Skip to main content
Full access
Published Online: 6 October 2020

Front Matter

Publication: Nature and Nurture in Mental Disorders: A Gene-Environment Model
NATURE AND NURTURE IN MENTAL DISORDERS
A GENE-ENVIRONMENT MODEL
Second Edition
NATURE AND NURTURE IN MENTAL DISORDERS
A GENE-ENVIRONMENT MODEL
Second Edition
Joel Paris, M.D.
Professor Emeritus of Psychiatry, McGill University,
Montreal, Quebec, Canada
Note: The authors have worked to ensure that all information in this book is accurate at the time of publication and consistent with general psychiatric and medical standards, and that information concerning drug dosages, schedules, and routes of administration is accurate at the time of publication and consistent with standards set by the U.S. Food and Drug Administration and the general medical community. As medical research and practice continue to advance, however, therapeutic standards may change. Moreover, specific situations may require a specific therapeutic response not included in this book. For these reasons and because human and mechanical errors sometimes occur, we recommend that readers follow the advice of physicians directly involved in their care or the care of a member of their family.
Books published by American Psychiatric Association Publishing represent the findings, conclusions, and views of the individual authors and do not necessarily represent the policies and opinions of American Psychiatric Association Publishing or the American Psychiatric Association.
The author has indicated that he has no financial interests or other affiliations that represent or could appear to represent a competing interest with his contributions to this book.
Copyright © 2021 American Psychiatric Association Publishing
ALL RIGHTS RESERVED
Second Edition
Manufactured in the United States of America on acid-free paper
24 23 22 21 20  5 4 3 2 1
American Psychiatric Association Publishing
800 Maine Avenue SW
Suite 900
Washington, DC 20024-2812
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Names: Paris, Joel, author.
Title: Nature and nurture in mental disorders : a gene-environment model / Joel Paris.
Other titles: Nature and nurture in psychiatry
Description: Second edition. | Washington, DC : American Psychiatric Association  Publishing, [2021] | Preceded by: Nature and nurture in psychiatry : a predisposition- stress model of mental disorders / by Joel Paris. Washington, DC : American  Psychiatric Press, Inc., c1999. | Includes bibliographical references and index.
Identifiers: LCCN 2020033272 (print) | LCCN 2020033273 (ebook) | ISBN  9781615373345 (paperback) alk. paper | ISBN 9781615373680 (ebook)
Subjects: MESH: Mental Disorders—genetics | Mental Disorders—psychology |  Genetic Predisposition to Disease | Stress, Psychological | Models, Psychological  Classification: LCC RC455.4.G4 (print) | LCC RC455.4.G4 (ebook) | NLM WM  140 | DDC 616.89/042—dc23
LC record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2020033272
LC ebook record available at https://lccn.loc.gov/2020033273
British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data
A CIP record is available from the British Library.

Contents

Preface to the Second Edition
Introduction
Part I
Theory
1 Historical Overview
2 Genetic Predispositions
3 Environmental Stressors
4 Gene-Environment Interactions
5 Diagnoses, Disorders, and Traits
Part II
Mental Disorders
6 ADHD and Conduct Disorder
7 Schizophrenia
8 Depressive Disorders
9 Anxiety Disorders and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
10 Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
11 Eating Disorders
12 Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders
13 Personality Disorders
Part III
Implications
14 Clinical Implications
15 Implications for Prevention and Further Research
References
Index

