Skip to main content

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The goal of this direct-interview family study was to replicate and extend an earlier finding of a familial liability for social phobia. The authors hypothesized that there would be higher rates of the generalized type of social phobia—but not the nongeneralized (or “discrete”) type—among relatives of probands with generalized social phobia. They also hypothesized that rates of avoidant personality disorder, a frequent comorbid condition, would be higher in relatives of probands with generalized social phobia. METHOD: The authors examined rates of three social phobia subtypes defined a priori—discrete, nongeneralized, and generalized—as well as rates of avoidant personality disorder by direct interview of 106 first-degree relatives of 23 patients with generalized social phobia and 74 first-degree relatives of 24 comparison subjects without social phobia. RESULTS: Relative risks for generalized social phobia and avoidant personality disorder were markedly higher (approximately 10-fold) among first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia than among first-degree relatives of comparison probands. In contrast, relative risks for discrete social phobia and nongeneralized social phobia were not significantly different between the two groups of first-degree relatives. CONCLUSIONS: These results confirm earlier findings of a higher rate of social phobia among relatives of probands with generalized social phobia and extend these findings by specifically indicating that it is only the generalized type (and its probable axis II counterpart, avoidant personality disorder) that occurs more often among the families of probands with generalized social phobia. Implications for subsequent genetic studies are discussed. (Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:90–97)
Social phobia is an anxiety disorder characterized by the fear and/or avoidance of situations where an individual is subject to the scrutiny of others. Under such circumstances, the individual fears that she or he will say or do something foolish or embarrassing or will appear anxious to others (DSM-IV and ICD-10). Although social phobia used to be considered fairly uncommon, two epidemiologic surveys (13) have placed the 12-month prevalence of social phobia in the range of 7%–8%, a rate severalfold higher than that determined by earlier studies (4, 5). Moreover, the recognition that social phobia is frequently associated with functional impairment (610) has brought it to attention as an important public health concern (11, 12).
In concert with its recognition as a disorder worthy of serious research have come attempts to understand the etiology and pathophysiology of social phobia. One area of interest is in the possible heritability of social phobia (13, 14). A large, population-based twin study of phobias in women demonstrated a heritable component to social phobia (15). Two studies to date have examined familial characteristics of social phobia. The first (16), using the family history method, found a significantly higher rate of social phobia among first-degree relatives of probands with social phobia (6.6%) than among first-degree relatives of control subjects (2.2%). The second (17), a direct-interview family study, also found a significantly higher rate of social phobia in first-degree relatives of probands with social phobia (16%) than in first-degree relatives of control subjects (5%). In an extension of this study, the investigators were also able to show that social phobia “bred true” in the sense that rates of agoraphobia or simple phobia were not higher among the relatives of probands with social phobia (18).
Researchers have surmised that social phobia may be a heterogeneous condition (1925). In particular, it has been noted that whereas some patients with social phobia suffer exclusively from one or several performance fears (e.g., speaking in public or writing in front of others), others experience a broader array of social fears, including many interactional situations (e.g., meeting new people, attending parties, or talking to people in authority) (1925). Although the field has yet to decide on a uniform nomenclature for classification of diagnostic subtypes in social phobia, there seems to be a consensus—reflected in the DSM-IV criteria—that there is a subgroup of individuals whose social fears span a broad range of social situations. Such individuals are referred to in the more recent literature as suffering from generalized social phobia.
In a reanalysis of their initial family study findings (17), Mannuzza et al. (26) examined differential family patterns between probands with generalized and nongeneralized phobia. They found that when probands with social phobia were subtyped as having either generalized or nongeneralized phobia, it was only among those with the generalized type that a significantly higher rate of social phobia among first-degree relatives occurred (16%) compared with control subjects (6%). The sample size, however, did not permit the investigators to determine whether it was the generalized subtype that was specifically higher among relatives of probands with generalized phobia. Still, this study provided the first hint that the generalized subtype of social phobia might characterize a familial form of the disorder.
With this background information available, we conducted a direct-interview family study of patients with generalized social phobia using a priori operational criteria for subtype designation. We hypothesized that we would find higher rates of generalized social phobia among relatives of probands with generalized social phobia. We also hypothesized that rates of avoidant personality disorder—thought by many to be the axis II equivalent of severe generalized social phobia (2530)—would be higher in relatives of probands with generalized social phobia. Finally, we postulated that rates of avoidant personality disorder would be higher among relatives of probands with early-onset than among relatives of probands with late-onset phobia. Such a finding, conforming to the assumption that patients with early onset have more severe forms of disease (31), would lend further credence to the conceptualization of avoidant personality disorder as a more severe form of generalized social phobia.

