The tenets of social epidemiology suggest that where and how we live, work, play, and age affect our health and well-being (
1). While some social determinants of health (SDoH) are modifiable (e.g., health behaviors) or related to our lived environment (e.g., affordability of and access to health care), others are difficult to change in older age (e.g., childhood education quality, nativity status, occupational history, and income). It is well established that Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults in the United States experience poorer health outcomes and have lower health care utilization compared with White adults (
2,
3). Disparities in one domain of health may intersect with and precipitate disparities in another domain (
2), and while some health disparities compound over time (i.e., cumulative disadvantage) (
4), others are triggered by a decline in health or are persistent across the lifespan (
2). The origins of most health disparities are complex and difficult to disentangle because overt and covert (structural) racism (
5) and discrimination have shaped SDoH among racially and ethnically marginalized adults in the United States.
Black adults are more likely than White adults to have multiple chronic diseases, while Hispanic/Latinx adults are less likely than both Black and White adults to have multimorbidity (
6). In analyses of the Health and Retirement Study (HRS), it was shown that Black adults developed multimorbidity at an earlier age than White adults and that both Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults were more likely than White adults to experience severe and persistent functional disability (
2,
7,
8). In addition to poorer physical health, marginalized older adults often report worse mental health, although Black and Hispanic/Latinx older adults are less likely than White older adults to be screened for or diagnosed with depression (
9). This may be due to disparities in mental health care initiation and adequacy (
10) or beliefs surrounding mental illness, stigma, and preferences for treatment (
11). Depressive symptomatology is thought to be greater among Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults than among White adults 50 to 75 years of age; however, this disparity narrows substantially between ages 76 and 90 (
12). In addition to having greater depressive symptomatology, Black older adults may be more socially disconnected, have greater perceived isolation, and have smaller social networks (
13). Some studies suggest that the rate of loneliness is higher among Hispanic/Latinx adults than among White adults (
14), while other studies do not show this relationship (
15). Deficits in cognition among marginalized adults have been linked to poor early-life educational quality and literacy in addition to other health, psychosocial, and socioeconomic factors (
16,
17). While these findings paint a picture of poorer health and well-being among Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults, the healthy immigrant paradox (
18) suggests that immigrants and migrants may appear healthier than their U.S.-born counterparts, and differences in these outcomes may exist across ethnic subgroups.
Understanding the effects of SDoH on psychological health and well-being across racial and ethnic groups is crucial for the development of primordial prevention strategies (e.g., policies targeting socioenvironmental risk factors at an early age to prevent downstream disadvantage and morbidity). Several SDoH are known to affect psychological health and well-being (operationalized in this study as depressive symptomatology, cognition, and self-rated health). Education and income indirectly influence health and well-being through differences in health behaviors, diet, environmental exposures, and access to health care (
19–
21) and have direct effects on factors such as cognitive reserve (
22) and allostatic load (
23). Beyond socioeconomic status and health care access, other SDoH, such as marital status (with depressed affect, loneliness, and social isolation as possible mechanisms) (
24), veteran status (potentially associated with increased allostatic load, posttraumatic stress disorder, and substance abuse) (
25), nativity status (resulting in acculturation and perceived discrimination) (
20,
26), and geographic place of residence (e.g., exposure to the “Southern diet” in the United States and differential access to quality education, health care, and jobs) (
27,
28), are known to influence health and well-being. Whether SDoH explain a larger proportion of the Black–White and Hispanic/Latinx–White disparities in psychological health and well-being compared with other well-known correlates of health is less well understood.
Using data from the HRS, we posit that selected SDoH (i.e., education, parental education, number of years worked, marital status, veteran status, geographic residence, nativity status, income, and health insurance coverage) will explain a larger share of the disparities in depressive symptomatology, cognition, and self-rated health among Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults compared with White adults than age, sex, measures of health, health behaviors, and health care utilization. By identifying the primary SDoH that drive racial and ethnic disparities in psychological health and well-being, government agencies and health care systems can better allocate resources to interventions and policies that will appreciably reduce health inequities.
Methods
Methodological references, detailed descriptions of the study variables, and model specifications are provided in the online supplement.