PREFACE TO THE SECOND EDITION

THE FIRST edition of this book was published in 1999. That year marked the end of the “decade of the brain.” Over the next two decades, neuroscience continued to advance and has become the primary basis of clinical work. Yet, in many ways, we just do not know enough to practice on the basis of neuroscience. Some claim that research will lead to personalized medicine based on individual differences in the genome, but that is not likely to happen anytime soon.
Nineteen ninety-eight was a time when many of us anticipated that the decoding of the genome would lead to dramatic scientific breakthroughs. As far as both medicine and psychiatry were concerned, however, we were wrong. This new edition of the book draws on genome-wide association studies to show that most human traits are influenced by hundreds if not thousands of genes. This level of complexity means it will be many decades before genetics can be applied to understanding and treating mental disorders.
We should keep in mind that it is early days for neuroscience, and findings could eventually lead to more treatment options. Yet an equally strong literature supports the importance of environmental risk factors in mental disorders. This book shows that, for most conditions seen in psychiatry, both genes and environment play a major role in etiology.
Most of the previous edition of this book is now out of date; thus, the second edition has been almost entirely rewritten. This new version shows how current evidence supports an interactive model, combining recent data from neuroscience with a large body of evidence about the influence of the psychosocial environment. I have therefore changed the subtitle of the book from “predisposition-stress” to the more precise “gene-environment” model. In the first edition, I focused concern about reliance on overly environmental models of psychopathology, particularly those based on psychoanalysis or on narrowly based sociocultural theories. But those ideas have undergone a steady decline. Over the past 20 years, psychiatry has become much more biological in orientation. I now see a different set of problems, with the greater danger now lying in ignoring the environment entirely.
We hear the common mantra that “mental disorders are brain disorders.” There is, of course, no such thing as mind without brain—they are just different levels of looking at human thought, emotion, and behavior. But I disagree with reducing the mind, which has crucial emergent properties, to the level of neuronal function. I also disagree with the idea that the complexity of mental illnesses, from the most severe to the most common, can be fully explained at a cellular or neurochemical level. That narrow vision helps explain why so many patients today, particularly those diagnosed with depression, are being treated entirely with medication. This kind of practice is based on the idea that mental disorders can be entirely accounted for by abnormalities in neurotransmission or neuroconnectivity.
This second edition argues that this reductionistic view is not supported by firm evidence. Instead, it supports a biopsychosocial model in which the object of study is the mind and the person. It rejects both biological and psychosocial reductionism; instead, the link between mind and brain is best accounted for by gene-environment interactions. When one thinks interactively, a nature-nurture dichotomy becomes unnecessary.
These ideas are hardly original; most clinicians would agree with them in principle. However, the biopsychosocial model is often paid lip service and not taken seriously. The result is a practice in which clinicians replace understanding of life experience with DSM-based checklists and overly zealous pharmacological interventions. Another message of this book is that a broader model has important clinical implications. Genes and environment need to be studied—not separately, but in interaction. Based on a large body of research conducted over the past 20 years, combining neuroscience with social sciences could become the basis of more effective treatment.
Unfortunately, the divide between the “two cultures” of psychiatry continues to interfere with progress in planning treatment. Researchers in neuroscience may either ignore effects of the environment or measure them with vague concepts such as “stress” or “trauma.” At the same time, although researchers on the psychosocial factors in mental disorders may know that half the variance affecting psychopathology is genetic, they apply models that allow them to ignore this fact. Few studies measure both genetic and environmental risk factors in the same populations. With some significant exceptions, neuroscience and developmental psychopathology have not found a common meeting place.
The human mind is programmed to look for simple and direct relationships between cause and effect. Clinicians, like the rest of us, have trouble with complexities. What can be hard to grasp is that no single or simple risk factors for most forms of psychopathology have been identified. A theory based on interacting multiple risk factors, taking into account large numbers of genes as well as multiple interacting environmental stressors, is needed to address these problems.