METHOD

Subjects

Thirty-three probands with generalized social phobia (age 18–65, English speaking) were recruited through our Anxiety Disorders Clinic. Subjects were assessed by using a version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R—Patient Version 1.0 (SCID-P) (32), which we had modified to reflect changes in the anxiety disorder diagnoses in DSM-IV. All probands with social phobia met DSM-IV criteria for the generalized subtype; there were no exclusion criteria based on lifetime comorbidity. To be considered for the study, probands needed to have at least two living first-degree relatives whom we might potentially contact. Two subjects were unwilling to give permission to contact all of their first-degree relatives; they were dropped from the study. Of the 31 patients who gave permission to contact their relatives, we were ultimately able to directly interview only one first-degree relative of eight; these eight probands are not considered further in the analyses described below. (It should be noted, however, that none of the findings change if these eight probands are included in the analyses.) Therefore, we were left with 23 probands with generalized social phobia for whom we were able to interview at least two first-degree relatives.
We recruited 24 comparison subjects without social phobia by means of advertisements posted in hospitals and medical clinic waiting rooms. We attempted to match the groups with and without social phobia for age and gender. The only inclusion criteria were that the person not have a personal history of social phobia and that the person have at least two living first-degree relatives whom they would allow us to contact. There were no exclusion criteria (i.e., we did not use a “supernormal” group of comparison subjects [33, 34]). After preliminary screening by telephone, each potential comparison subject was interviewed with the same diagnostic instrument (modified SCID-P) used with the probands with generalized social phobia. Of 27 volunteers who contacted us about the study, three were excluded because they had personal histories of social phobia.
All first-degree relatives 16 years of age and older whom we were able to interview were included in the study.
All subjects gave their informed, written consent to participate in this study. This study was approved by the Human Subjects Committee of the Faculty of Medicine, University of Manitoba.

Diagnostic Interviews

Interviews were conducted either in person (whenever this was feasible) or by telephone. The interviews with 54 (50.9%) of the 106 first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia were conducted in person, compared with 31 (41.9%) of the 74 first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia (continuity-adjusted χ2=1.09, df=1, p=0.30). Mean interview duration was 69 minutes (SD=24, range=20–130) for the first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia, compared with 56 minutes (SD=21, range=20–100) for the first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia (t=3.37, df=176, p<0.001).
Interviewers were selected on the basis of having had extensive previous experience with diagnostic interviewing in a research setting. Interviewers included two predoctoral-level clinical psychologists and one psychiatric research nurse (P.F., D.C., and S.L.). All interviewers attended workshops conducted by the principal investigator (M.B.S.) and a senior clinical psychologist (J.R.W.) on the use of the diagnostic instruments. Interviews with first-degree family members were conducted blind to information about the proband. All subjects were specifically instructed not to divulge any information about the proband to the interviewer, and “broken” blinds were noted.
The interview consisted of the mood (minus the dysthymia section), anxiety, and eating disorder sections of the SCID-P (32), which we modified as follows: 1) The anxiety disorders module was modified to incorporate the changes that were anticipated for DSM-IV (35). Because several options were available for some diagnoses, additional questions were added to enable us to go back and assign definitive diagnoses once the DSM-IV criteria were finalized. 2) The social phobia section was amplified with the aim of enabling us to assign subtypes on an a priori basis. To this end, we added probes for a wide range of social situations (appendix 1); many of these probes were borrowed from the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia—Lifetime Version Modified for the Study of Anxiety Disorders (36). Among our research group, the modified social phobia module has excellent interrater reliability for the diagnosis of DSM-IV social phobia (interrater reliability kappa=0.73, percent agreement=88%).
Social phobia was diagnosed according to DSM-IV criteria. We operationally assigned subtype diagnoses of social phobia according to the following conventions: 1) The performance type could be either present or absent. If someone endorsed severe distress or any degree of avoidance with respect to at least one of the performance situations in appendix 1, then the performance type was considered present. 2) The interactional type could also be either present or absent. If someone endorsed one or two socially interactive situations in appendix 1, then they were considered to have the limited interactional type, whereas if they endorsed three or more they were considered to have the generalized interactional type. All subjects categorized in our study as having the generalized interactional type of phobia can be considered by definition as having the DSM-IV generalized subtype. Subjects with performance-only, limited-interactional-only, or performance-plus-limited-interactional types of phobia can be considered as having what is being increasingly referred to in the field as the nongeneralized subtype.
We conducted second interviews with 38 first-degree relatives of probands with social phobia 12 months after their initial interview. We found reliability (which, in this case, reflects an amalgam of test-retest and interrater reliability) to be very good (kappa=0.84, percent agreement=92%) for the generalized versus nongeneralized distinction.
A diagnostic module for avoidant personality disorder was included. This module was derived from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders (SCID-II) (37) and conformed to DSM-III-R criteria. Follow-up interviews conducted with 36 subjects 12 months after their initial interview with the DSM-III-R avoidant personality disorder module demonstrated acceptable reliability (kappa=0.67, percent agreement=83%) for the avoidant personality disorder diagnosis. In the light of the changes in the criteria for avoidant personality disorder that have taken place between DSM-III-R and DSM-IV, we can say with confidence only that our results are applicable to the former.