Participants
The HRS is a publicly available prospective cohort study of adults ≥51 years of age in the United States that started in 1992 with assessments occurring every 2 years. Black adults were oversampled from the southern portion of the United States, and Hispanic/Latinx adults were oversampled from the western portion of the United States. We utilized the RAND HRS longitudinal file because it contains imputed variables (e.g., cognition and wealth and income), which were crucial for modeling. We used data from wave 13 (2016) of the HRS.
Few participants age 90 or older identified as Black or Hispanic/Latinx, and therefore participants in the Baby Boomer and Silent Generation cohorts were selected (born between 1928 and 1964). After excluding participants based on their birth year, participants identifying their race as “other” and not identifying as Hispanic/Latinx and those without scores on the 8-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) or the Modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS-M) were excluded (see Figure S1 in the online supplement). A final sample of 11,143 adults was retained as our analytic sample (2,306 non–Hispanic/Latinx Black, 1,593 Hispanic/Latinx, and 7,244 non–Hispanic/Latinx White adults). Participants identifying as Black, “other,” or White and Hispanic/Latinx were categorized as Hispanic/Latinx.
Outcomes and Measures
We operationalized psychological health and well-being as depressive symptomatology, cognition, and self-rated health. Depressive symptomatology was measured by the CES-D (range, 0–8). Global cognition was measured with the total cognition score from the TICS-M (range, 0–30). Participants were asked to rate their general health from 1 (excellent) to 5 (poor).
Selected Social Determinants of Health
Selected SDoH included education (years), mother’s education (years), father’s education (years), number of years worked (years), marital status (dummy coded with 1 indicating married, married with an absent spouse, or partnered and 0 indicating separated, divorced, widowed, or never married), veteran status (yes [1] or no [0]), southern U.S. resident (dummy coded with 1 indicating south and 0 indicating northeast, midwest, west, or other U.S. resident), nativity status (non-U.S. born [1] or U.S. born [0]), annual household income (in U.S. dollars, natural-log transformed), employer-sponsored health insurance coverage (yes [1] or no [0]), Medicare coverage (yes [1] or no [0]), and Medicaid coverage (yes [1] or no [0]).
Statistical Analysis
Participant characteristics are reported with descriptive statistics, and unadjusted differences between racial/ethnic groups were examined with analysis of variance or chi-square tests. For both continuous and categorical variables, effect sizes, computed as Cohen’s d, were used to compare groups of Black and White adults and groups of Hispanic/Latinx and White adults (see Methods section in the online supplement).
To determine whether SDoH explained much of the Black–White and Hispanic/Latinx–White disparities in depressivesymptomatology, cognition, and self-rated health, twofold Blinder-Oaxaca decomposition was used with reference coefficients recovered from a pooled regression model that included group membership in the model, as recommended by Jann (
29). Blinder-Oaxaca decomposition was used to estimate the magnitude of the disparity between Black and White participants and then between Hispanic/Latinx and White participants. Next, the disparity was decomposed into a characteristics effect (i.e., variance explained by differences in the characteristics of each group) and a coefficients effect (i.e., variance explained by differences in beta coefficients when linear regressions are conducted for each group).
As an example, assume that Black participants were younger than White participants in the HRS. The characteristics effect would determine what proportion of the disparity in cognition was explained by the younger age of the Black participants. The coefficients effect would determine what proportion of the disparity in cognition was driven by differences in the beta coefficients for age. Because no simple explanation exists for why a beta coefficient differs by race/ethnicity, the coefficients effect is often referred to as the “unexplained” effect. In labor economics, the coefficients effect often represents discrimination. However, in our study, the coefficients effect could represent nonequivalent effects of the covariates (e.g., 1 year of education does not have an equivalent effect across groups), testing biases by group (e.g., questions in the CES-D may be culturally specific), or error variance due to unobserved constructs. In this report, the results focus on the characteristics effects.
To improve interpretability of the results, estimates were converted to shares, which represent the proportion of the disparity that the covariate explains. A positive share for age would suggest that if Black and White adults were the same age, the disparity in cognition would decrease. In contrast, a negative share would indicate that if Black and White adults were the same age, the disparity in cognition would increase. Shares that exceed 100% indicate that the marginalized group would be better off than White adults if characteristics were equivalent. Rather than reporting p values, 95% confidence intervals were calculated. Share intervals that contained 0% were considered null effects.
