Skip to main content
Full access
New Research
Published Online: 1 September 2011

Exaggerated Activation of Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex During Cognitive Interference: A Monozygotic Twin Study of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Abstract

Combat veterans with PTSD showed greater activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate than combat veterans without PTSD, but so did their monozygotic co-twins without combat exposure, suggesting genetic vulnerability to PTSD.

Abstract

Objective:

Neuroimaging studies have revealed functional abnormalities in the anterior cingulate cortex in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The goal of this study was to determine whether hyperresponsivity of the dorsal anterior cingulate in PTSD is an acquired characteristic or a familial risk factor.

Method:

Using a case-control twin design, the authors studied combat-exposed veterans with PTSD (N=12) and their identical combat-unexposed co-twins (N=12), as well as combat-exposed veterans without PTSD (N=14) and their identical combat-unexposed co-twins (N=14). Participants underwent functional MRI during completion of the Multi-Source Interference Task, which reliably activates the dorsal anterior cingulate.

Results:

Combat-exposed veterans with PTSD and their unexposed co-twins had significantly greater activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate and tended to have larger response time difference scores, as compared to combat-exposed veterans without PTSD and their co-twins. Dorsal anterior cingulate activation in the exposed twins was positively correlated with their PTSD symptom severity. Dorsal anterior cingulate activation in the unexposed twins was positively correlated with their combat-exposed co-twins' PTSD symptom severity, but not with depression or alcohol use severity in the combat-exposed co-twins.

Conclusions:

Hyperresponsivity in the dorsal anterior cingulate appears to be a familial risk factor for the development of PTSD following psychological trauma.
Several recent neuroimaging studies have reported functional abnormalities in the anterior cingulate cortex in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The anterior cingulate is a structure in the medial prefrontal cortex that comprises several functional subdivisions (1, 2). Rostral regions of the anterior cingulate activate during emotional states and during tasks that involve interference from emotional stimuli (38). In contrast, dorsal regions of the anterior cingulate (2, 9) typically activate during a wide variety of tasks that involve interference from nonemotional stimuli (5). The dorsal anterior cingulate is thought to play a role in multiple cognitive processes, such as performance monitoring, response selection, error detection, and decision making (5, 6, 10, 11).
In PTSD, rostral portions of the anterior cingulate appear to be hyporesponsive during the presentation of trauma-related and other negative stimuli (1218) and during emotional interference tasks (19, 20). In contrast, dorsal portions appear to show exaggerated responsivity in PTSD during Stroop interference, oddball tasks, fear conditioning, and extinction recall (12, 2025), although not all studies have reported this finding (2629).
Whether functional abnormalities in the anterior cingulate in PTSD are acquired characteristics of the disorder or are familial risk factors for the development of PTSD after trauma is unclear. In order to begin to address this question, we recently used positron emission tomography (PET) and fluorodeoxyglucose (FDG) to study resting regional cerebral metabolic rate for glucose in Vietnam combat veterans with and without PTSD, as well as in their combat-unexposed identical co-twins without PTSD (30). We found that resting metabolic rate in a region that included the dorsal anterior cingulate and midcingulate cortex was elevated in the combat veterans with PTSD and their combat-unexposed co-twins without PTSD, as compared to combat veterans without PTSD and their co-twins. In addition, we found a significant positive correlation between resting metabolic rate in this region in the unexposed twins and their exposed twins' PTSD symptom severity scores. Overall, these findings suggest that resting hypermetabolism in the dorsal anterior cingulate is a familial risk factor for the development of PTSD after psychological trauma.
Interpreting the cognitive correlates of metabolic rate abnormalities in a given structure at rest is challenging because the finding is, by definition, unassociated with a specific cognitive task. To help clarify the role of the dorsal anterior cingulate as a possible risk factor for the development of PTSD, we examined its function using functional MRI (fMRI) and the Multi-Source Interference Task, which yields behavioral indicators of cognitive interference and reliably activates the dorsal anterior cingulate in healthy individuals (9, 31). We studied Vietnam combat veterans with and without PTSD, as well as their combat-unexposed identical co-twins. According to the logic of the design (32), a finding of exaggerated dorsal anterior cingulate activation both in the combat veterans with PTSD and in their identical co-twins would be consistent with a familial risk factor. In contrast, exaggerated dorsal anterior cingulate activation in the combat veterans with PTSD and not in their identical co-twins would be consistent with an acquired characteristic of PTSD. Based on previous PET-FDG findings, we hypothesized that combat veterans with PTSD and their identical co-twins would show greater activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate as compared to veterans without PTSD and their identical co-twins. Given minimal previous evidence of significantly elevated response times or error rates on nonemotional Stroop interference tasks in PTSD (19, 3335), we had no directional a priori hypotheses regarding group differences on these behavioral measures.

