Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective:

The purpose of this study was to determine whether, controlling for genetic effects, drug abuse was transmitted within families as predicted by a contagion model.

Methods:

The authors examined 65,006 parent-offspring, sibling, and cousin pairs ascertained from Swedish population registries in which the primary case subject had a drug abuse registration. The rate of drug abuse registration among at-risk secondary case subjects ages 19–23 was studied. Utilizing matched control pairs, a difference-in-difference approach was used to infer causal effects.

Results:

In offspring, risk for drug abuse registration in the 3 years after an index registration of a parent residing in the same household, neighborhood, or municipality increased 5.9%, 3.4%, and 1.8%, respectively. For siblings of sibling index case subjects, parallel results were 5.9%, 3.9%, and 1.2%. For cousins of cousin index case subjects, excess risk for those in the same neighborhood or municipality was 2.9% and 0.9%, respectively. In all sets of relatives, drug abuse transmission was strongest in male-male pairs and in pairs closest in age. In sibling pairs, stronger transmission was observed in older to younger siblings compared with younger to older siblings. Transmission was stronger within than across the two drug classes with sufficient data (opiates and cannabis).

Conclusions:

These results suggest that drug abuse can be transmitted within families by an environmentally mediated temporally defined model of contagion. The most important methodological limitation is that drug abuse registration is an inaccurate measure of the onset of drug abuse. Indeed, as predicted, drug abuse risk increased among potential secondary case subjects in the year before drug abuse registration of the index case subject.
Drug abuse is strongly transmitted within families (1) by genetic and familial-environmental influences (27). Although substantial efforts are under way to identify molecular genetic risk for drug abuse (812), less attention is being given to familial-environmental transmission.
Within siblings, shared exposures to community risks such as peer deviance (1315) and psychosocial deprivation (16), inadequate parenting (17), and family stressors (2, 18) likely contribute to the familial aggregation of drug abuse. In this study, we explored another mechanism for familial-environmental effects on drug abuse risk: environmental transmission or “contagion.” Although models of contagion for drug abuse have been applied to community settings (19, 20), they have rarely been tested in families.
Utilizing the precise timing of drug abuse registration from national Swedish registries, we examined contagion models for drug abuse within relative pairs. First, we examined whether risk for drug abuse among offspring increased after registration for parental drug abuse, and then we applied the same method to full siblings and cousins. Next, we explored whether the magnitude of transmission differed in relative pairs as a function of geographic proximity, age difference, sex, older-younger compared with younger-older pairs, and within classes compared with between classes of psychoactive drugs.