Discussion
Our hypotheses were fully supported for the Black–White disparities in depressive symptomatology, cognition, and self-rated health, with SDoH explaining the largest share of each disparity (51%, 39%, and 37%, respectively). For the Hispanic/Latinx–White comparisons, the findings were less straightforward; SDoH did not account for the largest share in depressive symptomatology (age and physical health primarily accounted for this disparity), but they were associated with the largest proportions of the disparities in cognition (76%) and self-rated health (75%). These effects were larger than what was reported for the Black–White comparison (39% and 37%, respectively), suggesting that disparities in cognition and self-rated health are more likely to be associated with differences in selected SDoH for Hispanic/Latinx adults than for Black adults. Because many SDoH are difficult to change in older age, primordial prevention strategies are needed to target health and health care access in early childhood and need to be carried throughout adulthood (e.g., policies aimed at educational quality, income inequality, insurance parity, and workers at risk of leaving the workforce).
In addition to the anticipated and well-documented effects of education and income inequity (
20–
22), number of years worked was associated with disparities in depressive symptomatology, cognition, and self-rated health among Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults. Number of years worked may index several factors, such as age, the ability to find and maintain employment, and disability status, all of which may correlate with psychological health and well-being. Although Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults were on average 6 years younger than White adults, they worked for 8 and 13 fewer years than White adults, respectively. This suggests that inequities in number of years worked were not explained by differences in age alone. It may be that physical functioning, ability to find and maintain adequate employment to meet financial needs (
30), and familial caregiving responsibilities (
31) contributed to the lower number of years worked among marginalized adults.
Given that most Black and Hispanic/Latinx older adults should be covered by Medicare in the United States, it is disconcerting that lower health care coverage and utilization was found among Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults. Although overall coverage and utilization rates among marginalized adults improved as a result of the Affordable Care Act (
3), Black adults saw the smallest benefit (
19). Several explanations for poorer health care coverage and utilization may exist, including de facto segregation of health care facilities and historical traumas. Although sanctioned forms of hospital segregation were eliminated in the 1960s, structural racism remains, partly due to housing segregation and other socioeconomic pressures (
32). In addition to geographical location having an impact on access, health care coverage and utilization are also affected by distrust of medical systems by communities of color as a result of historical traumas (e.g., the Tuskegee syphilis experiment in Black men and forced sterilization of Latinas), age (i.e., beneficiary status), and willingness of employers to sponsor health insurance for blue-collar or unskilled workers—among whom Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults are disproportionately represented in the U.S. workforce (
30). Furthermore, immigrants must be lawful permanent residents in the United States to receive Medicare, suggesting that nativity status may have had an indirect impact on these constructs through insurance parity.
One area of health that deserves greater attention is cognition, given the perpetuation of structural racism (
5) despite the end of legal racial segregation and recent societal trends of improved access to education and health care. Our study found that the Black–White disparity in cognition was more difficult to explain (86% unexplained) than the Hispanic/Latinx–White disparity in cognition (36% unexplained). Participants’, mothers’, and fathers’ education in years were associated with the disparity to a greater extent among Hispanic/Latinx adults than among Black adults. This phenomenon suggests that 1 year of education is not equivalent among Black, Hispanic/Latinx, and White adults and that improving educational attainment (years alone) may not ameliorate cognitive disparities among Black adults in later life. The quality of education that Black older adults received in the Jim Crow era may have affected the comparability of years of education. Additionally, reading level (
16), ageism, racism, xenophobia, and test bias are other relevant factors when measuring cognition among marginalized populations (
33,
34) but were not accounted for in our study. Beyond education and discrimination, income and insurance parity correlated with cognition among Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults, meaning that health care access and utilization may influence these disparities. Language fluency and acculturation may also affect cognitive performance, particularly on measures that assess word recall and recognition if participants were tested in a nonnative language. Moreover, telephone and Internet assessments may not generalize as well to older adults from lower socioeconomic backgrounds. Understanding how SDoH map onto cognitive trajectories will be increasingly important, given the large and growing proportion of marginalized adults diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias (
35).
