Skip to main content
Full access
Editorial
Published Online: 1 October 2008

A Developmental and Evolutionary Perspective on Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: Whence and Whither Compulsive Hoarding?

Going to bed is also complicated for the 2 year old...bedtime demands have often grown into an elaborated and rigid structure. There is a coming upstairs ritual, brushing the teeth ritual, getting into bed, pulling down the shades, kissing, and even a specially worded “good night” ritual.
Gesell and Ilg ( 1, p. 182)
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a prevalent condition that is heterogeneous in its clinical presentation. In addition, there is compelling evidence that supports the value of developmental and dimensional approaches to OCD as well as viewing OCD as part of a larger spectrum of disorders including tic disorders, trichotillomania, body dysmorphic disorder, and possibly compulsive hoarding. Specific subtypes of OCD also need to be considered, especially pediatric-onset and tie-related OCD, as well as pediatric autoimmune neuropsychiatric disorders associated with streptococcal (PANDAS) infections. As a result, there are conflicting views regarding how best to classify OCD in the next edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) (2, 3) . Three of the articles in this issue of the Journal are relevant to this topic and warrant our attention. Before each of these articles and their contributions to the field are considered, a brief overview may be of value.
Beginning around the second year of life most children develop a variety of rituals, habits, routines, and preferences, some of which resemble the behaviors associated with OCD (4, 5) . The idea that compulsive ritualistic behaviors may be normative in young children is not new. Gesell and his colleagues (1, 6) were among the first to recognize that young children—particularly those around the age of two and a half—begin to establish rigid routines that Gesell termed the “ritualisms of the ritualist.” Rather than addressing emotional needs, Gesell believed children engage in rituals to master the tasks of a specific developmental epoch, for instance, matters of feeding, toileting, and dressing. By the age of 3 years more than 80% of parents of children report the presence of a bedtime ritual (4) . Less well known is that for most children a regular sequence exists in which a need to arrange things “just right” or in a symmetrical pattern appears next, followed by the child’s being very concerned with dirt and germs, and finally, the need to collect and store objects. With the exception of hoarding, each of these behaviors peaks at age 3 years. Hoarding, in contrast, shows a monotonic increase, at least until the age of 6 years, when more than 60% of normal children display this trait (4, 5) . The earlier the age at which these concerns appear, the more advanced is the child’s developmental level. It is also striking that children with severe intellectual disabilities whose mental age remains in the low range show a persistence of many of these obsessive-compulsive behaviors (7, 8) . This larger body of data has led some to conclude that obsessive-compulsive behaviors are evolutionarily conserved (9, 10) and have a tendency to reappear normally at particular developmental transitions, such as falling in love or the advent of a firstborn infant (10, 11) .
These developmental data have also supported the view that OCD is composed of temporally stable symptom dimensions (12) and that clinically meaningful OCD arises as a result of a developmental sequence in which familial factors, both genetic and environmental, play critical roles. Within clinical populations the existence of these symptom dimensions appears beyond doubt, as confirmed by our recent meta-analysis that examined the results of more than 20 studies involving close to 5,000 OCD cases (13) . Briefly, these dimensions include 1) forbidden thoughts, 2) obsessions with symmetry and exactness and related ordering and arranging rituals as well as counting and repeating rituals, 3) contamination obsessions and washing compulsions, and 4) hoarding obsessions and compulsions. How useful to the clinician are these dimensions? Do they inform clinical practice? For example, do they reflect patterns of clinical response or longer-term outcome? More to the point, are they aligned with specific vulnerability genes, specific neural circuits, and neuropsychological traits? Although we advocate, with many of our colleagues (14), that a combined category-and-dimension approach makes the most sense for DSM-V and that specific subtypes and dimensions would be useful specifiers, more data are needed. The one exception to this conclusion is compulsive hoarding.