Methods

Several Swedish population-based national registers were utilized and were linked by each study subject’s unique identification number. For confidentiality, this identification number was replaced by a serial number. We secured ethical approval from the regional ethical review board of Lund University. Drug abuse was identified through Swedish medical and criminal registries (for further details, see Table S1 in the online supplement). Individuals could have several registrations in the criminal or medical registers. To avoid double-counting, we allowed for a 90-day period after each registration in which a new registration was not counted.
Using the Swedish multigenerational register, we selected all parent-offspring pairs in which the parent (primary case subject) was registered for drug abuse when the offspring (at risk of becoming a secondary case subject) was between 19 and 23 years old. Drug abuse registration begins at age 16, and at least 3 years of data were required to define a stable baseline. An upper limit of 23 years old was selected because offspring rarely cohabit with their parents beyond age 23. We refer to this registration of the primary case subject as the index registration. We also required that individuals at risk of becoming secondary case subjects reside in the same household with the primary case subject at the time of the index drug abuse registration. For individuals at risk of becoming secondary case subjects, we included drug abuse registration for six different time points: within 1–365 days, within 366–730 days, and within 731–1,096 days before and after the drug abuse registration in the index case. For each pair comprising a primary parental case and an at-risk offspring, we matched several control pairs on sex and year of birth of both parent and child, country of birth and neighborhood socioeconomic status of the child, and number of lifetime drug abuse registrations, type of registration (medical or criminal), and educational status of the parent. We matched on these variables both to control for demographic characteristics (21) and to control for risk of drug abuse among the relatives of affected case subjects (22).
Control subjects were matched on drug abuse registration of the primary case subject (residing in the same household as the at-risk secondary case subject) that occurred when the putative secondary case subject was between ages 10 and 19, drug abuse registration of the primary case subject (residing in the same household as the at-risk secondary case subject) that occurred when the putative secondary case subject was between ages 0 and 9, drug abuse registration of the primary case subject that occurred before the putative secondary case subject was 19 years old (whether or not residing in the same household, and the registration could occur before age 0 of the secondary case subject), and lifetime drug abuse registration (yes or no) of the other biological parent and drug abuse registration (yes or no) of the other biological parent that occurred when the at-risk secondary case subject was between ages 19 and 23. All of these variables substantially predicted risk for drug abuse in offspring of affected fathers.
The matching procedure ensured that the difference between individuals at risk of becoming secondary case subjects and matched control subjects was that control subjects 19–23 years old did not have a parent with a drug abuse registration while they were residing in the same household with the parent. In particular, case and control subjects were matched for familial or genetic risk. We included a maximum of five control pairs, but pairings with fewer than five control subjects were also included in the analyses (Table 1). The use of a control group in the analyses was necessary, because there was a yearly increase in drug abuse registrations among individuals ages 19–23 that we otherwise could not have separated from the effect of the index drug abuse registration among the primary case subjects.
TABLE 1. Characteristics of primary case subjects with drug abuse registration and at-risk secondary case subjectsa
VariableParentsFull SiblingsFirst Cousins
Household Pairs (N=2,651) (%)SAMS Pairs (N=1,324) (%)Municipality Pairs (N=4,189) (%)Household Pairs (N=9,464) (%)SAMS Pairs (N=7,107) (%)Municipality Pairs (N=9,311) (%)SAMS Pairs (N=5,900) (%)Municipality (N=25,060) (%)
Male primary case subjects3953598278767675
Male at-risk secondary case subjects6155506051495452
Criminal registration of primary case subject2534367666637270
Drug abuse registrations of primary case subjects        
 15241353733333636
 21415131614141615
 37689991010
 >32737443844443840
Drug abuse of primary case subject        
 Before the at-risk secondary case subject was 19 years old2433391720211417
 When the at-risk secondary case subject was 10–19 years old and residing in the same household, SAMS, or municipality2232371720211417
 When the at-risk secondary case subject was 0–9 years old and residing in the same household, SAMS, or municipality4611
Drug abuse in other biological parent of at-risk secondary case subject888