Depressive symptomatology and self-rated health are constructs intimately related to psychological health and well-being. The Black–White mental health paradox suggests that Black adults have lower or similar rates of mental health disorders relative to White adults despite poorer physical health and being subjected to greater adversities (
36). In our study and other studies that use the HRS (
37), Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults endorsed greater depressive symptomatology and rated their health as poorer than White adults. Although the CES-D is commonly used to identify clinically significant depression, symptomatology cannot be directly related to psychiatric disorders. For example, Black adults may endorse greater symptomatology than White adults but may not meet the clinical threshold for major depressive disorder, as the criteria may be culturally, racially, or ethnically specific (see the Discussion and Reference sections in the
online supplement). Interestingly, marital status was associated with the disparity in depressive symptomatology among Black adults, whereas age and physical health were associated with the disparity among Hispanic/Latinx adults. Being married is protective against depression (
38), and the literature suggests that higher socioeconomic status, greater physical health, and being married may attenuate depression among Black and White adults (
39). Although our study showed a relationship between physical health and depressive symptomatology, only a modest effect of income was reported. Rather, our study suggests that number of years worked was more strongly associated than income with the Black–White and Hispanic/Latinx–White disparities in depressive symptomatology. This provides further evidence that number of years worked remains a unique social determinant of health that operates independently from age and socioeconomic status, such that finding stable employment throughout one’s life may have a similar effect on psychological health and well-being whether at $20,000 or $200,000 per annum. Furthermore, others have proposed that chronic stress exposure and stress appraisal may explain the Black–White disparity in depressive symptomatology observed in the HRS (
37). These constructs are likely along the causal pathway between SDoH and depressive symptomatology and should be investigated further.
Disparities in self-rated health were associated with differences in the characteristics of Black (85% explained) and Hispanic/Latinx (92% explained) adults. Inequities in selected SDoH accounted for a greater proportion of the disparities in self-rated health than age, sex, measures of health, health behaviors, and health care utilization. SDoH such as low educational attainment and income may have an impact on self-rated health through poor neighborhood safety and low physical activity (
40). Our study provides nuance to these findings because self-rated health was not exclusively related to education and income. Self-rated health was also associated with number of years worked (among Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults) and parental education levels (among Hispanic/Latinx adults). Moreover, in line with the cumulative disadvantage theory (
4), we found that inequities in cognition and depressive symptomatology were strongly associated with the disparity in self-rated health and that inequities in self-rated health were also associated with the disparities in depressive symptomatology among Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults.
This study has several limitations. Comparing Black and Hispanic/Latinx adults with White adults is arbitrary. Clinicians should refrain from viewing White adults as the gold standard of health and should work alongside their patients to develop culturally sensitive and appropriate treatment goals. Blinder-Oaxaca decomposition and the theory from which it developed assumes that one group is marginalized and that this marginalization can be explained through observed and unobserved effects. When using Blinder-Oaxaca decomposition to study disparities between two marginalized groups, it loses much of the meaning. Although we could not use Blinder-Oaxaca decomposition to adequately measure disparities between marginalized communities (i.e., to compare Black with Hispanic/Latinx adults), we believe that this topic deserves further investigation.
Our findings should be understood in the context of some level of selection bias and overrepresentation of specific ethnic subgroups. The majority of the Hispanic/Latinx participants were oversampled from the western portion of the United States and are disproportionately of Mexican ancestry. The Black participants were oversampled from the southern portion of the United States, where education, income, and health care access—historically—are particularly inequitable.
This study had a cross-sectional design and thus cannot be used to infer causality. Additionally, our definition of psychological health and well-being was limited to measures from a single scale of depressive symptomatology, a single item of self-rated health with five possible responses, and a single scale of cognitive function. Finally, using splines in future analyses may better control for nonlinear effects of age. (See the Discussion and Reference sections in the online supplement for additional material.)
In summary, we found strong evidence that selected SDoH accounted for larger proportions of Black–White disparities in depressive symptomatology, cognition, and self-rated health than each of the other four domains (demographics, physical health, mental health and cognition, and health behaviors and health care utilization). Conversely, selected SDoH were associated with larger proportions of the Hispanic/Latinx–White disparities in cognition and self-rated health, but other factors, such as age and physical health, were related to the disparity in depressive symptomatology.