Diagnostic Standardization and Consensus Procedures

Diagnoses were established by consensus of the “blinded” members of the study team, which included the principal investigator (M.B.S.), three interviewers (S.L., P.F., and D.C), and two experienced clinician-researchers (M.J.C. and A.L.H). At a weekly diagnostic conference, interviews were presented in detail and diagnostic “benchmarks” were systematically noted as being met, not met, or indeterminate. When necessary to facilitate diagnostic clarity, subjects were reinterviewed either by the same or, in some cases, a second interviewer, and the new interview results were presented at a subsequent conference. When consensus could not be reached despite reinterview(s), the principal investigator served as the final arbiter.
As the study progressed, we established diagnostic conventions as became necessary. Whenever a new convention was established, all interviews that had previously been reviewed in conference were reexamined by the group to determine if any diagnostic reclassification was in order.

Data Analysis

Between-group differences in demographic variables were tested by using Student's t test or the chi-square test, as appropriate. Fisher's exact test or chi-square tests (continuity-adjusted for two-by-two tables) were used to contrast familial prevalence of disorders. To examine possible age or gender effects, the familial contrasts were repeated and hazards analysis (SAS PHGLM procedure) applied with these two variables as covariates. The pattern of results was not changed in this analysis, and so we report the results here without controlling for these factors.
The proportion of affected individuals (not including the proband) was computed for each family, and these values were compared for families of probands with generalized social phobia and for families of probands without social phobia by using a Wilcoxon rank-sum test. When numerous ties were encountered in the ranked values, a permutation test of the Wilcoxon statistic was also performed by sampling from the possible random assignments of probands to families (38, p. 189). Relative risk for generalized social phobia among first-degree relatives of probands in the two groups was determined as simply the ratio of proportions of affected relatives. However, familial clustering would tend to make standard relative risk confidence intervals (obtained by pooling across families) (39, p. 182) too narrow; 95% confidence intervals, therefore, were obtained by first estimating the relative risk that any member of a family was affected (40) and then adjusting for the average number of affected relatives per affected family. Unless otherwise indicated, statistical tests were two-tailed and p values <0.05 were considered statistically significant.

RESULTS

Characteristics and Comorbidity of the Probands

There were no significant differences between probands with generalized social phobia and probands without social phobia in either gender or age distribution. Six (26.1%) of the 23 probands with generalized social phobia and 10 (41.7%) of the 24 probands without social phobia were women (continuity-adjusted χ2=0.67, df=1, p=0.41). The mean age of the probands with generalized social phobia was 36.7 years (SD=12.2, median=35, range=18–62), compared with 32.9 years (SD=10.8, median=32, range=19–60) for the probands without social phobia (t=1.13, df=45, p=0.26).
Seventeen (73.9%) of the 23 probands with generalized social phobia had avoidant personality disorder, but none of the probands without social phobia received that diagnosis. Lifetime comorbidity among the probands with generalized social phobia was as follows: two had a history of panic disorder, four simple phobia, five generalized anxiety disorder, and seven major depression. Lifetime comorbidity among the probands without social phobia was as follows: one had a history of panic disorder, one simple phobia, two generalized anxiety disorder, and three major depression.