Drug abuse of other biological parent when the at-risk secondary case subject was 19–23 years old0.20.51 
Education of primary case subjects        
 Low2737354347444442
 Middle5757564847485049
 High167997879
Neighborhood socioeconomic status of primary case subjects at the time of drug abuse registration        
 Low13610168131017
 Middle5960605153556459
 High2734303339332623
Control pairsb        
 1282335231415915
 2171520171214411
 3111011131013210
 4887991018
 53744283854469456
a
Ns in the column headings refer to pairs, not individuals. The mean year of birth for the parent group, full-sibling group, and cousin group, respectively, was as follows: household, 1982 (SD=5.6 years), Small Area Market Surveys (SAMS), 1982 (SD=6.1 years), and municipality, 1981 (SD=6.5 years); household, 1985 (SD=7.2 years), SAMS, 1980 (SD=9.4 years), and municipality, 1980 (SD=10.0 years); and SAMS, 1984 (SD=6.0 years), and municipality, 1982 (SD=5.8 years).
b
The number of control subjects for the parent group, full-sibling group, and cousin group, respectively, was as follows: household, 8,104, SAMS, 4,420, and municipality, 11,399; household, 30,222, SAMS, 26,783, and municipality, 33,247; and SAMS, 26,226, and municipality, 92,636.
We used a difference-in-difference approach to examine the causal effect of drug abuse transmission from parent to offspring (23). The underlying assumption in the difference-in-difference model is that the trend in drug abuse among individuals at risk of becoming secondary case subjects would have been the same as the risk among matched control subjects in the absence of the index registration of the primary case subject. To validate this assumption, we included the history of drug abuse registrations among individuals at risk of becoming secondary case subjects (and among control subjects) for 3 years before the index drug abuse registration of the primary case subject. A causal interpretation is supported by a similar trend in the risk for drug abuse before the index registration of the primary case subject among both control subjects and individuals at risk of becoming secondary case subjects but a different trend after the index registration of the primary case subject. We used a three-level linear probability model with individual drug abuse registrations (yes or no) during the six specified time periods nested within each secondary case and each control case that in turn were nested within a stratum consisting of both individuals at risk of becoming secondary case subjects and their matched counterparts. This model takes into account the repeated measures for each secondary case subject and each control subject. In the model, we included a dummy variable defining case or control and dummy variables for the different time periods, except within 2–3 years before drug abuse registration of the index case subject who was used as a reference. Finally, we included interaction terms between the five time dummy variables on the one hand and the case or control covariate on the other. A significant interaction term suggests that the slope of drug abuse was steeper among possible secondary case subjects than among their matched controls during the specific time period. The use of this linear probability model, from which our result is the percent gain in prevalence among at-risk secondary case subjects after index registration of the primary case subject, is warranted as an alternative logit model—which could produce odds ratios—would not give us the ability to explore the common trend assumption (23).
We replicated the analyses using different definitions of geographical proximity. First, we used Small Area Market Surveys (SAMS), a definition of neighborhoods used by Statistics Sweden, the Swedish government-owned statistics bureau. There are approximately 9,200 SAMS throughout Sweden, with an average population of 1,000. Second, we used municipalities. There are 290 municipalities in Sweden, and they comprise the country’s lower-level local government entities. Pairs included in the household analyses were not included in the SAMS analyses, and pairs included in the household or SAMS were not included in the municipality analyses. Thereafter, we replicated the same approach using full-sibling pairs and first-cousin pairs, and thus the primary case subject was an individual who was registered for drug abuse at a time when his or her sibling or cousin was between 19 and 23 years old and resided in the same household, SAMS, or municipality as the primary case subject.
In all analyses, we investigated within- and across-sex transmission. For the parent-offspring analyses, we stratified the analyses on the basis of age difference: <20 years, 20–30 years, and >30 years. For siblings and cousins, we used 1–3 years, 4–6 years, and 7–10 years. For siblings and cousins, we also stratified on the basis of whether the primary case subject was 1–3 years older or 1–3 years younger. In the stratified analyses, we combined the household and SAMS samples to increase power.
All statistical analyses were performed with SAS, version 9.4, and the R package lme4 (24).

