Skip to main content
Full access
Editorials
Published Online: 1 September 2016

Can Positive Parental Reinforcement Counter Genetic Risk for Callous-Unemotional Behavior?

Conduct problems are a common reason for a childhood referral to mental health and educational services and represent a substantial public health cost (1). Callous-unemotional behaviors, indicative of lack of empathy and remorse, characterize children who are at risk of developing persistent conduct problems (2). Previous research indicates that callous-unemotional behaviors are moderately to highly heritable (3) and that conduct problems that co-occur with high levels of callous-unemotional behaviors may be more heritable than conduct problems that do not co-occur with these behaviors (4). In other words, children with conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors may be genetically vulnerable to developing persistent antisocial behavior. However, for complex behaviors genetic vulnerability does not equal destiny for developing a particular outcome; there are no genes that directly code for callous-unemotional behavior. Genes code for proteins that influence characteristics such as neurocognitive vulnerabilities that may in turn increase risk for developing callous-unemotional behaviors and conduct problems. Although an individual’s genome likely limits a “range for phenotypic expression” (so-called reaction norm), it does not prespecify how an individual will turn out. The specific developmental trajectory of any individual is determined by a complex interplay between genetic propensities and other factors that constrain how those genetic propensities are expressed at different levels of analysis, and throughout different developmental stages. Genetic variants that are associated with callous-unemotional behaviors (once they are reliably ascertained) are likely to confer advantages as well as disadvantages, depending on the environmental context. The challenge for researchers and practitioners alike is to uncover the mechanisms through which individuals with different genetic and environmental vulnerabilities arrive at maladaptive or better-adjusted outcomes. Parenting may represent one of these mechanisms.
Harsh and negative parenting has been associated with higher levels of callous-unemotional behaviors, whereas a warm parental style has been associated with lower levels of callous-unemotional behaviors in children (5). But it is not self-evident that such parenting correlates of callous-unemotional behaviors reflect purely environmental causal influences of parenting on behavioral development. Parents with genetic risk factors for antisocial behavior are likely to display parenting behaviors in line with these risks (e.g., harsh parenting) and also pass these genetic risk factors, which are likely to influence conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors, to their offspring. This means that part of the association between less than optimal parenting strategies and conduct problems/callous-unemotional behaviors may represent a genetic confounder (passive gene-environment correlation), which has been demonstrated for antisocial behavior (6). We also know that children with conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors are extremely challenging to parent. They typically show diminished empathy for others, display less remorse, manipulate others, and do not seem to want to please adults or readily show affection to others. It is therefore likely that the parenting reactions they evoke are different from those that less challenging children evoke, and recent research suggests that this is the case (7) (evocative gene-environment correlation).
To date, only two genetically informative longitudinal studies have investigated parenting and development of callous-unemotional behaviors (8, 9). Results from the first of these studies, capitalizing on a monozygotic twin differences design, suggest that the association between harsh and negative parenting and higher levels of callous-unemotional behaviors in children may, at least in part, reflect genetic vulnerability within families (8). This could reflect either a shared genetic vulnerability for poor parenting and callous-unemotional behaviors or an effect of callous-unemotional behaviors in evoking negative or harsh parenting.
Complementing and extending this work, a highly informative and exciting adoption study by Hyde and colleagues in this issue of the Journal (9) demonstrates both the impact of biological risk for conduct problems and callous-unemotional behaviors and a clear indication that protective environmental factors are able to moderate the expression of that risk. In the Hyde et al. study, a total of 561 adopted children and their adoptive and biological families from the Early Growth and Development Study were assessed longitudinally. Adopted children were assessed on callous-unemotional, oppositional, and attention-deficit behaviors at age 27 months. Severe antisocial behavior was assessed in biological mothers as an index of biological risk, which may reflect genetic risk totally or partially. Adoptive mothers’ positive reinforcement was assessed when the child was 18 months old. The main findings from longitudinal structural equation modeling showed that 1) severe antisocial behavior in the biological mother predicted callous-unemotional behaviors in the child (β=0.16, p<0.01) but not attention-deficit and oppositional behaviors; 2) positive reinforcement by the adoptive mother exerted a protective influence on callous-unemotional (β=−0.19, p<0.01) and oppositional behaviors (β=−0.15, p<0.01); and 3) severe antisocial behavior in the biological mother did not predict callous-unemotional behaviors if the adoptive mother engaged in a high degree of positive reinforcement toward the child (β=0.01, p>0.90). These findings are extremely encouraging, as the biological risk for early callous-unemotional behaviors appeared to be completely buffered by positive reinforcement from the adoptive mother.
Despite the excitement that these findings should naturally generate, key challenges regarding their potential for translation should be addressed. First, the long-term protective benefits of positive reinforcement on the development of callous-unemotional behavior in vulnerable children needs confirmation. Recent studies have highlighted the importance of genetic effects on long-term developmental trajectories of conduct problems, whereas environmental influences tend to be short-term (10). Therefore, the observed protective benefits of environmental influences, including positive reinforcement in toddlerhood, may not be maintained throughout childhood, and other developmentally specific genetic and environmental factors may take over. These include genetic factors pertaining to the maturation of those brain areas involved in planning, impulse control, and complex social interactions, as well as developmentally specific environmental risk factors, such as peer relationships and neighborhood factors.
Furthermore, as Hyde et al. point out, it is important to bear in mind that parents in adoptive families are typically highly motivated to undertake the challenges of parenting and are also often well resourced. By contrast, in biological families, parents of children with callous-unemotional behaviors are likely to have a host of genetic and contextual risk factors, which can pose challenges for promoting interventions that seek to increase positive reinforcement behaviors toward the child—particularly if the child is challenging. Therefore, the efficacy of such interventions in biological families, as well as the size and the duration of any beneficial impact on callous-unemotional behaviors and conduct problems, still must be established.
In sum, Hyde et al. (9) have made an important contribution to our understanding of how biological and environmental risk interact in shaping the early development of callous-unemotional behaviors. Follow-ups in the Early Growth and Development Study and other genetically informative studies will hopefully shed further light on the long-term significance of these findings and bring us closer to a causal understanding of risk and protective pathways to callous-unemotional behavior and conduct problems across different development periods.