Method

Participants

Participants were drawn from a pool of male identical twins who had participated in a previous study (36). Twenty-six pairs participated; 18 of these pairs participated in our previous PET-FDG study (30). Each combat-exposed twin had served in the Vietnam combat theater, whereas his combat-unexposed co-twin had not. Of the exposed twins, 12 had developed current combat-related PTSD, and 14 had never developed PTSD, as determined by the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; 37) using criteria from DSM-IV. Thus, there were four participant groups: combat-exposed veterans who had current combat-related PTSD (N=12) and their combat-unexposed co-twins (N=12), and combat-exposed veterans who never had combat-related PTSD (N=14) and their combat-unexposed co-twins (N=14). The study was approved by the Partners HealthCare System Institutional Review Board. All participants provided written informed consent.

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

Forty-nine participants were right-handed, and three (one combat-exposed with PTSD; two combat-exposed without PTSD) were left-handed. None of the participants reported a history of major head injury involving loss of consciousness for more than 10 minutes, tumor, epilepsy, cerebrovascular accident, or other neurological disorder.
According to the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (38), participants in the combat-exposed group with PTSD met criteria for current comorbid diagnoses: major depression (N=4), dysthymia (N=2), panic disorder (N=3), social phobia (N=1), specific phobia (N=2), and cannabis dependence (N=1). Combat-unexposed co-twins of the PTSD group met criteria for the following current diagnoses: major depression (N=1), bipolar disorder (N=1), specific phobia (N=3), panic disorder (N=1), alcohol dependence (N=1), and civilian PTSD (N=1). Analyses were conducted both with and without the participant with civilian PTSD and his co-twin. Among combat-exposed veterans who never had PTSD, current disorders included dysthymia (N=1) and specific phobia (N=1), and among their combat-unexposed co-twins, two met criteria for current dysthymia.
Eight participants were taking antidepressants at the time of study: three combat-exposed participants with PTSD and one unexposed co-twin, and one combat-exposed participant without PTSD and three unexposed co-twins. Six participants were taking other types of medications: in the combat-exposed group with PTSD, two were taking opioids, one a benzodiazepine, one an antipsychotic, and one a sympatholytic. One co-twin of a combat-exposed participant without PTSD was taking a sympatholytic.
Participants completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; 39), the Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test (MAST; 40), the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (41), and a measure of the severity of combat exposure (42).

Task Procedures

In the Multi-Source Interference Task, participants viewed sets of three numbers or letters (possible numbers were 1, 2, and 3; letters were always x) on a computer screen projected via a tilted mirror. They were told that one number would always be different from the other two items, which would match one another. Participants were asked to report via button-press the identity of the number that was different from the others; on the button-press device, the three buttons represented the numbers 1, 2, and 3, respectively. Participants were also instructed to respond as quickly as possible while minimizing errors and to complete 48 practice trials before beginning the experiment.
The task consisted of two conditions presented in separate alternating blocks. In the control (C) condition, the identity of the target number always matched its position on the button-press device. In the interference (I) condition, the identity of the target number never matched its button-press position (sample stimuli are shown in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article). The task began and ended with 30 seconds of fixation (F), which consisted of a white dot presented in the middle of the screen.
Stimuli were presented with MacStim 3, a stimulus presentation software package, on a Macintosh Powerbook computer. Each stimulus remained on the screen for 1.5 seconds with 0.25 seconds between stimuli. Each block consisted of 24 stimuli. Each run consisted of 10 blocks of alternating conditions in a fixed order (FCICICICIF) and lasted a total of 6 minutes, 36 seconds. Participants completed up to three runs of the Multi-Source Interference Task. Because response time differences between conditions (I – C) and activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate can decline with each additional run in this task, we decided a priori to analyze data from the first run only (9). (Results of the second run are provided in the online data supplement.)

fMRI Procedures

We used a Symphony/Sonata 1.5-T high-speed scanner (Siemens Medical Systems, Iselin, N.J.) with a three-axis gradient head coil. After shimming, high-resolution structural MRI images (three-dimensional magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition with gradient echo; repetition time=2.73 sec, echo time=3.31 msec, flip angle=7°) with a 1.3-mm slice thickness were collected. fMRI blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) images were acquired using a gradient echo T2*-weighted sequence (repetition time=1.5 sec, echo time=40 msec, flip angle=90°) in 16 coronal slices perpendicular to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure line (thickness=5 mm, 1 mm skip).