Results

Descriptive Features

Key descriptive features of our eight classes of informative pairs of relatives are presented in Table 1. The largest sample was first-cousin pairs residing in the same municipality, followed by full siblings in the same household and same municipality. For full siblings and cousins, the primary case subjects were predominantly male, but the opposite was observed among parents residing in the same household with the offspring. This is a result of the high divorce rate among parental primary case subjects and the greater probability that the offspring will reside with the mother after divorce. The proportion of primary case subjects ascertained for drug abuse in the criminal registry was higher among sibling and cousin pairs than among parents.

Main Pattern of Findings

We first examined results for parents and siblings cohabiting within the same household (Figure 1). For parental primary case subjects in the same household, the risk for drug abuse registration among offspring was as predicted 2 years before the index registration. However, in the year before the parental index registration, the risk increased substantially, and it remained elevated during the subsequent 3 years. The pattern was similar when we examined primary and secondary drug abuse case subjects among sibling pairs living in the same household, except that the confidence intervals were smaller given the larger sample size.
FIGURE 1. Increase in risk for drug abuse (DA) registration among offspring, siblings, and cousins of index case subjects 3 years before and after DA registrationa
a The graphs show the index relative as a function of the proximity of the index case and secondary case (orange dotted lines), results in offspring or siblings of control subjects (green dotted lines), and results expected in the at-risk secondary cases if the parent did not have an index registration for DA (black dotted lines). Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals. SAMS=Small Area Market Surveys.
Among parent-offspring and sibling and cousin pairs living in the same SAMS (Figure 1), the pattern among siblings and cousins was similar to that noted above, except the effect was attenuated more in cousin pairs than in siblings pairs. We observed two differences in the curves for parent-offspring pairs residing in the same SAMS. First, the pattern was left-shifted, with increased risk for the target offspring beginning 2 years as opposed to 1 year before the index parental registration. Second, the observed risk for the target offspring of both case and control parents did not differ at our starting point 3 years before the index registration.
Among pairs residing in the same municipality, the shapes of the curves are similar to those observed for pairs living in the same household or SAMS, although the effects are smaller. The results defining the risk period as the first 3 years after the index registration are presented in Figure 2. For offspring residing in the same household, neighborhood, or municipality with the primary parental case subject, the prevalence of drug abuse increased 5.9%, 3.4%, and 1.8%, respectively. The increased risk among case subjects compared with control subjects can be expressed by relative risks, which were 1.58 (95% CI=1.46, 1.70), 1.38 (95% CI=1.27, 1.50), and 1.17 (95% CI=1.08, 1.26), respectively. For siblings of sibling index case subjects, parallel results were prevalence increases of 5.9%, 3.9%, and 1.2% and relative risks of 1.52 (95% CI=1.42, 1.63), 1.37 (95% CI=1.28, 1.47), and 1.12 (95% CI=1.04, 1.21). For cousins of cousin index case subjects, excess risk among individuals residing in the same neighborhood or municipality was 2.9% and 0.9%, respectively, which produced relatives risks of 1.48 (95% CI=1.32, 1.67) and 1.23 (95% CI=1.09, 1.41), respectively.
FIGURE 2. Increased risk for drug abuse (DA) registration in potential secondary cases 1–3 years after DA registration of the index relative as a function of the proximity of the primary and secondary case subjectsa
a Asterisks above columns indicate the statistical significance of the analysis. SAMS=Small Area Market Surveys. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
*p<0.05. ***p<0.0001.

Moderation of Transmission by Sex, Age Differences, and Older-Younger Status

We observed consistent differences in the magnitude of transmission of risk by sex of the primary and secondary case subjects across all three relationships (parents, full siblings, cousins) (Figure 3). Consistently, male-to-male transmission was strongest, and female-to-male transmission was second strongest. For parents and siblings, female-female transmission was stronger than that seen in male-female pairs. This was not observed for cousins, but effect sizes for all except the male-male pairs were small and confidence intervals wide.
FIGURE 3. Increased risk for drug abuse (DA) registration in potential secondary cases 1–3 years after DA registration of the index relative as a function of sex and age relationships of the primary and secondary case subjectsa
a Asterisks above columns indicate the statistical significance of the analysis; asterisks adjacent to the name of the variable under examination indicate whether the results in the analyses are statistically heterogeneous. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
*p<0.05. **p<0.001. ***p<0.0001.
In all relative pairs, transmission of risk from the primary case subject to the target relative was stronger for those closer in age. The risks were significantly different only for full siblings. In both siblings and cousins, no appreciable transmission of risk was observed among pairs with a 7- to 10-year age difference.

Assessment of Registry Bias

Could drug abuse registration of a primary case subject upwardly bias the probability of registration for an at-risk secondary case subject? Both police and medical personnel may be alert to drug abuse problems of secondary case subjects if they know the relationship between them and the affected primary individual. We have indirect evidence that this cannot explain our total effect, because this bias should be weak to absent in two relatives living in different parts of major Swedish cities. We can directly address this concern by examining cross-registry transmission of risk. As shown in Figure 4, in parent-offspring, full-sibling, and cousin pairs, within-registry transmission (both criminal to criminal and medical to medical) was significant for all relative classes. Of the six cross-registry analyses, five (all but criminal to medical among cousins) were also significant. Furthermore, in all three relative classes, the medical-to-criminal transmission was equal to or greater than that observed for medical-to-medical transmission.
FIGURE 4. Increased risk for drug abuse (DA) registration among potential secondary cases 1–3 years after DA registration of the primary case subject for parents, siblings, and cousins as a function of the source of registration for DA among the primary and secondary case subjectsa
a Cr=criminal registry, Med=medical registry. Asterisks above columns indicate the statistical significance of the analysis; asterisks adjacent to the name of the group of relatives indicate whether the results in the class are statistically heterogeneous. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.
*p<0.05. **p<0.001. ***p<0.0001.