Acknowledgments

Dr. Viding is a Royal Society Wolfson Research Merit Award holder.

References

1.
Scott S, Knapp M, Henderson J, et al: Financial cost of social exclusion: follow up study of antisocial children into adulthood. BMJ 2001; 323:191
2.
Fontaine NMG, McCrory EJP, Boivin M, et al: Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol 2011; 120:730–742
3.
Viding E, McCrory EJ: Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the development of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol 2012; 24:969–983
4.
Viding E, Blair RJR, Moffitt TE, et al: Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005; 46:592–597
5.
Waller R, Gardner F, Hyde LW: What are the associations between parenting, callous-unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clin Psychol Rev 2013; 33:593–608
6.
Moffitt TE: The new look of behavioral genetics in developmental psychopathology: gene-environment interplay in antisocial behaviors. Psychol Bull 2005; 131:533–554
7.
Hawes DJ, Dadds MR, Frost AD, et al: Do childhood callous-unemotional traits drive change in parenting practices? J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2011; 40:507–518
8.
Viding E, Fontaine NMG, Oliver BR, et al: Negative parental discipline, conduct problems, and callous-unemotional traits: monozygotic twin differences study. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195:414–419
9.
Hyde LW, Waller R, Trentacosta CJ, et al: Heritable and nonheritable pathways to early callous-unemotional behaviors. Am J Psychiatry 2016; 173:903–910
10.
Pingault J-B, Rijsdijk F, Zheng Y, et al: Developmentally dynamic genome: evidence of genetic influences on increases and decreases in conduct problems from early childhood to adolescence. Sci Rep 2015; 5:10053

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 862 - 863
PubMed: 27581693

History

Accepted: June 2016
Published online: 1 September 2016
Published in print: September 01, 2016

Keywords

  1. Administration
  2. Other Psychological Issues

Authors

Details

Essi Viding, Ph.D.
From the Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London.
Jean-Baptiste Pingault, Ph.D.
From the Division of Psychology and Language Sciences, University College London.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Viding ([email protected]).

Funding Information

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share