Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 May 2008

The Other-Race Effect in Face Processing Among African American and Caucasian Individuals With Schizophrenia

Abstract

Objective: Studies of emotion recognition abilities in schizophrenia show greater impairment for non-Caucasians with schizophrenia compared with Caucasians. These studies, however, included only Caucasian faces as stimuli. There is evidence from healthy individuals for a performance disadvantage on face memory and emotion recognition when processing faces from a different ethnicity. The authors sought to measure the “other-race effect” in schizophrenia, which could account for previous findings and provide information about sensitivity to such social cues in patients. Method: The study included 540 participants from four groups: African Americans with schizophrenia (N=135), Caucasians with schizophrenia (N=135), African American community comparison subjects (N=135), and Caucasian community comparison subjects (N=135). All participants completed face recognition and facial emotion identification tasks that included both Caucasian and African American faces as stimuli. Results: Although comparison participants performed better than individuals with schizophrenia across all tasks, both comparison participants and participants with schizophrenia exhibited a strong and significant other-race effect for face memory and emotion recognition. The magnitude of the other-race effect did not differ between these two groups. Conclusions: These findings reveal an intact other-race effect in patients with schizophrenia and highlight a methodological concern in the measurement of face processing abilities in schizophrenia, namely, that findings of greater impairment in African American patients are spurious when Caucasian faces are used as stimuli. Despite overall impairments in face memory and emotion recognition, the presence of a normative other-race effect in schizophrenia may reflect typical experiences with faces during development.
Emotion recognition deficits in schizophrenia are well established (see references 14 for reviews) and are strongly related to social functioning (5) . Because these deficits are related to abnormal brain function in a complex neural network underlying face processing and emotion recognition (6), understanding them may help elucidate the neurobiological underpinnings of schizophrenia.
Emotion recognition deficits have been related to clinical and behavioral features as well as neural measures, and in recent years cultural, racial, and ethnic influences on emotion processing have been explored. For example, Brekke and colleagues (7) reported that Caucasians with schizophrenia performed better on a facial emotion perception task than African American and Latino patients. Similarly, Habel et al. (8) reported better emotion discrimination abilities among American and German participants compared with Indian participants. While these studies appear to support an emotion recognition advantage for Caucasians with schizophrenia, they both failed to address a potential confound by including only Caucasian faces as stimuli. This confound becomes particularly important when considering a large literature from healthy individuals demonstrating clear cross-racial effects for face memory and emotion recognition. An other-race effect has been established indicating that memory for, and discrimination among, other-race faces is impaired relative to own-race faces (9) . A meta-analysis of culture and emotion perception revealed an in-group advantage such that accuracy of emotion identification is higher when the perceiver is the same race as the person expressing the emotion (i.e., the stimulus) (10) . Such cross-racial effects may have confounded previous results, artificially inflating between-group differences and suggesting disproportionately greater impairments in non-Caucasian individuals with schizophrenia.
In this study, we address this potential confound by evaluating the presence of an other-race effect for face and emotion processing in individuals with schizophrenia. Caucasian and African American patients with schizophrenia and healthy community comparison subjects completed tasks of face recognition memory and facial emotion identification that included both Caucasian and African American faces as stimuli. It was anticipated that compared with the healthy comparison subjects, patients would show impairments in both face memory and emotion identification. The presence of an other-race effect was explored by examining participant race by stimulus race interactions in both face memory and emotion recognition. Such interactions would indicate the extent to which patients and healthy comparison subjects perform disproportionately worse at identifying emotions and recognizing previously seen faces when the participant and stimulus faces are of a different race. An other-race effect in schizophrenia would show a normative sensitivity to same- versus other-race cues in faces, which would contrast with the marked face processing deficits observed in individuals with this disorder.