Assessment of Individual Drugs of Abuse

The category of abused drug was available in 37% of sibling drug abuse cases. Sample sizes were large enough for meaningful analysis only for opiates and cannabis. The magnitude of transmission from opiate to opiate, cannabis to cannabis, opiate to cannabis, and cannabis to opiate was statistically heterogeneous (for further details, see Figure S1 in the online supplement). Opiate-to-opiate transmission was much stronger than opiate-to-cannabis. Cannabis-to-cannabis transmission was modest but stronger than cannabis-to-opiate transmission, for which we found virtually no evidence.

Discussion

We examined evidence for a contagion model of environmental transmission of drug abuse within families. We used the date of drug abuse registration of primary case subjects to define periods of high risk among their relatives. Compared with matched control subjects, an appreciably increased risk for drug abuse was found among relatives 1–3 years after the index drug abuse registration of the primary case.
The validity of our results is supported by six findings. First, within parent-offspring, sibling-sibling, and cousin-cousin pairs, transmitted risk was strongly correlated with geographical proximity. Second, when proximity was controlled for, transmitted risk was related to familial closeness, with stronger transmission from parents to offspring and sibling to sibling than from cousin to cousin. Third, within types of relatives, transmitted risk was stronger in those closer in age. Fourth, consistent with research showing that older siblings have greater influence on younger siblings compared with the reverse (2529), we observed the same effects for drug abuse transmission. Fifth, strength of transmission strongly depended on the sex composition of the pair and was consistently highest for male-male pairs and second highest for female-male pairs. This is congruent with research showing that the decision to use psychoactive substances is more affected by peer influences and the desire to conform to subgroup values among males compared with females (30, 31). Among Swedish high school students, peer deviance was a stronger predictor of drug use among males compared with females (32). Sixth, in the two most common substance abuse categories (opiates and cannabis), greater within-substance transmission than between-substance transmission was observed in siblings, which suggests direct transmission, although such resemblance could arise from local patterns of drug availability.
Although we examined only biological relatives, our findings are similar to those we reported previously on marital pairs in which neither spouse had prior drug abuse registrations (33). When one spouse had a first drug abuse registration, the risk for drug abuse registration for the other spouse increased substantially in the subsequent year and remained elevated over the next 4 years (33).
In considering the underlying basis for the transmission of drug abuse within relatives, we suggest the utility of differentiating two mechanisms. The first, a form of social learning (34), would consist of the primary case subject transmitting to relatives both positive attitudes toward drug use and a positive model for the drug use itself. Much of our results can be explained by a key postulate of social learning theory: the potency of a role model depends on the strength of the identification with the individual. We would predict that a male teenager would, on average, identify more strongly with a young father and an older brother close in age than with a mother or a younger cousin. Our results demonstrate this pattern. The second mechanism for environmental transmission of drug abuse is simpler: the primary case subject either supplies the substance to the at-risk relatives or tells them specifically how to obtain the substance. That this process is also at work is evidenced by the much stronger within-substance transmission than cross-substance transmission of drug abuse between relatives.
The drug abuse field is legitimately excited by advances in molecular techniques that will further elucidate the underlying nature of the genetic contributions to drug abuse. However, given that our case and control subjects were matched for genetic and familial risk, a model in which familial aggregation of drug abuse is solely a result of genetic factors could not plausibly explain the observed substantial temporal relationship of onsets in pairs of relatives.
Using triangulation—the application of multiple complementary methods to the same research questions (35)—we have demonstrated, in traditional twin, sibling, and adoption studies (2, 36), in novel designs using stepparents (37) and marital partners (33), in age differences within sibling (33) and cousin pairs (38), and now in contagion models, that environmental factors also contribute substantially to the transmission of drug abuse within families. Importantly, these environmentally informative designs highlight potential foci for prevention, including the identification of individuals at particularly high risk, such as close relatives of an individual with recent-onset drug abuse.