Characteristics of the First-Degree Relatives

There were no significant differences between groups in the proportion of relatives interviewed. We were able to directly interview 106 (80.9%) of 131 living first-degree relatives of the probands with generalized social phobia and 74 (78.7%) of 94 living first-degree relatives of the probands without social phobia (continuity-adjusted χ2=0.06, df=1, p=0.81). There were also no significant differences between the probands with generalized social phobia and the probands without social phobia in the proportion of relatives interviewed according to gender. Seventy-one (67.0%) of the 106 interviewed first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia were female, compared with 40 (54.1%) of the 74 interviewed first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia (continuity-adjusted χ2=2.2, df=1, p=0.14). Among the living first-degree relatives who were not interviewed, 15 (60.0%) of 25 relatives of probands with generalized social phobia were women, compared with seven (35.0%) of 20 relatives of probands without social phobia (continuity-adjusted χ2=1.87, df=1, p=0.17).
There were no significant differences between groups in family size (χ2=7.62, df=3, p=0.07). Specifically, among the 23 families of probands with generalized social phobia, two family members were interviewed in two families, three were interviewed in nine families, four were interviewed in four families, and five or more were interviewed in eight families. Among the 24 families of probands without social phobia, two family members were interviewed in nine families, three were interviewed in 10 families, four were interviewed in one family, and five or more were interviewed in four families.
The mean age of the first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia was 42.2 years (SD=16.5), compared with 39.3 years (SD=15.6) for the first-degree relatives of the probands without social phobia (t=1.18, df=177, p=0.24).

Aggregation of Social Phobia Subtypes in Families

The rates of particular subtypes of social phobia subtypes in families are shown in figure 1. The performance-only (or discrete) subtype was present in 15 (14.2%) of 106 first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia, compared with 11 (14.9%) of 74 first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia (continuity-adjusted χ2=0.01, df=1, p=0.94).
We next considered in aggregate the performance-only or limited-interactional subtype—a grouping synonymous with the nongeneralized subtype. This was found in 24 (22.6%) of 106 first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia, compared with 13 (17.6%) of 74 first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia (continuity-adjusted χ2=0.41, df=1, p=0.52).
The generalized subtype of social phobia was present in 28 (26.4%) of 106 first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia, compared with two (2.7%) of 74 first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia (relative risk=9.7, 95% confidence interval=2.51–38.1). The proportion of affected individuals per family (excluding the proband) was 0.23 (SD=0.21) for the families of probands with generalized social phobia, compared with 0.4 (SD=0.14) for the families of probands without social phobia (z=3.63, p=0.0003). (Because of the presence of ties in the ranked values, a permutation test of the Wilcoxon statistic was also performed by sampling from the possible random assignments of probands to families [38], yielding a similar value of p=0.0002 in 20,000 permutation samples.)

Aggregation of Avoidant Personality Disorder in Families

Avoidant personality disorder was present in 21 (19.8%) of 106 first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia, compared with none of 74 first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia (relative risk not meaningful because no family members of probands without social phobia were affected); each of these individuals also had generalized social phobia. The proportion of affected individuals with avoidant personality disorder per family was significantly higher for families of probands with generalized social phobia (mean=0.17, SD=0.18) than for families of probands without social phobia, who had none (z=4.15, p=0.0001; a permutation test of 20,000 samples similarly gave p=0.0001). The likelihood of a relative having avoidant personality disorder was not associated with the presence of avoidant personality disorder in the probands with generalized social phobia: four (26.7%) of 15 first-degree relatives of six probands with generalized social phobia but not avoidant personality disorder had avoidant personality disorder, compared with 18 (30.5%) of 59 first-degree relatives of 17 probands with generalized social phobia plus avoidant personality disorder (p=1.00, Fisher's exact test).

Age at Onset and Family History

There was a nonsignificant trend for age at onset to be lower in the probands with a positive family history of generalized social phobia (mean=11.1 years, SD=4.5) than in the probands who had no family history of generalized social phobia (mean=15.6 years, SD=8.5) (t=1.71, df=21, p<0.11). There was no difference, however, in the rates of avoidant personality disorder (or generalized social phobia) among relatives of probands with early-onset (prior to age 13) versus late-onset (age 13 or later) generalized social phobia (p>0.10).

Other Anxiety Disorders, Major Depressive Disorder, and Eating Disorders

Lifetime rates of other anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder (including the subcategory of early-onset major depression [i.e., age at onset ≤30 years]) (41, 42) are shown in table 1. Although there were no significant differences in rates of any of these disorders among the first-degree relatives of the two proband groups, rates of panic disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, and major depression were elevated among relatives of probands with generalized social phobia. Given the relatively small number of subjects and our resultant inability to control for rates of proband comorbidity for each of these disorders, these data should be considered of exploratory value only and will require confirmation in future studies.