Method

Participants

The study included 540 participants from four groups: African Americans with schizophrenia (N=135), Caucasians with schizophrenia (N=135), African American community comparison subjects (N=135), and Caucasian community comparison subjects (N=135). All individuals participated in larger, multisite studies investigating the genetic architecture of candidate endophenotypic markers of schizophrenia. Caucasian participants were recruited through the Multi-Generational Investigation of Schizophrenia (11) and the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia (12) . African American participants were recruited through the Project Among African Americans to Explore Risks for Schizophrenia (13) and the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia. Details of ascertainment and assessment methods are provided in primary papers associated with each study (1113) and are summarized below.
After all procedures were fully explained, all participants provided written informed consent. All participants received the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies, which assesses psychotic, mood, and substance-related symptoms on which reliable DSM-IV diagnoses are based (14), as well as self-reported demographic and medical history information. After review of results from the Diagnostic Interview for Genetics Studies, the Family Interview for Genetic Studies, and available medical records, lifetime best-estimate final diagnoses for each participant were determined via consensus review by at least two faculty-level clinicians at the ascertaining site. Only patients who received a best-estimate final diagnosis of schizophrenia and comparison subjects with no known medical or psychiatric diagnoses and no first-degree relatives with psychosis or mood disorder were included in the study. These criteria yielded 135 Caucasians with schizophrenia. In order to match cell sizes, we selected participants for the other groups from the larger studies by randomly sampling across sites within each larger study to match participants at each level of education.

Measures

Facial memory and emotion recognition tasks were administered as part of a larger computerized neurocognitive battery validated in healthy individuals (15) and individuals with schizophrenia (16) . The battery was administered in a fixed order so that face memory tasks always preceded the emotion recognition task.

Face Memory

In the Penn Face Memory Test (17), participants are shown 20 gray-scale photographs of neutral faces (one every 5 seconds) and are then tested for immediate and delayed (20 minutes) recognition. For both immediate and delayed recognition, participants are presented with the 20 originally viewed faces mixed with 20 novel foils. Foils are equated to target faces for age, sex, and race ( Figure 1 ). The number of correctly recognized faces is the dependent variable for both the immediate and delayed conditions.
Figure 1. Characteristics of Stimuli in Face Memory and Emotion Recognition Tasks Used in Assessing the Other-Race Effect in African American and Caucasian Patients With Schizophrenia and Healthy Comparison Subjects
a Targets are the same as those of the immediate face memory task.
b Foils are different from those of immediate face memory task.

Emotion Recognition

The Penn Emotion Recognition Task (18) comprises 40 color photographs of faces expressing one of four basic emotions—happiness, sadness, anger, or fear—or showing a neutral expression. The participant’s task is to identify the emotion expressed by each face. The race, sex, and target emotion of stimulus faces are listed in Figure 1 . Because there were fewer Hispanic and Asian faces and because we were primarily interested in Caucasian and African American faces, we omitted responses for the Hispanic and Asian stimuli from our analysis. The dependent variable was the number of correctly identified expressions.

Statistical Analyses

Hypotheses were tested with three repeated-measures ANOVAs, one for performance on each measure (immediate and delayed memory and emotion recognition). Stimulus and participant sex were also considered. Thus, within each analysis, stimulus race and sex were entered as within-subject factors and participant race, sex, and group (schizophrenia patients and comparison subjects) were entered as between-subject factors. Age was entered as a covariate in each analysis to control for differences in age across groups.

Results

As expected, the four groups differed significantly in sex distribution (χ 2 =40.64, p<0.001), with a greater proportion of males in the patient groups ( Table 1 ). They also differed in age (F=7.51, df=3, 536, p<0.001), with Caucasian comparison subjects being older on average than both Caucasian (p<0.001) and African American patients (p=0.006).

Face Memory

For immediate face memory, there was a significant main effect for group, demonstrating that comparison subjects had better target recognition than participants with schizophrenia (mean=82.7% and mean=74.5%, respectively; F=69.16, df=1, 531, p<0.001; partial eta-squared [ η p 2 ]=0.115). Main effects were also evident for stimulus race and stimulus sex such that African American faces were better remembered than Caucasian faces (mean=81.7% and mean=75.5%, respectively; F=6.75, df=1, 531, p=0.01; η p 2 =0.013) and male faces were better remembered than female faces (mean=85% and mean=72.2%, respectively; F=46.33, df=1, 531, p<0.001; η p 2 =0.08). Notably, there was no main effect of participant race or sex.
As predicted, there was a significant interaction between stimulus race and participant race (F=46.00, df=1, 531, p<0.001; η p 2 =0.08), showing an other-race effect: Caucasian participants remembered both Caucasian and African American faces equally well, while African American participants remembered African American faces better than Caucasian faces ( Figure 2 ). This interaction did not differ across groups (schizophrenia patients and comparison subjects), and there was no significant three-way interaction by group. Additionally, the interaction between stimulus sex and participant sex was not significant, indicating that participants did not better remember same-sex faces.
Figure 2. Performance on Face Processing Measures, by Participant Race and Stimulus Race
Performances on delayed face memory were similar. Comparison subjects remembered more faces than did schizophrenia patients (mean=84.6% and mean=77.6%, respectively; F=44.94, df=1, 531, p<0.001; η p 2 =0.078), and male faces were better remembered than female faces (mean=82.8% and mean=79.4%, respectively; F=16.11, df=1, 531, p<0.001; η p 2 =0.029). The main effect of stimulus race was no longer significant, nor were main effects of participant race or sex. More important, the interaction between stimulus race and participant race was again significant (F=42.66, df=1, 531, p<0.001; η p 2 =0.074), demonstrating that both African American and Caucasian participants better remembered same-race faces ( Figure 2 ). Again, this other-race effect did not differ between the schizophrenia patients and comparison subjects, as indicated by a nonsignificant three-way interaction with group. Finally, in delayed memory, there was also a significant stimulus sex by participant sex interaction (F=6.57, df=1, 531, p=0.011; η p 2 =0.012) such that males were more likely to remember male faces and females were approximately equally likely to remember both male and female faces. This effect did not differ across groups. Thus, in delayed memory, all participants better remembered faces of their own race as compared with other-race faces, and male participants showed an advantage for remembering male faces.

