care organizations— "collective innovation-decision"—and the efforts required by clinicians to adequately test the new systems—limited "triability." However, the lack of debate on the subject may reflect an especially deeply entrenched obstacle—the use of computers in evaluation and treatment might be alien to psychiatrists' self-image of their role.
Particularly germane to understanding psychiatry is what Rogers refers to as the nature of the social system. Some social systems, like the dot-com world before it burst, raced to be different. Psychiatry, on the other hand, fosters a more conservative professional identity. Like Nelida, promoters of computers in psychiatry face skeptical natives. Changes affecting the interaction between the psychiatrist and the patient are particularly sensitive, and, data or not, such proposals have a tinge of blasphemy.
But, unlike Peruvian villagers, psychiatrists have an identity that rests on being scientific, empirical, and open-minded. Those conveying the successes of computer-based methods in psychiatry will never be able to use the proven methods of the pharmaceutical industry: acting as an agent and widely affecting the educational and research apparatus of the profession. But as long as a whole body of unchallenged research describing safe and efficient methods of evaluation and treatment sits, largely ignored, in the middle of the published literature, there is an intellectual discontinuity in the field. Psychiatry should be challenged to apply its own scientific standards rigorously, and to practice accordingly. Incrementally, these voices will have influence, and the field will advance.

Footnote

Dr. Freedman, who is editor of this column, is past-president of the American Association of Technology and Psychiatry and assistant clinical professor at the University of California, Los Angeles. Send correspondence to him at 235 Main Street, Apartment 218, Venice, California 90291.

References

1.
Erdman HP, Greist JH, Gustafson DH, et al: Suicide risk prediction by computer interview: a prospective study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 48:464-467, 1987
2.
Selmi PM, Klein MH, Greist JH, et al: Computer-administered cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 147:51-56, 1990
3.
Erdman HP, Klein MH, Greist JH, et al: A comparison of two computer-administered versions of the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule. Journal of Psychiatric Research 26:85-95, 1992
4.
Turner CF, Ku L, Rogers SM, et al: Adolescent sexual behavior, drug use, and violence: increased reporting with computer survey technology. Science 280:867-873, 1998
5.
Kobak KA, Greist JH, Jefferson JW, et al: Computer-administered clinical rating scales: a review. Psychopharmacology 127:291-301, 1996
6.
Greist JH, Marks IM, Baer L, et al: Behavior therapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder guided by a computer or by a clinician compared with relaxation as a control. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 63:138-145, 2002
7.
Osgood-Hynes DJ, Greist JH, Marks IM, et al: Self-administered psychotherapy for depression using a telephone-accessed computer system plus booklets: an open U.S.-U.K. study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59:358-365, 1998
8.
Rogers EM: Diffusion of Innovations, 4th ed. New York, Free Press, 1995
9.
McIntyre JS, Romano J: Is there a stethoscope in the house (and is it used)? Archives of General Psychiatry 34:1147-1151, 1977
10.
Krummel S, Kathol RG: What you should know about physical evaluations in psychiatric patients: results of a survey. General Hospital Psychiatry 9:275-279, 1987
11.
Olfson M, Marcus SC, Druss B, et al: National trends in the outpatient treatment of depression. JAMA 287:203-209, 2002

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Psychiatric Services
Go to Psychiatric Services
Psychiatric Services
Pages: 1539 - 1540
PubMed: 12461211

History

Published online: 1 December 2002
Published in print: December 2002

Authors

Details

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share