Limitations

Our findings should be interpreted in the context of six potential methodological limitations. First, drug abuse registration is not a precise index of the onset of drug abuse. Indeed, in most analyses, transmission of drug abuse from primary to secondary cases begins in the year before index registration, as would be expected because problematic drug use often precedes drug abuse registration by months or years. Prospective studies suggest that about 50% of individuals who transition from the use of common illicit drugs to abuse or dependence do so within 2–4 years, with faster transitions to abuse (3941). Second, given the increased risk for drug abuse among secondary case subjects before index registration of the primary case, we cannot rule out reverse causation (from the secondary to primary case). To investigate this, we examined siblings residing in the same household or community in our usual way and then repeated the analyses censoring all secondary case or control subjects with a drug abuse registration in the 3 years before index registration of the primary case subject. Although rates of drug abuse were lower in the censored subsamples, case-control differences in risk were unchanged (for further details, see Figure S2 in the online supplement), suggesting that reverse causation contributed minimally to our findings. Third, our drug abuse assessments differ qualitatively from those obtained by personal interviews and have the advantage of being objective and are unaffected by cooperation or recall bias. However, false negative reports are likely, because some drug abusers will avoid detection by registries. It is possible that by examining more severe cases of drug abuse, we overestimated the familial-environmental transmission of drug abuse. Fourth, we did not match index case and control subjects on geographical region. Could the correlations in risk between primary and secondary case subjects result from local drug abuse epidemics? To evaluate this, we compared estimates of excess rates of secondary cases among the siblings of index cases with those obtained by matching on municipality (since matching on SAMS proved to be infeasible). These estimates were 5.05% (95% CI=4.30–5.80) and 4.42% (95% CI=3.67–5.17), respectively, suggesting a modest reduction in rates of transmitted drug abuse when controlling for local period effects. Fifth, our main analyses defined the proximity of pairs of relatives (household, SAMS, or municipality) only at the time of index case registration. To determine the possible effect of this definition, we reanalyzed our sample including only pairs with maintained proximity over the entire 3-year follow-up period (for further details, see Tables S1 and S2 in the online supplement). In this censored sample, the excess risk for drug abuse in case subjects compared with control subjects increased substantially for pairs residing in the same household and modestly for those living in the same SAMS but decreased slightly for those residing in the same municipality. Finally, contagious transmission of drug abuse does not replicate all the features of infectious disease models (42). Future work will need to determine how instructive such models can be in understanding person-to-person transmission of drug abuse.

Conclusions

In this study, we expanded our insights into the within-family transmission of drug abuse by exploring and validating an environmentally mediated model of contagion. These effects are substantial, temporally delimited, and behave as would be expected if they were largely causal. Although genetic factors are clearly important, our results suggest that drug abuse can be transmitted as a contagious process within families. Such transmission likely involves both the communication of positive attitudes and role models for drug initiation, as well as subsequent misuse and the actual transfer of illicit substances. Understanding the importance of these within-family mechanisms has obvious implications for prevention.