As presented in this issue by Pertusa and his colleagues in their naturalistic study of compulsive hoarding (15) and by Samuels et al. in their recent population-based study of hoarders (16), OCD patients with prominent hoarding symptoms differ from nonhoarding OCD patients across many important respects, including an earlier onset but later presentation for treatment. The pattern of comorbidity is also distinct, with hoarders of all types showing higher levels of social phobia and a greater number of personality disorders. Hoarding symptoms are consistently associated with poor response and premature dropout from cognitive-behavioral therapy for OCD. The picture with regard to pharmacological inventions is less clear-cut. Most reports indicate that hoarding is a strong predictor of poor response. However, in one recent study compulsive hoarders responded as well to paroxetine as nonhoarding OCD patients (17) . However, this study was limited by the fact that the compulsive hoarders took paroxetine for approximately 20–25 days longer than the OCD patients without hoarding. Furthermore, the percentage improvement in the OCD group (about 30% were responders) was lower than would be typically expected. Compulsive hoarders have lower global functioning and more severe family and social disability. Hoarding patients, with or without OCD, are more likely to be living alone and to have significantly lower incomes. As Pertusa et al. show in this issue, hoarding is a highly familial disorder, with approximately 50% of patients reporting a first-degree relative with compulsive hoarding. However, Pertusa et al. also found a high rate (26%) of OCD in the first-degree family members in their group of hoarders without OCD, which would argue for keeping compulsive hoarding closely aligned with OCD in the DSM-V structure.
The most remarkable part of the hoarding story is that there have been a number of reports that demonstrate that compulsive hoarding can newly emerge as a result of brain injury. The greatest lesion overlap in hoarders is in the right medial prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and adjacent white matter (18) . Although we do not fully understand the relationship between brain lesions and patterns of brain activity, both lesion and functional imaging studies have indicated the involvement of specific frontolimbic brain regions in compulsive hoarders (19) . These areas are largely distinct from the circuits often implicated in OCD proper (20) . This difference speaks the most directly to the fact that a separate set of neural circuits may underlie our evolutionarily conserved, and developmentally expressed, tendency to be collectors.
This brings us to the two remaining OCD papers in this issue. The first point of note is that both groups specifically excluded OCD patients with prominent hoarding symptoms as well as patients with a diagnosis of Tourette’s disorder. This increasingly common practice suggests that experts in the field are beginning to consider that patients with a personal or family history of tics and those with prominent hoarding symptoms are fundamentally different from other OCD patients. Both nonhoarding OCD patients and compulsive hoarders may be better served by diagnostically separating these two conditions. Our scientific understanding and treatment of both conditions may be held back by the lumping together of these two conditions that appear phenotypically, genetically, and neurobiologically distinct.
As new imaging technologies emerge, we can be confident that they will contribute to our knowledge of how best to lump and/or split OCD into component parts. Regrettably, the story is far from clear. This month Menzies et al. (21) report a diffusion tensor imaging study that included both adult patients with OCD and an equal number of unaffected first-degree relatives. Remarkably, both the patients and their relatives showed white matter changes: a significant increase in fractional anisotropy in a right medial frontal region and significantly reduced fractional anisotropy in a large region of the right inferior parietal white matter. A similar reduction in fractional anisotropy within an overlapping area within the right inferior parietal region was previously reported by Szeszko et al. in a group of patients with childhood-onset OCD (22) . The existence of similar abnormalities in the unaffected first-degree relatives suggests that these are not compensatory changes but rather potential trait markers that could serve as markers of vulnerability. A good deal of additional work is needed to further replicate these findings and