Data Analysis

Behavioral analyses.

Response times from only correct trials were averaged within each condition and run for each participant. Errors were expressed for each participant as a percentage of the total number of trials on which the participant responded within each condition and run. Difference scores were created by subtracting the average response time in the control condition from the average response time in the interference condition. Difference scores were likewise calculated for error rates.

fMRI analyses.

We conducted two types of analyses on the fMRI data: whole-brain voxelwise comparisons and then analysis of variance (ANOVA) of fMRI data that were extracted from the dorsal anterior cingulate in the voxelwise maps of individual participants. In both types of analyses, we treated exposed versus unexposed co-twins as a repeated measure (i.e., main effect of exposure). In addition, we treated the twin pairs in which the combat-exposed twin had PTSD as a separate group from the twin pairs in which the exposed twin never had PTSD. A significant difference between these two groups (i.e., a significant main effect of PTSD diagnosis) would be consistent with a familial risk factor (as long as there was also no interaction between PTSD diagnosis and exposure). This finding would indicate that the combat-exposed twins with PTSD have the same functional abnormality as their unexposed co-twins without PTSD. A significant PTSD diagnosis-by-exposure interaction that reflected an abnormality in only the exposed twins with PTSD would indicate an acquired sign of PTSD. Lastly, a significant main effect of exposure (i.e., a significant difference between all combat-exposed twins as compared to all combat-unexposed twins collapsing across PTSD diagnosis) in the absence of an interaction would suggest that the functional abnormality is associated with exposure to combat and not with PTSD.

Voxelwise analyses.

Statistical parametric mapping analysis of the imaging data was conducted using the SPM2 software package (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm2). Each participant's functional images were motion corrected and coregistered to his high-resolution structural MRI image. The resulting images were spatially normalized in a standard stereotactic space (Montreal Neurological Institute, MNI) and then smoothed (8 mm full width at half maximum). At each voxel, the BOLD data were fitted to a linear statistical model by the method of least squares. Hypotheses were tested as contrasts in which linear compounds of the model parameters were evaluated using t statistics, which were then transformed to z scores.
We used an approach that consisted of two hierarchical levels of analysis, in which the second level's random-effects analysis absorbed the random effects from the first level. First, interference-versus-control contrast images were generated for each participant. For the purpose of examining the main effect of PTSD diagnosis, the contrast images of the combat-exposed and combat-unexposed participants were averaged (first level), and then the PTSD and non-PTSD pairs were compared (second level). For the purpose of examining the PTSD diagnosis-by-exposure interaction, the contrast images of the combat-exposed and combat-unexposed participants were contrasted (first level), and then the PTSD and non-PTSD pairs were compared (second level). Then, for the purpose of examining the main effect of combat exposure, the contrast images of the combat-exposed and combat-unexposed participants were compared in a two-group t test.
The statistical parametric maps resulting from the above analyses were inspected for main effects and their interaction in the dorsal anterior cingulate, which was defined as the portion of the anterior cingulate that is superior to the corpus callosum, between y=0 and y=+30 mm (43). Given our strong a priori hypotheses, we applied a significance threshold of 0.001, one-tailed and uncorrected (z score ≥3.09), to activations in the dorsal anterior cingulate. For regions about which we had no a priori prediction, we applied a more conservative constant significance threshold of 0.00002, two-tailed and uncorrected (z score ≥4.27) (29, 30).

Region-of-interest analyses.

We extracted fMRI data from the dorsal anterior cingulate activation at the maximum voxel within each participant, using previously established methods (9). (Data could not be extracted for four participants [two combat-exposed twins with PTSD, one combat-exposed twin without PTSD, and one of their co-twins] because the maximum voxel value of their interference-versus-control activations did not fall within the dorsal anterior cingulate proper.) We then further analyzed these data for the main effects of PTSD diagnosis and combat exposure and their interaction using a mixed model that treated combat exposure as a within-pairs repeated measure, PTSD diagnosis as a between-pairs measure, and twin pairs as a random effect (44). We also performed correlational analyses with the extracted data in order to determine whether dorsal anterior cingulate activation in all combat-exposed twins was correlated with their own CAPS scores, as well as whether dorsal anterior cingulate activation in the combat-unexposed twins was correlated with their combat-exposed twins' CAPS scores and other clinical measures. All p values are two-tailed except as otherwise indicated.