Emotion Recognition

Comparison subjects again scored better than patients in emotion recognition (mean=82.8% and mean=75.6%, respectively; F=48.11, df=1, 531, p<0.001; η p 2 =0.083). A main effect of stimulus race was also apparent (African American faces: mean=83%; Caucasian faces: mean=75.3%; F=13.97, df=1, 531, p<0.001; η p 2 =0.026), indicating that African American stimuli were more likely to be accurately labeled than Caucasian stimuli, and there were no significant main effects for stimulus sex or participant sex. Contrary to previous work (7, 8), there was no main effect for participant race. Instead, there was a significant interaction between stimulus race and participant race (F=3.99, df=1, 531, p=0.046; η p 2 =0.007), demonstrating that African American participants were worse than their Caucasian counterparts in identifying emotion on Caucasian faces but performed equally well on African American faces ( Figure 2 ). As with face memory, this effect did not differ between patient and comparison groups. Lastly, the interaction between stimulus sex and participant sex was not significant.

Discussion

The primary purpose of this study was to assess the other-race effect, or differential abilities in processing same- versus other-race faces, in schizophrenia. The magnitude of this phenomenon could account for previous reports of greater impairment in emotion perception abilities among non-Caucasian compared with Caucasian individuals with schizophrenia when Caucasian stimuli were used. We found evidence supportive of an other-race effect in both comparison subjects and patients. In immediate face memory, African American participants were more likely to recognize same-race faces than other-race faces, and in delayed face memory, all participants better recognized faces of their own race as compared with other-race faces. Likewise, on emotion recognition, both African American and Caucasian participants performed similarly when identifying emotion on African American faces, but African American participants—in both the patient and comparison groups—performed disproportionately worse on Caucasian faces. These interactions demonstrate the presence of an intact other-race effect in schizophrenia that affects both face memory and emotion recognition.
Two previous studies (7, 8) found that in patients with schizophrenia, Caucasians performed better on emotion recognition tasks than non-Caucasians. Whereas these studies used only Caucasian stimuli, our tasks included stimuli with both Caucasian and African American faces and failed to find a main effect of race in comparison subjects or patients. Notably, had we analyzed only responses to Caucasian stimuli, our results would have replicated those of the previous studies. Instead, our findings indicate that an other-race effect may account for the differences described in these earlier studies and highlight an important measurement issue in the extension of face processing and emotion research to clinical populations.
Because African American patients did not differ from their Caucasian counterparts after cross-racial effects were considered, the two groups appear equally impaired on face and emotion recognition and are thus likely to show similar dysfunction in neural regions underlying these abilities. The fusiform face area, thought to be an experientially modulated region of the brain implicated in face specialization (19), shows sensitivity to racial information in healthy individuals (20) . While individuals with schizophrenia demonstrate reduced activation in the fusiform face area (21), the presence of an intact other-race effect suggests that this brain region may also show differential activation in patients, depending on race. Likewise, the amplitude of the face-specific N170 component of the visual event-related potential response is modulated by stimulus race (22) . Recent work demonstrates smaller N170 responses in individuals with schizophrenia while viewing faces (23), but again, it is possible that patients may show modulation of this response based on race. Such findings would not only mirror the behavioral findings reported here but would suggest a potentially interesting dissociation between functionally normal patterns of face processing within the context of overall deficits.
While individuals with schizophrenia are impaired in a wide range of face processing abilities, including orienting to faces (24), visual scanning (25), face memory, and facial emotion recognition, the presence of an intact other-race effect in schizophrenia suggests a normative developmental pattern. An other-race effect can be found in very young children (26), and it increases over the course of childhood (27) . These developmental changes suggest that the other-race effect may be experientially related and driven by a disproportional exposure to same-race faces relative to other-race faces during childhood (2830) . Thus, despite face processing deficits in other domains, the other-race effect exhibited by individuals with schizophrenia in this study may reflect normative developmental experience with faces and perhaps even normative development of the neural mechanisms of face processing. It would be informative, therefore, to compare individuals with schizophrenia to individuals with other neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism who similarly show face processing deficits in adulthood (31, 32) but demonstrate qualitative and quantitative deficits with facial experience throughout childhood (33, 34) .