INTRODUCTION

HUMANITY IS a single species. All of us share most of our 20,000+ genes and have the same body plan as well as similar mental patterns. These similarities are driven by natural selection. Yet at the same time, people are very different from each other. Any theory of mental disorders must account for both similarities and differences. What is the source of individual variability? Some of these differences are also shaped by natural selection. The genes that drive individual differences may be in the minority, but they have profound effects. Each reflects an adaptation to an historical or current environment or a range of traits than can cope with different environmental circumstances. Thus, humans differ in physical characteristics, such as height, facial features, and skin color. We are also mentally different—in our thoughts, our emotions, and our behaviors.
Physical differences have always been understood as related to genetic influence. However, mental differences have not always been seen in the same light. Over the centuries, philosophers and scientists have struggled with the nature-nurture problem. In other words, are we more a product of our genes or of our environment?
Even if the answer is “both,” this issue remains the center of intense controversy. For example, clinicians and researchers continue to debate the extent to which clinical depression derives from a chemical imbalance or whether lowered mood is largely a response to loss and disappointment. Similar debates have raged over the origins of other mental disorders, but as psychiatry moved back to its roots in medicine, it became more biological and tended to favor nature over nurture.
This dichotomy has played an important role in psychiatric practice. When I was a resident, nurture was often favored over nature. One of my teachers, a psychoanalyst who headed a hospital department, was sympathetic to attempts to cure schizophrenia with psychotherapy. When I suggested that this was not a reasonable goal for a disorder that was strongly genetic, he became angry, stating, “I will not see patients as incurable and let them go down the drain.” Years later, he changed his mind after reading a landmark study showing that psychotherapy is not effective for the management of acute psychoses (May 1968).
Psychotherapists who believe that everything problematic in life derives from childhood adversities and inadequate parenting tend to have a utopian vision of what mental health treatment can accomplish. This is why they, all too often, see patients for years, hoping to achieve the impossible. Biological psychiatrists are more likely to think that, until we learn how to rewrite the genome, there will always be constraints on how much people can change. Yet they too tend to chase impossible outcomes, usually by prescribing too many drugs. An interactive model can help us to come to terms with our limitations.
These clinical problems are also reflected in the research community, in which neuroscience ignores the environment while psychology ignores genetics. Future research needs to integrate nature and nurture and use measures of both in the same populations. Although that program is in a very early stage, psychiatrists of the future will eventually benefit from knowing more precisely how genes and environment interact to produce mental disorders.