Results

Table 1 summarizes the demographic and clinical characteristics of the study participants.
TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Combat-Exposed Vietnam Veterans With and Without PTSD and Their Combat-Unexposed Identical Co-Twins
 PTSD PairsaNon-PTSD PairsbMixed-Model Analysis of Variancec
 Exposed (N=12)Unexposed (N=12)Exposed (N=14)Unexposed (N=14)DiagnosisCombat ExposureInteraction
MeasureMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDFpFpFp
Age (years)55.02.955.02.956.42.256.42.22.10.160    
Education (years)12.83.113.33.113.61.713.62.00.4ns0.5ns0.2ns
Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test8.64.48.34.96.02.14.81.84.5d0.0511.0dns0.4dns
Combat severity, 18-item measure8.71.5  3.62.7  31.2e<0.001    
Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale61.722.3  7.610.2  66.3<0.001    
Beck Depression Inventory11.59.04.55.53.14.63.54.63.4f0.0806.6f0.0208.3f0.011
Childhood Trauma Questionnaire60.48.759.39.963.46.262.23.61.4ens0.56ens0.003ens
Birth weight (oz)89.514.097.010.088.524.482.823.60.85gns0.05gns2.4gns
a
Presence of current combat-related PTSD in the combat-exposed twin.
b
Absence of current or past combat-related PTSD in the combat-exposed twin.
c
df=1, 24 except as otherwise noted.
d
df=1, 15.
e
df=1, 23.
f
df=1, 16.
g
df=1, 18.

Behavioral Results

Response time and error rate difference scores (I–C) were submitted to separate 2×2 (PTSD diagnosis by exposure) repeated-measures ANOVAs. Regarding response time difference scores, a main effect of PTSD diagnosis was detected but fell short of statistical significance (F=3.61, df=1, 24, p=0.07). The combat-exposed participants with PTSD and their co-twins tended to have larger response time difference scores than the combat-exposed participants without PTSD and their co-twins (Table 2). No other effect was significant. When we excluded from the analysis the PTSD twin pair in which the unexposed co-twin had civilian PTSD, the main effect of PTSD diagnosis became significant (F=5.30, df=1, 23, p=0.03).
TABLE 2. Response Time and Error Rate Data on the Multi-Source Interference Task Among Combat-Exposed Vietnam Veterans With and Without PTSD and Their Combat-Unexposed Identical Co-Twins
 PTSD PairsaNon-PTSD Pairsb
 Exposed (N=12)Unexposed (N=12)Exposed (N=14)Unexposed (N=14)
MeasureMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Mean response times (msec) 
    Interference1,0661171,06413698395998100
    Control6618466510363311466998
    Difference score405129399933507032981
Mean error rates (%) 
    Interference6.04.55.76.05.75.64.34.1
    Control0.50.91.12.01.01.70.30.9
    Difference score5.54.74.65.54.74.44.04.3
a
Presence of current combat-related PTSD in the combat-exposed twin.
b
Absence of current or past combat-related PTSD in the combat-exposed twin.
Regarding error rate difference scores, no effects were significant, even when the PTSD pair in which the unexposed co-twin had civilian PTSD was excluded from the analyses.

fMRI Results

Voxelwise analyses.

There was a significant main effect of PTSD diagnosis in the dorsal anterior cingulate (MNI coordinates, x=+10, y=+6, z=+46; z score=3.17; k at p<0.001=1). Combat-exposed veterans with PTSD and their co-twins exhibited greater BOLD signal changes in the interference-versus-control contrast than combat-exposed veterans without PTSD and their co-twins. When we ran a small-volume correction based on the volume of the right dorsal anterior cingulate in this sample, the false discovery rate p value for the main effect of PTSD diagnosis was 0.05. The small-volume correction based on the volume of the left and right dorsal anterior cingulate yielded a false discovery rate p value of 0.08. Contrasts between the subgroups revealed a similar pattern (Table 3).
TABLE 3. Results of Voxelwise fMRI Analyses of Data From the Multi-Source Interference Task in Combat-Exposed Vietnam Veterans With and Without PTSD and Their Combat-Unexposed Identical Co-Twinsa
ComparisonRegionz ScoreCoordinatesb (x, y, z)
Main effect of diagnosis   
PTSD pairs > non-PTSD pairsDorsal anterior cingulate3.1710, 6, 46
  2.79–2, 2, 40
Non-PTSD pairs > PTSD pairsNone  
Combat-exposed twins   
PTSD > non-PTSDDorsal anterior cingulate2.7110, 6, 42
Non-PTSD > PTSDNone  
Combat-unexposed twins   
PTSD > non-PTSDDorsal anterior cingulate3.222, 2, 40
Non-PTSD > PTSDNone  
a
Interference versus control contrast images were used in these analyses.
b
Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates.
No regions exhibited significantly lower BOLD signal changes in the PTSD twin pairs relative to the non-PTSD twin pairs. No brain regions met significance thresholds for a main effect of exposure or a diagnosis-by-exposure interaction.