In addition to a significant other-race effect, individuals with schizophrenia also showed expected impairments in both face memory and emotion recognition relative to comparison subjects. Although our results cannot speak to the specificity of face processing impairments or the magnitude of face memory deficits compared with nonface or verbal memory, it is interesting to note that both groups performed better on delayed face memory relative to immediate memory. This finding is consistent with previous work using a recognition paradigm with both face stimuli (35, 36) and other spatial stimuli (37) . The effect likely occurs because presenting all target stimuli for a second time in the immediate memory condition may enhance recognition during the delayed task.
While this study offers new information about face processing abilities in schizophrenia, some limitations should be considered. First, the tasks used were not specifically designed to examine an other-race effect and included fewer African American stimuli than Caucasian stimuli. Although this design still meets requirements for adequately testing an in-group advantage as espoused by Matsumoto (38) and Elfenbein and Ambady (39), the disparity in our task compositions may help explain some of the unexpected findings, such as a main effect for stimulus race in the immediate face memory and emotion recognition tasks. It is possible that the smaller number of African American faces caused them to be perceived as more novel and salient, thus resulting in better performance for these stimuli. However, because performance on African American faces differed as a function of participant race, the unexpected main effect for stimulus race does not affect the other-race effect findings. Similarly, the presence of an other-race effect even with unequal numbers of Caucasian and African American stimuli suggests a robust phenomenon that should be considered in future studies and further clarified with balanced stimuli. Second, face memory stimuli were presented in gray-scale images, whereas emotion recognition stimuli were presented in color images. To our knowledge, no studies have examined whether the color format of face stimuli influences the other-race effect; however, this possibility merits further study.
These limitations notwithstanding, our findings show that despite overall impairments in face memory and emotion recognition, individuals with schizophrenia demonstrated a pattern of cross-racial effects similar to that of healthy people, suggesting normative experiences with faces during development. These results also reveal an important methodological concern in measurement of face processing abilities in individuals with schizophrenia that should be considered in task development and in future investigations.

Footnotes

Received Oct. 13, 2007; revision received Nov. 28, 2007; accepted Dec. 18, 2007 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07101604). From the Department of Neuropsychiatry, University of Pennsylvania. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Pinkham, Department of Neuropsychiatry, University of Pennsylvania, 10th Floor, Gates Pavilion, Philadelphia 19104; [email protected] (e-mail).
All authors report no competing interests.
Supported by NIMH grants T32 MH-019112, MH-042191, R01 MH-066121, R01 MH-065578, and R01 MH-060722 to the University of Pennsylvania. The authors thank J. Daniel Ragland, Ph.D., Felipe Da Silva, and Nick Phend for their assistance in data processing and validation.
Data collection for the Multiplex-Genetics Investigation was supported by NIMH collaborative RO1’s MH42191, MH63480, MH61622, MH60722, MH59490, and MO1RR0040 and the efforts of the following investigators: Laura Almasy, Ph.D., Vishwajit L. Nimgaonkar, M.D, Ph.D., and Michael F. Pogue-Geile, Ph.D. Data collection for the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia was supported by NIMH collaborative RO1’s MH065562, MH43518, MH065554, MH65707, MH065571, MH65588, MH65578, and MH65558 and the efforts of the following investigators: David L. Braff, M.D., Kristin S. Cadenhead, M.D., Dorcas J. Dobie, M.D., Robert Freedman, M.D., Michael F. Green, Ph.D., Tiffany A. Greenwood, Ph.D., Gregory A. Light, Ph.D., Jim Mintz, Ph.D., Keith H. Nuechterlein, Ph.D., Ann Olincy, M.D., Allen D. Radant, Ph.D., Nicholas J. Schork, Ph.D., Larry J. Seidman, Ph.D., Larry J. Siever, Ph.D., Jeremy M. Silverman, Ph.D., William S. Stone, Ph.D., Neal R. Swerdlow, M.D., Debby W. Tsuang, M.D., Ming T. Tsuang, M.D., Ph.D., and Bruce I. Turetsky, M.D. Data collection for the Project Among African-Americans to Explore Risk for Schizophrenia was supported by NIMH RO1’s MH66006, MH66278, MH066049, MH66181, MH66121, MH066005, MH66050, MH66263, and MH66004 and the efforts of the following investigators: Rodney C.P. Go, Ph.D., Muktar H. Aliyu, M.D., Dr.P.H., Trina Allen, M.D., L. DiAnne Bradford, Ph.D., Bernie Devlin, Ph.D., Neil Edwards, M.D., Joseph Kwentus, M.D., Judith O’Jile, Ph.D., Paul D. Lyons, M.D., Ph.D., Vishwajit L. Nimgaonkar, M.D., Ph.D., Alberto B. Santos, M.D., and Robert M. Savage, Ph.D.