DISCUSSION

In this direct-interview family study, we found that the rate of generalized social phobia—but not discrete or nongeneralized social phobia—was approximately 10 times higher (26.4% versus 2.7%) among the first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia probands than among the first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia. These observations confirm the finding from an earlier study of a higher rate of social phobia among relatives of probands with generalized social phobia (26) and extend these findings by indicating that it is the generalized subtype that is uniquely higher.
The fact that the generalized form of social phobia “bred true” in this study, in the sense that other forms of social phobia were equally common among relatives of probands with generalized social phobia and relatives of probands without social phobia, suggests that generalized social phobia may be distinct from less pervasive forms of social phobia (11, 19, 26). If the generalized versus nongeneralized distinction were purely one of severity, then one would have expected to see higher rates not only of severe social phobia (i.e., the generalized type) among first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia but also of less severe forms of social phobia (i.e., the nongeneralized type) as well. The absence of such a tendency in our data is further supportive of this subtype distinction on a familial basis. Still, our data are suggestive but not confirmatory in this regard because we lacked the contrasting control necessary to confirm a causal distinction between the generalized and nongeneralized subtypes. To do so one would need to examine the morbid risks for generalized and nongeneralized social phobia among the relatives of probands with nongeneralized social phobia.
We found that avoidant personality disorder was confined to the relatives of probands with generalized social phobia. In all cases (both in probands and in relatives), avoidant personality disorder occurred only when generalized social phobia was also present. This observation, rather than reflecting true comorbidity per se, most likely is due to the substantially overlapping criteria sets for the two disorders (2730). In our opinion, the observation that avoidant personality disorder is strongly familial must be interpreted in this context, i.e., that it is for all intents and purposes just another way of saying that generalized social phobia is strongly familial. On the other hand, our failure to find higher rates of avoidant personality disorder in relatives of probands with early-onset phobia than in relatives of probands with late-onset phobia does not fully support a continuum model for these disorders (i.e., that avoidant personality disorder is a more severe form of generalized social phobia). In order to fully understand the nature of the familial relatedness between avoidant personality disorder and generalized social phobia, it would be necessary to conduct a family study where four groups of probands (and their relatives) could be examined: subjects with avoidant personality disorder and generalized social phobia; subjects with avoidant personality disorder but no generalized social phobia (if such people exist); subjects with generalized social phobia and no avoidant personality disorder; and subjects without either generalized social phobia or avoidant personality disorder.
Although we assessed rates of other anxiety and mood disorders in the relatives in this study, our findings are inconclusive with respect to determining their familial relatedness to social phobia. The reason for this is that the number of subjects in our study was simply too small for this purpose. In order to examine the familial relatedness of some of the conditions that are frequently comorbid with social phobia in clinical samples (e.g., patients with major depression, panic disorder, and/or agoraphobia) (19–25, 43, 44), it will be necessary to include sufficient probands with and without each of these comorbid disorders to permit these informative contrasts to be made (45, 46).
One common comorbid condition that, regrettably, we did not evaluate in this study is alcoholism. There lingers considerable controversy about the familial relationship between alcohol abuse and dependency and social phobia and, indeed, about whether individuals with social phobia are at greater risk for alcoholism (4750). We chose not to invest our limited resources in the thorough assessment of substance use disorders in this study, but we recognize this as a shortcoming of the present study and hope to be able to address this important issue in future studies.
Another potential shortcoming of this study might be our use of telephone interviews whenever a face-to-face interview was not possible. Although we did not find systematic differences in diagnosis on the basis of the mode of interview, the possibility that the results would have been different if all subjects had been interviewed in person cannot be excluded. On the other hand, our use of telephone interviews when necessary permitted us to interview an extraordinarily high proportion of living relatives, and we would submit that this advantage far outweighed any potential disadvantages of telephone interviews. Furthermore, there is strong support in the literature (51, 52) for the utility of telephone interviews and their comparability to face-to-face interviews for the diagnosis of lifetime psychiatric disorders. In our previous research (53), we have used telephone interviews to advantage in the assessment of social phobia and social phobic symptoms.
In interpreting our findings, it must be remembered that with a family study we are unable to discern to what extent the familial nature of generalized social phobia is truly “heritable,” as opposed to what extent it is the product of a particular constellation of family-specific experiential factors. For example, there is reason to hypothesize that particular parenting styles might create or at least perpetuate social phobia in children (54, 55). Testing this hypothesis, however, must take into consideration the probability that parental genetic makeup influences parenting behavior (56). If future studies are able to determine that there is a heritable component to social phobia—and this would be strongly suspected to be the case, given the extant data—then it will be prudent to ask the question, “What exactly is inherited?” One plausible candidate is the temperament known as “behavioral inhibition” (57, 58).
Behavioral inhibition refers to the tendency among some young children to respond to the unfamiliar with wariness and avoidance (57, 58). Rates of behavioral inhibition are higher among children of parents with panic disorder and agoraphobia, and behavioral inhibition itself may represent a childhood precursor to social phobia (5961). To test this hypothesis, it will be important to include “high-risk” probands—children of parents with generalized social phobia—in future studies in which behavioral inhibition is assessed. It will also be important to follow the children prospectively to determine to what extent behavioral inhibition represents a temperamental precursor (or “risk factor”) for the eventual crystallization of generalized social phobia.
In summary, we conclude that generalized social phobia appears to be a familial form of the disorder. More work is required to determine whether other forms of social phobia (e.g., discrete or nongeneralized subtypes) are also familial and, moreover, whether these forms segregate distinctly. Once the phenotypic spectrum of transmission is determined, it will be possible to use a variety of research approaches (62) to elucidate genetic factors that might be important for the expression of social phobia as a disorder and, perhaps, shyness as a trait.