Region-of-interest analyses.

Data from the dorsal anterior cingulate activation in individual participants were extracted and further analyzed. The main effect of PTSD diagnosis was significant (F=8.9, df=1, 20, p=0.007; Figure 1) and remained significant after exclusion of the PTSD pair in which the combat-unexposed co-twin had civilian PTSD (F=7.9, df=1, 19, p=0.01).
FIGURE 1. fMRI Data Extracted From the Dorsal Anterior Cingulate of Individual Participants During the Multi-Source Interference Task in Combat-Exposed Vietnam Veterans With and Without PTSD and Their Combat-Unexposed Identical Co-Twinsa
a In the PTSD twin pairs, the combat-exposed twin had current combat-related PTSD; in the non-PTSD pairs, the combat-exposed twin never had PTSD. These data show a main effect of PTSD diagnosis (F=8.9, df=1, 20, p=0.007), consistent with the whole-brain voxelwise analyses. Error bars indicate standard error of the mean.
The following covariates were tested as potential confounders of the main effect of PTSD diagnosis by examining their association with the dependent measure using a screening threshold of p<0.20: birth weight, age, total score on the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire, years of education, BDI score, MAST score, diagnosis of major depression, use of psychiatric medication, left-handedness, and severity of combat exposure. Only age met this threshold. Adjusted for age, the main effect of PTSD diagnosis was significant (F=4.9, df=1, 19, p=0.04).
BOLD signal changes in the dorsal anterior cingulate in the combat-exposed twins were positively correlated with their own current CAPS scores (r=0.37, N=21, p=0.043, one-tailed; see the online data supplement). BOLD signal changes in the dorsal anterior cingulate in the combat-unexposed twins were positively correlated with their combat-exposed twins' current CAPS scores (r=0.34, N=23, p=0.05, one-tailed; see the online data supplement) and combat exposure scores (r=0.38, N=22, p=0.03, one-tailed). However, these correlations became nonsignificant when a potential outlier was removed. BOLD signal changes in the dorsal anterior cingulate in the combat-unexposed twins were not significantly correlated with their combat-exposed twins' BDI scores or response time difference scores. In a post hoc analysis, response time difference scores in the combat-unexposed twins were not significantly correlated with their combat-exposed twins' CAPS scores.