evaluate their potential value as endophenotypes. First, how interrelated are these two white matter findings? Are they separate vulnerabilities or does one predict the other? These questions are important as they will suggest either a single aberrant circuit or possibly two or more, potentially separable circuits. Second, is either finding related to the deficits in inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility, visuospatial ability, nonverbal intelligence, and executive planning that are frequently reported in OCD patients and unaffected family members with OCD (19, 23, 24) ? Given the Cambridge group’s interest in this area, we will likely know the answer to this question shortly. Indeed, their recent report in Science (25), in which they identified reduced activation of several cortical regions, including the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, in both OCD patients and a matched group of unaffected first-degree relatives, during a reversal learning task adds support to this being a potentially valuable endophenotype.
Third, given the fact that the majority of patients had either prominent contamination obsessions and washing compulsions or prominent checking compulsions, it will be fascinating to see if either of these white matter findings is closely related to any OCD symptom dimension. Our a priori hypothesis is that the spatial/attentional circuit proposed by Menzies et al. (20) that involves the parietal regions will be associated with deficits in inhibitory control and related to obsessions of symmetry and a need for things to feel or look “just right” and to compulsive checking, ordering, and arranging. Likewise, the circuit described by Menzies et al. as the “affective” circuit we would expect to be more related to contamination obsessions and washing compulsions as well as to obsessions about harm and the need for related compulsive checking.
Finally, the new report by Szeszko et al. (26), also in this issue, indicates that in addition to white matter changes, some pediatric patients with OCD have structural changes in regions comprising cortical-striatal-thalamic-cortical circuits. Specifically, using voxel-based morphometry, this team of investigators found that, compared to healthy volunteers, pediatric OCD patients had significantly more gray matter bilaterally both in the orbital frontal cortex and in the putamen. Both of these structures have been repeatedly implicated in previous studies (19) . Two great strengths of this study were that all of the patients were drug naive, so the data are free of the potential confound of prior use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, and that the patient’s predominant domain of OCD symptoms is documented. The patients with either predominant contamination/cleaning or aggressive/checking symptoms had significantly more gray matter bilaterally in both the putamen and orbital frontal cortex, suggesting that these structures may contribute to the circuitry that supports these OCD symptom dimensions. In line with this finding, neuroimaging studies have found that OCD patients with predominantly washing symptoms show increased neural responses to washing-related stimuli and to disgusting photographs in specific brain regions, including the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior insula, and putamen/globus pallidus (2729) .
The completion of human genome sequencing provides an extraordinary opportunity to identify the genetic basis of disorders of brain function. Although none of these reports in this issue of the Journal addresses the role of genes in OCD, we await the integration of genetic and neuroimaging research. It is likely that the genetic structure of common disorders differs substantially from current diagnostic classifications based on symptoms (30) . Our prediction is that some genes will be specific to certain subtypes or dimensions, while others will be “generalist” genes that influence the expression of OCD and closely related disorders, possibly including major depression and other internalizing disorders as well as body dysmorphic disorder, trichotillomania, tic disorders, and compulsive hoarding (31) . These generalist genes may exist within modules of coexpressed genes that are functionally related. By using this framework, it will be worthwhile to determine whether overlapping transcriptional networks underlie the expression of the obsessive-compulsive spectrum of normal phenomena as they are expressed within specific evolutionarily conserved neural networks. Then when these networks become dysregulated, for whatever reason, OCD and related disorders emerge as disorders of mind, brain, and behavior.