APPENDIX 1. Social Phobic Situations Covered by the Family Study Interview

Performance Situations
Public speaking
Performing in public (other than public speaking)
Eating in front of others
Writing in front of others
Urinating in a public bathroom
Taking a test
Entering a room where people are already seated
Using a pay phone (with someone standing nearby)
Trying on clothing in a store
Playing charades
Speaking up at a meeting
Interactional Situations
Speaking on the telephone (unobserved)
Interacting with strangers (e.g., asking directions)
Attending social gatherings (e.g., house parties, weddings)
Interacting with the opposite sex (e.g., asking someone out for a date)
Dealing with an authority figure (e.g., teacher, boss)
Returning items to a store
Making eye contact
Expressing disagreement/disapproval to someone
Ordering food in a restaurant
Dealing with a salesperson
Conversing in a group (e.g., at a party, in informal situations at work)
TABLE 1
FIGURE 1. First-Degree Relatives of Probands With and Without Social Phobia or Avoidant Personality Disorder Who Had Social Phobia or Avoidant Personality Disordera
aSubjects with nongeneralized social phobia include all subjects diagnosed as having discrete phobia; nongeneralized and generalized types are mutually exclusive. Significantly more first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia than first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia had generalized social phobia (continuity-adjusted χ2=16.0, df=1, p<0.0005). Significantly more first-degree relatives of probands with generalized social phobia than first-degree relatives of probands without social phobia had avoidant personality disorder (continuity-adjusted χ2=14.7, df=1, p<0.0005).

Footnote

Received Oct. 18, 1996; revision received July 21, 1997; accepted Aug. 25, 1997. From the Anxiety and Traumatic Stress Disorders Research Program, Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, and the St. Boniface General Hospital Research Centre and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada. Address reprint requests to Dr. Stein, Anxiety and Traumatic Stress Disorders Research Program, Department of Psychiatry (0985), University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Dr., La Jolla, CA 92093-0985; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported in part by a grant from the St. Boniface General Hospital Research Foundation. The authors thank Abby J. Fyer, M.D., Salvatore Mannuzza, Ph.D., and Diana O. Perkins, M.D., for their suggestions and Fred Wright, Ph.D., for statistical advice.