Discussion

Vietnam combat veterans with PTSD and their identical co-twins showed greater fMRI activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate during a nonemotional interference task as compared to Vietnam combat veterans without PTSD and their identical co-twins. In addition, activation of the dorsal anterior cingulate in the combat-exposed twins was positively correlated with their own CAPS scores. Furthermore, dorsal anterior cingulate activation in the combat-unexposed twins was positively correlated with their combat-exposed twins' CAPS and combat exposure scores but not with other clinical measures. These results suggest that dorsal anterior cingulate hyperresponsivity is a familial risk factor for PTSD rather than an acquired characteristic of PTSD.
The finding of exaggerated activation in the dorsal anterior cingulate in PTSD is consistent with some previous findings in PTSD singletons (12, 2022, 24, 25), but not all (2629). Two previous studies found exaggerated dorsal anterior cingulate activation during interference tasks in combat veterans with PTSD (20, 25). In those studies, the exaggerated activation in PTSD was not accompanied by a significant behavioral effect—that is, the response time difference scores were not significantly greater in PTSD than in the trauma-exposed comparison group. This could be attributable to the small sample sizes in those studies. In the present study, with a larger sample size, the behavioral effect approached statistical significance (p=0.07) with all participants included and became significant (p=0.03) when we excluded from the analysis a PTSD pair in which the combat-unexposed co-twin had civilian PTSD. Interestingly, although dorsal anterior cingulate activation in the combat-unexposed twins was correlated with their combat-exposed co-twins' CAPS scores, response time difference scores in the combat-unexposed twins were not. This suggests that dorsal anterior cingulate activation might be a more sensitive measure of vulnerability than behavioral measures of interference.
Our findings in this study are consistent with those of our previous PET-FDG twin study (which included 18 of the same pairs studied here) (30). Thus, PTSD twin pairs exhibited increased metabolic rates in the dorsal anterior cingulate at rest, as well as greater activation during an interference task involving nonemotional information. Moreover, the locations of these two findings are similar (MNI coordinates in the PET-FDG study: x=+10, y=+2, z=+42; in the present fMRI study: x=+10, y=+6, z=+46). Although these findings await replication in a new sample, it appears that the functional abnormality in the dorsal anterior cingulate in PTSD can be elicited using either technique. With fMRI, radioisotopes are not required, and the implementation of a specific cognitive task helps to pinpoint the cognitive correlates of the functional abnormality.
Exaggerated dorsal anterior cingulate activation in the PTSD group and their identical co-twins could reflect increased cognitive interference and/or response selection, an interpretation that is consistent with the trend for greater response time differences in those groups. This exaggerated activation is not likely attributable to error monitoring as all groups had similar error rate difference scores. Because the task was presented in blocks, we were unable to remove fMRI data from individual trials on which errors occurred. Exaggerated dorsal anterior cingulate activation could also reflect autonomic arousal or its regulation during task performance (22, 45, 46), although we do not have psychophysiologic measures to support this possibility. However, previous behavioral studies have found that individuals with PTSD are not more physiologically responsive during Stroop interference tasks or other cognitive stressors as compared to control groups (19, 33, 47). Finally, given that the dorsal anterior cingulate is activated during the expression of conditioned fear responses in healthy humans (46) and is hyperresponsive during extinction recall in PTSD (23), the exaggerated activation observed in PTSD in the present study may reflect a general hyperresponsivity of this brain region that could be related to the exaggerated fear responses observed in PTSD. The positive correlation observed between dorsal anterior cingulate activation and PTSD symptom severity is consistent with this speculation.
All participants in this study were male, which may limit the generalizability of the findings. Other limitations include the presence of disorders other than PTSD and medication use in some of the twins. Another possible limitation is the presence of civilian PTSD in one co-twin. However, civilian PTSD in this co-twin did not drive the main effect of diagnosis because the hyperactivation remained even when this twin pair was excluded from the analysis. Given that we did not include combat-exposed individuals who had past or partial PTSD, our findings may not generalize to those conditions. Future studies might implement event-related designs and psychophysiologic monitoring to further clarify the role of dorsal anterior cingulate function as a potential familial risk factor for the development of PTSD following psychological trauma. In addition, future studies should seek to determine whether hyperresponsivity of the dorsal anterior cingulate during interference tasks in PTSD is associated with specific genotypes, such as the presence of a short allele of the serotonin transporter polymorphism, which has been associated with an increased risk of PTSD after exposure to traumatic events (48).

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Mary Foley and Lawrence White for technical assistance.

Footnote

Received Dec. 23, 2009; revisions received Jan. 13 and April 14, 2011; accepted April 18, 2011.

Supplementary Material

File (ajp_168_09_979_01.pdf)