Footnotes

Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Leckman, Child Study Center, Yale University School of Medicine, 230 South Frontage Rd., New Haven, CT 06520-7900; [email protected] (e-mail). Editorial accepted for publication June 2008 (doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08060891).
The authors report no competing interests.

References

1.
Gesell A, Ilg FL: Infant and Child in the Culture of Today. New York, Harper & Row, 1943
2.
Abramowitz JS, Deacon BJ: The OCD spectrum: a closer look at the arguments and the data, in Concepts and Controversies in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Edited by Abramowitz JS, Houts AC. New York, Springer, 2005, pp 141–149
3.
Bartz JA, Hollander E: Is obsessive-compulsive disorder an anxiety disorder? Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2006; 30:338–352
4.
Evans DW, Leckman JF, Carter A, Reznick JS, Henshaw D, King RA, Pauls D: Ritual, habit, and perfectionism: the prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Dev 1997; 68:58–68
5.
Zohar AH, Felz L: Ritualistic behavior in young children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2001; 29:121–128
6.
Gesell A: Infancy and Human Growth. New York, Macmillan, 1928
7.
Evans DW, Gray FL: Compulsive-like behavior in individuals with Down syndrome: its relation to mental age level, adaptive and maladaptive behavior. Child Dev 2000; 71:288–300
8.
Greaves N, Prince E, Evans DW, Charman T: Repetitive and ritualistic behaviour in children with Prader-Willi syndrome and children with autism. J Intellect Disabil Res 2006; 50:92–100
9.
Leonard HL, Goldberger EL, Rapoport JL, Cheslow DL, Swedo SE: Childhood rituals: normal development or obsessive-compulsive symptoms? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1990; 29:17–23
10.
Evans DW, Leckman JF: Origins of obsessive-compulsive disorder: developmental and evolutionary perspectives, in Developmental Psychopathology, 2nd ed. Edited by Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 2006, pp 404–435
11.
Leckman JF, Carter CS, Hennessy MB, Hrdy SB, Kervene EB, Klann-Delius G, Schradin C, Todt D, von Holst D: Biobehavioral processes in attachment and bonding, in Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis: Dahlem Workshop Report 92. Edited by Carter CS, Ahnert L. Cambridge, Mass, MIT Press, 2005, pp 303–349
12.
Mataix-Cols D, Rosario-Campos MC, Leckman JF: A multidimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:228–238
13.
Bloch MH, Landeros-Weisenberger A, Rosario MC, Pittenger C, Leckman JF: Meta-analysis of the symptom structure of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry (in press)
14.
Mataix-Cols D, Pertusa A, Leckman JF: Issues for DSM-V: how should obsessive-compulsive disorder be classified? (editorial). Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:1313–1314
15.
Pertusa A, Fullana MA, Singh S, Alonso P, Menchón JM, Mataix-Cols D: Compulsive hoarding: OCD symptom, distinct clinical syndrome, or both? Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:1289–1298
16.
Samuels JF, Bienvenu OJ, Grados MA, Cullen B, Riddle MA, Liang KY, Eaton WW, Nestadt G: Prevalence and correlates of hoarding behavior in a community-based sample. Behav Res Ther 2008; 46:836–844
17.
Saxena S, Brody AL, Maidment KM, Baxter LR Jr: Paroxetine treatment of compulsive hoarding. J Psychiatr Res 2007; 41:481–487
18.
Anderson SW, Damasio H, Damasio AR: A neural basis for collecting behaviour in humans. Brain 2005; 128:201–212
19.
An SK, Mataix-Cols D, Lawrence NS, Wooderson S, Giampietro V, Speckens A, Brammer MJ, Phillips ML: To discard or not to discard: the neural basis of hoarding symptoms in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2008; Jan 8 (Epub ahead of print)
20.
Menzies L, Chamberlain SR, Laird AR, Thelen SM, Sahakian BJ, Bullmore ET: Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: the orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008; 32:525–549
21.
Menzies L, Williams GB, Chamberlain SR, Ooi C, Fineberg N, Suckling J, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET: White matter abnormalities in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their first-degree relatives. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:1308–1315
22.
Szeszko PR, Ardekani BA, Ashtari M, Malhotra AK, Robinson DG, Bilder RM, Lim KO: White matter abnormalities in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:782–790
23.
Savage CR, Baer L, Keuthen NJ, Brown HD, Rauch SL, Jenike MA: Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:905–916
24.
Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, Blackwell AD, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ: Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:335–338
25.
Chamberlain SR, Menzies L, Hampshire A, Suckling J, Fineberg NA, del Campo N, Aitken M, Craig K, Owen AM, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ: Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science 2008; 321:421–422
26.
Szeszko PR, Christian C, MacMaster F, Lencz T, Mirza Y, Taormina SP, Easter P, Rose M, Michalopoulou GA, Rosenberg DR: Gray matter structural alterations in psychotropic drug-naive pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder: an optimized voxel-based morphometry study. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:1299–1307
27.
Lawrence NS, An SK, Mataix-Cols D, Ruths F, Speckens A, Phillips ML: Neural responses to facial expressions of disgust but not fear are modulated by washing symptoms in OCD. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:1072–1080
28.
Phillips ML, Marks IM, Senior C, Lythgoe D, O’Dwyer AM, Meehan O, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, Bullmore ET, McGuire PK: A differential neural response in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients with washing compared with checking symptoms to disgust. Psychol Med 2000; 30:1037–1050
29.
Mataix-Cols D, Wooderson S, Lawrence N, Brammer MJ, Speckens A, Phillips ML: Distinct neural correlates of washing, checking, and hoarding symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:564–576
30.
Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC: The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psychiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:929–937
31.
Davis OS, Kovas Y, Harlaar N, Busfield P, McMillan A, Frances J, Petrill SA, Dale PS, Plomin R: Generalist genes and the Internet generation: etiology of learning abilities by web testing at age 10. Genes Brain Behav 2008; 7:455–462

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1229 - 1233
PubMed: 18829875

History

Published online: 1 October 2008
Published in print: October, 2008

Authors

Details

James F. Leckman, M.D.
Michael H. Bloch, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share