References

1.
Magee WJ, Eaton WW, Wittchen H-U, McGonagle KA, Kessler RC: Agoraphobia, simple phobia, and social phobia in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:159–168
2.
Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, Nelson CB, Hughes M, Eshleman S, Wittchen H-U, Kendler KS: Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:8–19
3.
Stein MB, Walker JR, Forde DR: Setting diagnostic thresholds for social phobia: considerations from a community survey of social anxiety. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:408–412
4.
Schneier FR, Johnson J, Hornig C, Liebowitz M, Weissman M: Social phobia: comorbidity and morbidity in an epidemiologic sample. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:282–288
5.
Davidson JRT, Hughes DL, George LK, Blazer DG: The epidemiology of social phobia: findings from the Duke Epidemiological Catchment Area Study. Psychol Med 1993; 23:709–718
6.
Schneier FR, Heckelman LR, Garfinkel R, Campeas R, Fallon BA, Gitow A, Street L, Del Bene D, Liebowitz MR: Functional impairment in social phobia. J Clin Psychiatry 1994; 55:322–331
7.
Davidson JRT, Hughes DC, George LK, Blazer DG: The boundary of social phobia: exploring the threshold. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:975–983
8.
Stein MB, Walker JR, Forde DR: Public speaking fears in a community sample: prevalence, impact on functioning, and diagnostic classification. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:169–174
9.
Weiller E, Bisserbe J-C, Boyer P, Lepine J-P, Lecrubier Y: Social phobia in general health care: an unrecognised undertreated disabling disorder. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168:169–174
10.
Safren SA, Heimberg RG, Brown EJ, Holle C: Quality of life in social phobia. Anxiety 1997; 4:126–133
11.
Stein MB: How shy is too shy? Lancet 1996; 347:1131–1132
12.
Den Boer JA: Social phobia: epidemiology, recognition, and treatment. BMJ 1997; 315:796–800
13.
Knowles JA, Mannuzza S, Fyer AJ: Heritability of social phobia, in Social Phobia: Clinical and Research Perspectives. Edited by Stein MB. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1995, pp 147–162
14.
Chapman TF, Mannuzza S, Fyer AJ: Epidemiology and family studies of social phobia, in Social Phobia: Diagnosis, Assessment, and Treatment. Edited by Heimberg RG, Liebowitz MR, Hope DA, Schneier FR. New York, Guilford Press, 1995, pp 21–39
15.
Kendler KS, Neale MC, Kessler RC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: The genetic epidemiology of phobias in women: the interrelationship of agoraphobia, social phobia, situational phobia, and simple phobia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:273–281
16.
Reich J, Yates W: Family history of psychiatric disorders in social phobia. Compr Psychiatry 1988; 29:72–75
17.
Fyer AJ, Mannuzza S, Chapman T, Liebowitz MR, Klein DF: A direct interview family study of social phobia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:286–293
18.
Fyer AJ, Mannuzza A, Chapman TF, Martin LY, Klein DF: Specificity in familial aggregation of phobic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:564–573
19.
Hazen AL, Stein MB: Clinical phenomenology and comorbidity, in Social Phobia: Clinical and Research Perspectives. Edited by Stein MB. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1995, pp 3–42
20.
Mannuzza S, Fyer AJ, Liebowitz MR, Klein DF: Delineating the boundaries of social phobia: its relationship to panic disorder and agoraphobia. J Anxiety Disorders 1990; 4:1–59
21.
Heimberg RG, Hope DA, Dodge CS, Becker RE: DSM-III-R subtypes of social phobia. J Nerv Ment Dis 1990; 178:172–179
22.
Gelernter CS, Stein MB, Tancer ME, Uhde TW: An examination of syndromal validity and diagnostic subtypes in social phobia and panic disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 1992; 53:23–27
23.
Heimberg RG, Holt CS, Schneier FR, Liebowitz MR: The issue of subtypes in the diagnosis of social phobia. J Anxiety Disorders 1993; 7:249–269
24.
Turner SM, Beidel DC, Townsley RM: Social phobia: a comparison of specific and generalized subtypes and avoidant personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101:326–331
25.
Brown EJ, Heimberg RG, Juster HR: Social phobia subtype and avoidant personality disorder: effect on severity of social phobia, impairment, and outcome of cognitive-behavioral treatment. Behavior Therapy 1995; 26:467–486
26.
Mannuzza S, Schneier FR, Chapman TF, Liebowitz MR, Klein DF, Fyer AJ: Generalized social phobia: reliability and validity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:230–237
27.
Schneier FR, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Fyer AJ, Liebowitz MR: The relationship of social phobia subtypes and avoidant personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry 1991; 32:496–502
28.
Holt CS, Heimberg RG, Hope DA: Avoidant personality disorder and the generalized subtype of social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101:318–325
29.
Herbert JD, Hope DA, Bellack AS: Validity of the distinction between generalized social phobia and avoidant personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101:332–339
30.
Jansen MA, Arntz A, Merckelbach H, Mersch PPA: Personality disorders and features in social phobia and panic disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 103:391–395
31.
Pauls DL, Alsobrook JP II, Goodman W, Rasmussen S, Leckman JF: A family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:76–84
32.
Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R—Patient Version 1.0 (SCID-P). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1990