THE ORIGINS OF THIS BOOK

This book has both intellectual and personal roots. One of my motivations in writing this second edition is to summarize what I have learned in 50 years as a psychiatrist. At the same time, I want to share with readers what I have had to unlearn. When I began my training in the 1960s, there was not much contact between strains of thought that favored nature or nurture. They ran in parallel without ever meeting. Like many North American residents at that time, I had psychoanalysts as teachers who believed that the causes of common mental disorders lie in an unhappy childhood. Their models were seductive because they offered an understanding of almost every form of psychopathology, but these ideas were inaccurate and simplistic.
At the same time, psychiatry was in the midst of a psychopharmacological revolution. In the most severe disorders, drugs were usually superior to psychotherapy. Moreover, drugs were starting to be applied to patients who were symptomatic but less impaired. I could not anticipate that millions of patients would be taking psychiatric drugs, sometimes for a lifetime. A new paradigm has replaced a biopsychosocial mode (Engel 1980) with what, among others, Bentall (2009) has called a “bio-bio-bio” model.
I have never disagreed with the principle that mental disorders are brain disorders. Of course they are! However, that does not mean that thoughts, behaviors, and emotions are best understood by reduction to a neuronal level. Moreover, a biopsychosocial model leads to different clinical implications. For a good number of patients, ranging from those with mild depression to those with a personality disorder, medication is of doubtful value and psychotherapy is the treatment of choice. For others, a combination of drugs and talking therapy is the best option.
When I began to practice psychiatry, I wanted to justify my years of training in medicine and treat patients who were sick enough to require the services of a physician. I was particularly interested and challenged by patients who were chronically suicidal. These patients were often avoided by nonmedical clinicians, and if, by the end of treatment, they decided to go on living, I would know that they had benefited. This is why I became interested in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. At the time, a psychodynamic model seemed to have the most to offer, and that is what I offered my patients in the early years of my career. Like most clinicians, I started with high hopes but achieved mixed results. Some patients did brilliantly—these are the cases clinicians like to write up and talk about. Other patients deteriorated in spite of my best efforts. Most showed middling levels of improvement.
I also took a psychodynamic perspective as a teacher of psychiatric residents. Whatever the presenting problems, I could come up with a plausible explanation based on the patient’s personal history. I later came to the conclusion that this way of thinking was specious, and I began to adopt a hardheaded approach based on empirical evidence. I came to consider myself a “born-again” proponent of evidence-based practice. I have wondered if I should undertake a “product recall” and inform residents from the 1970s to ignore my earlier views, but my students have almost certainly forgotten most of what I taught them.
In the course of a long career, my ideas about psychiatry have changed dramatically. For nearly 50 years, I have been in charge of an outpatient consultation clinic evaluating a large number of new patients referred from the community. I have now seen about 50,000 cases—approximately the population of a small town. Exposure to a very broad range of psychopathology helped me to appreciate the complexity of the pathways to mental illness. I benefit, at least in principle, from this experience. However, as this book argues, clinical experience is not a reliable guide to understanding causes. Like any scientific hypothesis, intuitive impressions need to be confirmed by systematic research.
I have been fortunate to have colleagues who helped me to develop a second career as a researcher. In spite of my late arrival on the scientific scene, beginning as a clinician-teacher had some advantages. My experiences led me to address the question as to why some patients develop one type of illness under adverse circumstances while other patients, faced with the same life events, develop a completely different type of illness, and still others develop no illness at all. My own research has focused on patients who do not benefit from standard treatments. This lack of response requires an explanation. Traditionally, clinicians believed that the intractability of personality disorders derived from their roots in early childhood. However, research does not confirm this view and shows that most people who experience early adversities never develop a mental disorder (Paris 2020). Life events alone do not account for the development of personality disorders or, for that matter, of most mental disorders.
Several lines of evidence, described in detail in this book, support this conclusion (Cicchetti 2016). Many patients with mental disorders have no history of childhood adversity. Patients with similar life experiences can develop completely different illnesses. Children experiencing severe adversities show high levels of resilience, and only a minority will develop diagnosable psychopathology in adulthood. Moreover, children raised in the same family and exposed to the same environment do not necessarily have the same outcome as adults. Finally, research in behavior genetics shows that about half of the variance affecting most mental disorders is genetic (Plomin 2018).
Psychopathology in any individual is not predictable, either from inborn temperament alone or from life experience by itself. The problem with current models in psychiatry is that they heavily tilt the scales, one way or the other, toward genes or environment. The biomedical model attempts to view psychopathology as entirely a function of abnormal neuroconnectivity or neurochemistry. In doing so, it radically oversimplifies the complexity of the human brain and mind. This model works best for psychoses, as well as neurodevelopmental disorders and neurocognitive disorders, but even there has limitations.
A purely psychological or psychosocial model of psychopathology is equally simplistic and misleading. When events in an individual’s life have been adverse, it may seem plausible to account for present difficulties by personal history. However, what clinicians do not always realize is that their patients are a highly selected subsample of those exposed to any given risk factor. When researchers go out into the community and interview nonclinical samples, they find that most people exposed to negative life events develop mild difficulties or none at all.
These facts are striking. To explain them, we need a different and more interactive theoretical model, one that integrates nature and nurture (Tabery 2014). This book describes how predisposing genetic factors particular to the individual, interacting with environmental stressors, can be the basis of a general model that can be applied to most categories of mental disorder. I apply these principles to most of the more common conditions that psychiatrists see.
I do not, however, focus on disorders in which the gene-environment model is not quite appropriate, particularly in conditions whose etiology is mainly biological. Thus, I do not discuss autism spectrum disorders, or neurocognitive disorders in which research shows that the environment plays only a minor role. Although schizophrenia is also highly genetic, I discuss research supporting a role for environmental risk factors. I also, much more briefly, describe the weaker evidence for environmental risk in bipolar disorders. I omit many other conditions listed in DSM manuals that are insufficiently researched to determine whether they fit the model.