Footnote

Supported by NIH (grant R01DA030005), the Swedish Research Council (grants K2012-70X-15428-08-3 and 2012-2378; 2014-10134), the Swedish Research Council for Health, Working Life, and Welfare (grant 2013-1836), Forte (grant 2014-0804), and Avtal om Läkarutbildning och Forskning funding from Region Skåne.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2018.18060637.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Merikangas KR, Stolar M, Stevens DE, et al: Familial transmission of substance use disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:973–979
2.
Kendler KS, Sundquist K, Ohlsson H, et al: Genetic and familial environmental influences on the risk for drug abuse: a national Swedish adoption study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69:690–697
3.
Tsuang MT, Bar JL, Harley RM, et al: The Harvard Twin Study of Substance Abuse: what we have learned. Harv Rev Psychiatry 2001; 9:267–279
4.
Kendler KS, Prescott CA: Cannabis use, abuse, and dependence in a population-based sample of female twins. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:1016–1022
5.
Lynskey MT, Heath AC, Nelson EC, et al: Genetic and environmental contributions to cannabis dependence in a national young adult twin sample. Psychol Med 2002; 32:195–207
6.
Cadoret RJ, Yates WR, Troughton E, et al: Adoption study demonstrating two genetic pathways to drug abuse. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1995; 52:42–52
7.
Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, et al: The causes of parent-offspring transmission of drug abuse: a Swedish population-based study. Psychol Med 2015; 45:87–95
8.
Sherva R, Wang Q, Kranzler H, et al: Genome-wide association study of cannabis dependence severity, novel risk variants, and shared genetic risks. JAMA Psychiatry 2016; 73:472–480
9.
Stringer S, Minică CC, Verweij KJ, et al: Genome-wide association study of lifetime cannabis use based on a large meta-analytic sample of 32 330 subjects from the International Cannabis Consortium. Transl Psychiatry 2016; 6:e769
10.
Gizer IR, Bizon C, Gilder DA, et al: Whole genome sequence study of cannabis dependence in two independent cohorts. Addict Biol 2018; 23:461–473
11.
Kalsi G, Euesden J, Coleman JR, et al: Genome-wide association of heroin dependence in Han Chinese. PLoS One 2016; 11:e0167388
12.
Cheng Z, Zhou H, Sherva R, et al: Genome-wide association study identifies a regulatory variant of RGMA associated with opioid dependence in European Americans. Biol Psychiatry 2018; 84:762–770
13.
Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Mezuk B, et al: Exposure to peer deviance during childhood and risk for drug abuse: a Swedish national co-relative control study. Psychol Med 2015; 45:855–864
14.
van den Bree MB, Pickworth WB: Risk factors predicting changes in marijuana involvement in teenagers. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:311–319
15.
Coie JD, Miller-Johnson S: Peer factors and interventions, in Child Delinquents: Development, Intervention, and Service Needs. Edited by Loeber R, Farrington DP. London, Sage Publications, 2001, pp 191–209
16.
Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, et al: The causal nature of the association between neighborhood deprivation and drug abuse: a prospective national Swedish co-relative control study. Psychol Med 2014; 44:2537–2546
17.
Donovan JE: A framework for studying parental socialization of child and adolescent substance use, in Alcohol Use Disorders: A Developmental Science Approach to Etiology. Edited by Fitzgerald HE, Puttler LI. New York, Oxford University Press, 2018, pp 143–180
18.
Needle RH, Su SS, Doherty W: Divorce, remarriage, and adolescent substance use: a perspective longitudinal study. J Marriage Fam 1990; 52:157–169
19.
Dishion TJ, Dodge KA: Deviant peer contagion in interventions and programs: an ecological framework for understanding influence mechanisms, in Deviant Peer Influences in Programs for Youth. Edited by Dodge KA, Dishion TJ, Lansford JE. New York, Guilford, 2006, pp 14–43
20.
Anthony JC: Deviant peer effects: perspectives of an epidemiologist, in Deviant Peer Influences in Programs for Youth. Edited by Dodge KA, Dishion TJ, Lansford JE. New York, Guilford, 2006, pp 44–66
21.
Murray CJ, Atkinson C, Bhalla K, et al: The state of US health, 1990–2010: burden of diseases, injuries, and risk factors. JAMA 2013; 310:591–608
22.
Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, et al: Clinical features of drug abuse that reflect genetic risk. Psychol Med 2014; 44:2547–2556
23.
Lechner M: The estimation of causal effects by difference-in-difference methods. Found Trends Econom 2010; 4:165–224
24.
Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, et al: Fitting linear mixed-effects models using Ime4. J Stat Softw 2015; 67:1–48
25.
Brody GH: Sibling relationship quality: its causes and consequences. Annu Rev Psychol 1998; 49:1–24
26.
Slomkowski C, Rende R, Conger KJ, et al: Sisters, brothers, and delinquency: evaluating social influence during early and middle adolescence. Child Dev 2001; 72:271–283
27.
Newman J: College students’ relationships with siblings. J Youth Adolesc 1991; 20:629–644
28.
Lamb ME, Sutton-Smith B: Sibling Relationships: Their Nature and Significance Across the Lifespan. Oxfordshire, United Kingdom, Psychology Press, 1982
29.
Brook JS, Whiteman M, Gordon AS, et al: Older brother's influence on younger sibling's drug use. J Psychol 1983; 114:83–90
30.
Graziano F, Bina M, Giannotta F, et al: Drinking motives and alcoholic beverage preferences among Italian adolescents. J Adolesc 2012; 35:823–831
31.
Borsari B, Carey KB: Peer influences on college drinking: a review of the research. J Subst Abuse 2001; 13:391–424
32.
Svensson R: Gender differences in adolescent drug use: the impact of parental monitoring and peer deviance. Youth Soc 2003; 34:300–329
33.
Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, et al: Within-family environmental transmission of drug abuse: a Swedish national study. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:235–242
34.
Bandura A, Huston AC: Identification as a process of incidental learning. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 1961; 63:311–318
35.
Munafò MR, Davey Smith G: Robust research needs many lines of evidence. Nature 2018; 553:399–401
36.
Kendler KS, Maes HH, Sundquist K, et al: Genetic and family and community environmental effects on drug abuse in adolescence: a Swedish national twin and sibling study. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:209–217
37.
Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist J, et al: Triparental families: a new genetic-epidemiological design applied to drug abuse, alcohol use disorders, and criminal behavior in a Swedish national sample. Am J Psychiatry 2015; 172:553–560
38.
Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist K, et al: Environmental influences on familial resemblance for drug abuse in first-cousin pairs: a Swedish national study. Psychol Med 2014; 44:371–379
39.
Wagner FA, Anthony JC: From first drug use to drug dependence: developmental periods of risk for dependence upon marijuana, cocaine, and alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology 2002; 26:479–488
40.
Lopez-Quintero C, Pérez de los Cobos J, Hasin DS, et al: Probability and predictors of transition from first use to dependence on nicotine, alcohol, cannabis, and cocaine: results of the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC). Drug Alcohol Depend 2011; 115:120–130
41.
Sartor CE, Kranzler HR, Gelernter J: Rate of progression from first use to dependence on cocaine or opioids: a cross-substance examination of associated demographic, psychiatric, and childhood risk factors. Addict Behav 2014; 39:473–479
42.
Grefenstette JJ, Brown ST, Rosenfeld R, et al: FRED (a Framework for Reconstructing Epidemic Dynamics): an open-source software system for modeling infectious diseases and control strategies using census-based populations. BMC Public Health 2013; 13:940