References

1.
Vogt BA, Finch DM, Olson CR: Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: the anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cereb Cortex 1992; 2:435–443
2.
Vogt BA, Berger GR, Derbyshire SW: Structural and functional dichotomy of human midcingulate cortex. Eur J Neurosci 2003; 18:3134–3144
3.
Bishop S, Duncan J, Brett M, Lawrence AD: Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: controlling attention to threat-related stimuli. Nat Neurosci 2004; 7:184–188
4.
Bush G, Whalen PJ, Rosen BR, Jenike MA, McInerney SC, Rauch SL: The counting Stroop: an interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging: validation study with functional MRI. Hum Brain Mapp 1998; 6:270–282
5.
Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI: Cognitive and emotional influence in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2000; 4:215–222
6.
Mohanty A, Engels AS, Herrington JD, Heller W, Ho MH, Banich MT, Webb AG, Warren SL, Miller GA: Differential engagement of anterior cingulate cortex subdivisions for cognitive and emotional function. Psychophysiology 2007; 44:343–351
7.
Phan KL, Wager T, Taylor SF, Liberzon I: Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: a meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI. Neuroimage 2002; 16:331–348
8.
Whalen PJ, Bush G, McNally RJ, Wilhelm S, McInerney SC, Jenike MA, Rauch SL: The emotional counting Stroop paradigm: a functional magnetic resonance imaging probe of the anterior cingulate affective division. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:1219–1228
9.
Bush G, Spencer TJ, Holmes J, Shin LM, Valera EM, Seidman LJ, Makris N, Surman C, Aleardi M, Mick E, Biederman J: Functional magnetic resonance imaging of methylphenidate and placebo in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder during the Multi-Source Interference Task. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65:102–114
10.
Botvinick MM: Conflict monitoring and decision making: reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2007; 7:356–366
11.
Carter CS, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD: The contribution of the anterior cingulate cortex to executive processes in cognition. Rev Neurosci 1999; 10:49–57
12.
Bremner JD, Vermetten E, Schmahl C, Vaccarino V, Vythilingam M, Afzal N, Grillon C, Charney DS: Positron emission tomographic imaging of neural correlates of a fear acquisition and extinction paradigm in women with childhood sexual-abuse-related post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med 2005; 35:791–806
13.
Britton JC, Phan KL, Taylor SF, Fig LM, Liberzon I: Corticolimbic blood flow in posttraumatic stress disorder during script-driven imagery. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:832–840
14.
Lanius RA, Williamson PC, Densmore M, Boksman K, Gupta MA, Neufeld RW, Gati JS, Menon RS: Neural correlates of traumatic memories in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI investigation. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1920–1922
15.
Phan KL, Britton JC, Taylor SF, Fig LM, Liberzon I: Corticolimbic blood flow during nontraumatic emotional processing in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:184–192
16.
Shin LM, McNally RJ, Kosslyn SM, Thompson WL, Rauch SL, Alpert NM, Metzger LJ, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK: Regional cerebral blood flow during script-driven imagery in childhood sexual abuse-related PTSD: a PET investigation. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:575–584
17.
Shin LM, Wright CI, Cannistraro PA, Wedig MM, McMullin K, Martis B, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Cavanagh SR, Krangel TS, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Whalen PJ, Rauch SL: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:273–281
18.
Williams LM, Kemp AH, Felmingham K, Barton M, Olivieri G, Peduto A, Gordon E, Bryant RA: Trauma modulates amygdala and medial prefrontal responses to consciously attended fear. Neuroimage 2006; 29:347–357
19.
Bremner JD, Vermetten E, Vythilingam M, Afzal N, Schmahl C, Elzinga B, Charney DS: Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional Stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 55:612–620
20.
Shin LM, Whalen PJ, Pitman RK, Bush G, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Orr SP, McInerney SC, Rauch SL: An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 50:932–942
21.
Bryant RA, Felmingham KL, Kemp AH, Barton M, Peduto AS, Rennie C, Gordon E, Williams LM: Neural networks of information processing in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58:111–118
22.
Felmingham KL, Williams LM, Kemp AH, Rennie C, Gordon E, Bryant RA: Anterior cingulate activity to salient stimuli is modulated by autonomic arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res 2009; 173:59–62
23.
Milad MR, Pitman RK, Ellis CB, Gold AL, Shin LM, Lasko NB, Zeidan MA, Handwerger K, Orr SP, Rauch SL: Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 66:1075–1082
24.
Pannu Hayes J, Labar KS, Petty CM, McCarthy G, Morey RA: Alterations in the neural circuitry for emotion and attention associated with posttraumatic stress symptomatology. Psychiatry Res 2009; 172:7–15
25.
Shin LM, Bush G, Whalen PJ, Handwerger K, Cannistraro PA, Wright CI, Martis B, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Orr SP, Pitman RK, Rauch SL: Dorsal anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 2007; 20:701–712
26.
Falconer E, Bryant R, Felmingham KL, Kemp AH, Gordon E, Peduto A, Olivieri G, Williams LM: The neural networks of inhibitory control in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 2008; 33:413–422
27.
Lanius RA, Williamson PC, Hopper J, Densmore M, Boksman K, Gupta MA, Neufeld RW, Gati JS, Menon RS: Recall of emotional states in posttraumatic stress disorder: an fMRI investigation. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53:204–210
28.
Moores KA, Clark CR, McFarlane AC, Brown GC, Puce A, Taylor DJ: Abnormal recruitment of working memory updating networks during maintenance of trauma-neutral information in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res 2008; 163:156–170
29.
Shin LM, Orr SP, Carson MA, Rauch SL, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, Marzol Peters P, Metzger L, Dougherty DD, Cannistraro PA, Alpert NM, Fischman AJ, Pitman RK: Regional cerebral blood flow in amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:168–176
30.
Shin LM, Lasko NB, Macklin ML, Karpf RD, Milad MR, Orr SP, Goetz JM, Fischman AJ, Rauch SL, Pitman RK: Resting metabolic activity in the cingulate cortex and vulnerability to posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:1099–1107
31.
Bush G, Shin LM, Holmes J, Rosen BR, Vogt BA: The Multi-Source Interference Task: validation study with fMRI in individual subjects. Mol Psychiatry 2003; 8:60–70
32.
Pitman RK, Gilbertson MW, Gurvits TV, May FS, Lasko NB, Metzger LJ, Shenton ME, Yehuda R, Orr SP: Clarifying the origin of biological abnormalities in PTSD through the study of identical twins discordant for combat exposure. Ann NY Acad Sci 2006; 1071:242–254
33.
Litz BT, Weathers FW, Monaco V, Herman DS, Wulfsohn M, Marx B, Keane TM: Attention, arousal, and memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 1996; 9:497–519
34.
Stein MB, Kennedy CM, Twamley EW: Neuropsychological function in female victims of intimate partner violence with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52:1079–1088
35.
Vasterling JJ, Duke LM, Brailey K, Constans JI, Allain AN, Sutker PB: Attention, learning, and memory performances and intellectual resources in Vietnam veterans: PTSD and no disorder comparisons. Neuropsychology 2002; 16:5–14
36.
Orr SP, Metzger LJ, Lasko NB, Macklin ML, Hu FB, Shalev AY, Pitman RK: Physiologic responses to sudden, loud tones in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: association with posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:283–288
37.
Weathers FW, Keane TM, Davidson JR: Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale: a review of the first ten years of research. Depress Anxiety 2001; 13:132–156
38.
Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1995
39.
Beck AT, Steer RA: Manual for the Revised Beck Depression Inventory. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1987
40.
Selzer ML: The Michigan Alcoholism Screening Test: the quest for a new diagnostic instrument. Am J Psychiatry 1971; 127:1653–1658
41.
Bernstein DP, Fink L, Handelsman L, Foote J, Lovejoy M, Wenzel K, Sapareto E, Ruggiero J: Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1132–1136
42.
Janes GR, Goldberg J, Eisen SA, True WR: Reliability and validity of a combat exposure index for Vietnam era veterans. J Clin Psychol 1991; 47:80–86
43.
Bush G, Vogt BA, Holmes J, Dale AM, Greve D, Jenike MA, Rosen BR: Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99:523–528
44.
Littlell RC, Milliken GA, Stroup WW, Wolfinger RD: SAS System for Mixed Models. Cary, NC, SAS Institute, 1996
45.
Critchley HD, Mathias CJ, Josephs O, O'Doherty J, Zanini S, Dewar BK, Cipolotti L, Shallice T, Dolan RJ: Human cingulate cortex and autonomic control: converging neuroimaging and clinical evidence. Brain 2003; 126:2139–2152
46.
Milad MR, Quirk GJ, Pitman RK, Orr SP, Fischl B, Rauch SL: A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:1191–1194
47.
Orr SP, Meyerhoff JL, Edwards JV, Pitman RK: Heart rate and blood pressure resting levels and responses to generic stressors in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress 1998; 11:155–164
48.
Xie P, Kranzler HR, Poling J, Stein MB, Anton RF, Brady K, Weiss RD, Farrer L, Gelernter J: Interactive effect of stressful life events and the serotonin transporter 5-HTTLPR genotype on posttraumatic stress disorder diagnosis in 2 independent populations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:1201–1209