33.
Kendler KS: The super-normal control group in psychiatric genetics: possible artifactual evidence for coaggregation. Psychiatr Genet 1990; 1:45–53
34.
Klein DF: The utility of the super-normal control group in psychiatric genetics. Psychiatr Genet 1993; 3:17–19
35.
American Psychiatric Association: DSM-IV Options Book: Work in Progress. Washington, DC, APA, 1991
36.
Mannuzza S, Fyer AJ, Klein DF, Endicott J: The Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia—Lifetime Version Modified for the Study of Anxiety Disorders (SADS-LA): rationale and conceptual development. J Psychiatr Res 1986; 20:317–325
37.
Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R Personality Disorders (SCID-II). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1990
38.
Cox DR, Hinkley DV: Theoretical Statistics. London, Chapman & Hall, 1974
39.
Khoury MJ, Beaty TH, Cohen BH: Fundamentals of Genetic Epidemiology. New York, Oxford University Press, 1993
40.
Fleiss JL: Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions, 2nd ed. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1981
41.
Weissman MM, Wickramaratne P, Adams PB, Lish JD, Horwath E, Charney D, Woods SW, Lehman E, Frosch E: The relationship between panic disorder and major depression: a new family study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:767–780
42.
Goldstein RB, Weissman MM, Adams PB, Horwath E, Lish JD, Charney D, Woods SW, Sobin C, Wickramaratne PJ: Psychiatric disorders in relatives of probands with panic disorder and/or major depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:383–394
43.
Horwath E, Wolk SI, Goldstein RB, Wickramaratne P, Sobin C, Adams P, Lish JD, Weissman MM: Is the comorbidity between social phobia and panic disorder due to familial cotransmission or other factors? Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:574–582
44.
Bellodi L, Battaglia M, Diaferia G, Draisci A, Sciuto G: Lifetime prevalence of depression and family history of patients with panic disorder and social phobia. Eur Psychiatry 1993; 8:147–152
45.
Wickramaratne PJ: Selecting control groups for studies of familial aggregation of disease. J Clin Epidemiol 1995; 48:1019–1029
46.
Fyer AJ, Mannuzza S, Chapman TF, Lipsitz J, Martin LY, Klein DF: Panic disorder and social phobia: effects of comorbidity on familial transmission. Anxiety 1996; 2:173–178
47.
Schuckit MA, Hesselbrock VM: Alcohol dependence and anxiety disorders: what is the relationship? Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1723–1734
48.
Merikangas KR, Risch NR, Weissman MM: Comorbidity and co-transmission of alcoholism, anxiety and depression. Psychol Med 1994; 24:69–80
49.
Rohde P, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR: Comparability of telephone and face-to-face interviews in assessing axis I and II disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:1593–1598
50.
Schuckit MA, Hesselbrock VM, Tipp J, Nurnberger JI, Anthenelli RM, Crowe RR: The prevalence of major anxiety disorders in relatives of alcohol dependent men and women. J Stud Alcohol 1995; 56:309–317
51.
Merikangas KR, Angst J: Comorbidity and social phobia: evidence from clinical, epidemiologic, and genetic studies. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995; 244:297–303
52.
Sobin C, Weissman MM, Goldstein RB, Adams P, Wickramaratne P, Warner V, Lish JD: Diagnostic interviewing for family studies: comparing telephone and face-to-face methods for the diagnosis of lifetime psychiatric disorders. Psychiatr Genet 1993; 3:227–233
53.
Stein MB, Walker JR, Forde DR: Reply to MN Kessler: Will social phobia subjects answer the telephone? (letter). Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:653–654
54.
Stemberger RT, Turner SM, Beidel DC, Calhoun KS: Social phobia: an analysis of possible developmental factors. J Abnorm Psychol 1995; 104:526–531
55.
Rapee RM: The potential role of childrearing practices in the development of anxiety and depression. Clin Psychol Rev 1997; 17:47–67
56.
Pérusse D, Neale MC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: Human parental behavior: evidence for genetic influence and potential implication for gene-culture transmission. Behav Genet 1994; 24:327–335
57.
Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N: Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science 1988; 240:167–171
58.
Robinson JL, Reznick JS, Kagan J, Corley R: The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: a twin study. Dev Psychol 1992; 28:1030–1037
59.
Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, Gersten M, Hirshfeld DR, Meminger SR, Herman JB, Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N: Behavioral inhibition in children of parents with panic disorder and agoraphobia: a controlled study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:463–470
60.
Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, Hirshfeld DR, Bolduc EA, Chaloff J: Behavioral inhibition in children: a possible precursor to panic disorder or social phobia. J Clin Psychiatry 1991; 52(suppl):5–9
61.
Turner SM, Beidel DC, Wolff PL: Is behavioral inhibition related to the anxiety disorders? Clin Psychol Rev 1996; 16:157–172
62.
Pauls DL: Behavioral disorders: lessons in linkage. Nat Genet 1993; 3:4–5

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 90 - 97
PubMed: 9433344

History

Published online: 1 January 1998
Published in print: January 1998

Authors

Details

Mariette J. Chartier, R.N., B.Sc.
Andrea L. Hazen, Ph.D.
Manuel E. Tancer, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share