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 239 - 248
PubMed: 30818984

History

Received: 1 June 2018
Revision received: 20 September 2018
Revision received: 19 October 2018
Accepted: 13 November 2018
Published online: 1 March 2019
Published in print: March 01, 2019

Keywords

  1. Psychoactive Substance Use Disorder
  2. Families

Authors

Details

Kenneth S. Kendler, M.D. [email protected]
From the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, the Department of Psychiatry, and the Department of Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond (Kendler); the Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden (Ohlsson, J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist); the Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York (J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist); and the Center for Community-Based Healthcare Research and Education, Department of Functional Pathology, School of Medicine, Shimane University, Japan (J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist).
Henrik Ohlsson, Ph.D.
From the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, the Department of Psychiatry, and the Department of Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond (Kendler); the Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden (Ohlsson, J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist); the Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York (J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist); and the Center for Community-Based Healthcare Research and Education, Department of Functional Pathology, School of Medicine, Shimane University, Japan (J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist).
Jan Sundquist, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, the Department of Psychiatry, and the Department of Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond (Kendler); the Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden (Ohlsson, J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist); the Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York (J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist); and the Center for Community-Based Healthcare Research and Education, Department of Functional Pathology, School of Medicine, Shimane University, Japan (J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist).
Kristina Sundquist, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, the Department of Psychiatry, and the Department of Human and Molecular Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond (Kendler); the Center for Primary Health Care Research, Lund University, Malmö, Sweden (Ohlsson, J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist); the Department of Family Medicine and Community Health, Department of Population Health Science and Policy, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York (J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist); and the Center for Community-Based Healthcare Research and Education, Department of Functional Pathology, School of Medicine, Shimane University, Japan (J. Sundquist, K. Sundquist).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Kendler ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

National Institute on Drug Abuse10.13039/100000026: R01DA030005

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share