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 979 - 985
PubMed: 21724666

History

Received: 23 December 2009
Revision received: 13 January 2011
Revision received: 14 April 2011
Accepted: 18 April 2011
Published online: 1 September 2011
Published in print: September 2011

Authors

Affiliations

Lisa M. Shin, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
George Bush, M.D.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Mohammed R. Milad, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Natasha B. Lasko, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Kathryn Handwerger Brohawn, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Katherine C. Hughes, B.A.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Michael L. Macklin, B.A.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Andrea L. Gold, M.S.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Rachel D. Karpf, B.A.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Scott P. Orr, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Scott L. Rauch, M.D.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.
Roger K. Pitman, M.D.
From the Department of Psychology, Tufts University, Medford, Mass.; the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; VA Research Service, Manchester, N.H.; the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.; and the Department of Psychiatry, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Mass.

Notes

Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Shin ([email protected]).

Funding Information

Dr. Bush has received research support from the Benson-Henry Institute for Mind-Body Medicine, the Center for Functional Neuroimaging Technologies (P41RR14075), the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, the David Judah Fund, Eli Lilly, the Johnson & Johnson Center for the Study of Psychopathology, the McIngvale Fund, McNeil, the Mental Illness and Neuroscience Discovery (MIND) Institute, NARSAD, the National Science Foundation, NIMH, and Pfizer; he has also served as a consultant or speaker for Eli Lilly, Janssen, Johnson & Johnson, McNeil, Novartis, and Shire and received an honorarium for serving as a judge for an Intel Corporation science competition. All other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.Supported by U.S. Public Health Service grant R01MH54636 to Dr. Pitman. The Department of Veterans Affairs provided financial support for the development and maintenance of the Vietnam Era Twin (VET) Registry, and the VET Registry is also supported by the Department of Defense; the National Personnel Records Center, National Archives and Records Administration; the Internal Revenue Service; NIH; the National Opinion Research Center; the National Research Council, National Academy of Sciences; and the Institute for Survey Research, Temple University.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share