References

1.
Edwards J, Jackson HJ, Pattison PE: Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: a methodological review. Clin Psychol Rev 2002; 22:789–832
2.
Kohler CG, Martin EA: Emotional processing in schizophrenia. Cognit Neuropsychiatry 2006; 11:250–271
3.
Penn DL, Corrigan PW, Bentall RP, Racenstein JM, Newman L: Social cognition in schizophrenia. Psychol Bull 1997; 121:114–132
4.
Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Lieberman J: Implications for the neural basis of social cognition for the study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:815–824
5.
Couture SM, Penn DL, Roberts DL: The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32(suppl 1):S44–S63
6.
Pinkham AE, Gur RE, Gur RC: Affect recognition deficits in schizophrenia: neural substrates and psychopharmacological implications. Expert Rev Neurother 2007; 7:807–816
7.
Brekke JS, Nakagami E, Kee KS, Green MF: Cross-ethnic differences in perception of emotion in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005; 77:289–298
8.
Habel U, Gur RC, Mandal MK, Salloum JB, Gur RE, Schneider F: Emotional processing in schizophrenia across cultures: standardized measures of discrimination and experience. Schizophr Res 2000; 42:57–66
9.
Meissner CA, Brigham JC: Thirty years of investigating the own-race bias in memory for faces: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Public Policy Law 2001; 7:3–35
10.
Elfenbein HA, Ambady N: On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2002; 128:203–235
11.
Gur RE, Nimgaonkar VL, Almasy L, Calkins ME, Ragland JD, Pogue-Geile MF, Kanes S, Blangero J, Gur RC: Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:813–819
12.
Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, Cadenhead KS, Olincy A, Freedman R, Green MF, Greenwood TA, Gur RE, Gur RC, Light GA, Mintz J, Nuechterlein KH, Radant AD, Schork NJ, Seidman LJ, Siever LJ, Silverman JM, Stone WS, Swerdlow NR, Tsuang DW, Tsuang MT, Turetsky BI, Braff DL: The Consortium on the Genetics of Endophenotypes in Schizophrenia: model recruitment, assessment, and endophenotyping methods for a multisite collaboration. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:33–48
13.
Aliyu MH, Calkins ME, Swanson CL Jr, Lyons PD, Savage RM, May R, Wiener H, McLeod-Bryant S, Nimgaonkar VL, Ragland JD, Gur RE, Gur RC, Bradford LD, Edwards N, Kwentus J, McEvoy JP, Santos AB, McCleod-Bryant S, Tennison C, Go RC, Allen TB: Project Among African-Americans to Explore Risks for Schizophrenia (PAARTNERS): recruitment and assessment methods. Schizophr Res 2006; 87:32–44
14.
Nurnberger JI Jr, Blehar MC, Kaufmann CA, York-Cooler C, Simpson SG, Harkavy-Friedman J, Severe JB, Malaspina D, Reich T: Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies: rationale, unique features, and training: NIMH Genetics Initiative. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:849–859
15.
Gur RC, Ragland JD, Moberg PJ, Turner TH, Bilker WB, Kohler C, Siegel SJ, Gur RE: Computerized neurocognitive scanning, I: methodology and validation in healthy people. Neuropsychopharmacol 2001; 25:766–776
16.
Gur RC, Ragland JD, Moberg PJ, Bilker WB, Kohler C, Siegel SJ, Gur RE: Computerized neurocognitive scanning, II: the profile of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacol 2001; 25:777–788
17.
Gur RC, Jaggi JL, Ragland JD, Resnick SM, Shtasel D, Muenz L, Gur RE: Effects of memory processing on regional brain activation: cerebral blood flow in normal subjects. Int J Neurosci 1993; 72:31–44
18.
Kohler CG, Turner TH, Bilker WB, Brensinger CM, Siegel SJ, Kanes SJ, Gur RE, Gur RC: Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: intensity effects and error pattern. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1768–1774
19.
Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Anderson AW, Skudlarski P, Gore JC: Activation of the middle fusiform “face area” increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2:568–573
20.
Golby AJ, Gabrieli JD, Chiao JY, Eberhardt JL: Differential responses in the fusiform region to same-race and other-race faces. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4:845–850
21.
Quintana J, Wong T, Ortiz-Portillo E, Marder SR, Mazziotta JC: Right lateral fusiform gyrus dysfunction during facial information processing in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53:1099–1112
22.
Ito TA, Urland GR: The influence of processing objectives on the perception of faces: an ERP study of race and gender perception. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2005; 5:21–36
23.
Turetsky BI, Kohler CG, Indersmitten T, Bhati MT, Charbonnier D, Gur RC: Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: when and why does it go awry? Schizophr Res 2007; 94:253–263
24.
Sasson N, Tsuchiya N, Hurley R, Couture SM, Penn DL, Adolphs R, Piven J: Orienting to social stimuli differentiates social cognitive impairment in autism and schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia 2007; 45:2580–2588
25.
Green MJ, Williams LM, Davidson D: Visual scanpaths to threat-related faces in deluded schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2003; 119:271–285
26.
Sangrigoli S, de Schonen S: Recognition of own-race and other-race faces by three-month-old infants. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004; 45:1219–1227
27.
Chance JE, Turner AL, Goldstein AG: Development of differential recognition for own- and other-race faces. J Psychol 1982; 112(1st half):29–37
28.
Feinman S, Entwisle DR: Children’s ability to recognize other children’s faces. Child Dev 1976; 47:506–510
29.
Sangrigoli S, Pallier C, Argenti AM, Ventureyra VA, de Schonen S: Reversibility of the other-race effect in face recognition during childhood. Psychol Sci 2005; 16:440–444
30.
Shepherd JW: Social factors in face recognition, in Perceiving and Remembering Faces. Edited by Davies G, Ellis H, Shepherd JW. London, Academic Press, 1981, pp 55–79
31.
Klin A, Jones W, Schultz R, Volkmar F, Cohen D: Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:809–816
32.
Pelphrey KA, Sasson NJ, Reznick JS, Paul G, Goldman BD, Piven J: Visual scanning of faces in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2002; 32:249–261
33.
Dawson G, Webb SJ, McPartland J: Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Dev Neuropsychol 2005; 27:403–424
34.
Sasson NJ: The development of face processing in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2006; 36:381–394
35.
Calkins ME, Gur RC, Ragland JD, Gur RE: Face recognition memory deficits and visual object memory performance in patients with schizophrenia and their relatives. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1963–1966
36.
Conklin HM, Calkins ME, Anderson CW, Dinzeo TJ, Iacono WG: Recognition memory for faces in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40:2314–2324
37.
Glahn DC, Gur RC, Ragland JD, Censits DM, Gur RE: Reliability, performance characteristics, construct validity, and an initial clinical application of a Visual Object Learning Test (VOLT). Neuropsychology 1997; 11:602–612
38.
Matsumoto D: Methodological requirements to test a possible in-group advantage in judging emotions across cultures: comment on Elfenbein and Ambady (2002) and evidence. Psychol Bull 2002; 128:236–242
39.
Elfenbein HA, Ambady N: Is there an in-group advantage in emotion recognition? Psychol Bull 2002; 128:243–249

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 639 - 645
PubMed: 18347000

History

Published online: 1 May 2008
Published in print: May, 2008

Authors

Details

Monica E. Calkins, Ph.D.
Raquel E. Gur, M.D., Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share