BEYOND REDUCTIONISM

In recent years, my critique of modern psychiatry has focused on the idea of biological reductionism. The pendulum has swung so far that the effects of psychosocial environment are dismissed, or at best paid lip service. Reductionism in science has had great success but has great limitations. We cannot understand water by reducing it to the properties of hydrogen and oxygen. Complex phenomena have emergent properties that cannot be explained by their components (Gillett 2016).
Moreover, the pathways leading to mental disorders are not linear but involve an enormously complex set of interactions. The principle that disorders have multiple causes may seem a truism. Yet contemporary psychiatry honors it “more in the breach than in the observance.” Like other disciplines that face complexity, psychiatry has been susceptible to reductionistic theories. We are tempted to reduce cognitive dissonance and to look for simple ways to explain what we observe.
Psychiatry has suffered from what Snow (1958/1993) called the problem of “two cultures.” A biomedical mental health culture takes the world view that mental disorders are primarily the result of factors inside the person. A psychosocial culture takes the world view that mental disorders are primarily the result of factors outside the person. Both models tend to be reductionistic, applying unidimensional theories in which one risk factor causes one disorder. The historical background of these models is illuminating. Biological theories dominated psychiatry during the nineteenth century but went into decline in the first half of the twentieth century, largely because few brain abnormalities could be observed and because no effective biological treatments for mental disorders were available. The postwar years were the heyday of psychoanalysis and other psychological therapies. In the latter decades of the twentieth century, however, and even more in the twenty-first century, successful pharmacological treatment came of age, and brain science again came to dominate psychiatric theory. Even so, modern psychiatry still lacks a comprehensive biological theory (Harrington 2019).
These world views suffer from reductionism, even at the biological level. The effects of genes are extremely complicated, and hundreds of them can be involved in a single disorder. Similarly, the role of life events in psychopathology is complex, and one should not assume that single traumatic events cause mental disorders, a complexity that has sometimes led to the use of the term “environome” (Plomin 2018).
The theoretical models that psychiatrists use are not just a matter of intellectual interest. They profoundly affect what these psychiatrists do clinically. If a psychiatrist believes that mental disorders arise from single causes, he or she will tend to treat patients with single methods. In a biomedical model, clinicians search for aberrant neurotransmitter pathways that can be specifically targeted by carefully designed pharmacological agents. In an environmental model, clinicians may search for life events and psychological conflicts or assumptions that can be addressed through psychotherapy. To do justice to the complex causes of psychopathology, we need much more sophisticated explanations of the relationship between risks and disorders (Kendler 2019). Any comprehensive and clinically relevant approach must be multivariate. (Modern statistics reflect this approach, replacing t-tests and chi-squares with regression equations.)
The psychosocial risk factors for psychiatric illness are important, but we must be cautious in attributing pathology of patients to negative events that are common in human life. Because clinicians only see people who became disordered, they tend to develop a distorted view of the relationship between adversity and illness. In biologically vulnerable people, however, negative events have a stronger effect. For example, in PTSD, adverse experiences, unless they activate predispositions, usually cause only short-term, not long-term effects (Yehuda 2002). Many of the associations in the literature between psychosocial risks and mental disorders can be accounted for by predisposed subpopulations.
Thinking interactively helps us avoid assuming simple relationships between causes and effects. Instead, the model encourages us to address complex relationships between many causes and many effects. It also offers a nonreductionistic way of conceptualizing the origins of psychopathology. Unfortunately, reductionism is alive and well. Clinicians of the future need to become more comfortable with complexity. In the long run, the weight of scientific evidence can change the way we think.

THE PURPOSE OF THIS BOOK

My aim is to provide the reader with an intelligible summary of the gene-environment model and to illustrate its application to the understanding of mental disorders. I do not claim to present a new or an original theory. I am hardly the first author to advocate an interactive approach. For those who want to delve further into the details of this subject, I recommend Michael Rutter’s (2006) seminal volume Genes and Behavior.
In principle, gene-environment interaction theory is in the mainstream of contemporary psychiatry. The idea that diseases arise from interactions between constitution and experience can be traced as far back as the writings of Galen (Monroe and Simons 1991). However, as subdisciplines of psychiatry have become separate and have taken on a narrower vision, interactions have been downplayed. This is particularly true in clinical practice, where ideology too often trumps evidence.
A few words are also needed to explain what this book is not about. A great deal of research has been done concerning the precise mechanisms that mediate biological predispositions to mental illness. Yet in spite of all the progress made in recent years, we are still very far from defining how genes act on brain chemistry and connectivity. Therefore, although neuroscience is one of the “hottest” areas of research in psychiatry, this volume only deals with this subject peripherally. One can study genetic influences without knowing exactly how they affect neurotransmitters. Moreover, this is a fast-changing field, in which today’s breakthrough often becomes tomorrow’s blind alley. In the future, when these issues are better understood, we should be in a position to develop a model fully grounded in neurobiology.
In showing how the model can be usefully applied to a very wide range of psychopathology, I have had to review a very wide literature. Synthesizing this information requires making compromises between the needs of different readers. Specialists may find some sections lacking in detail. However, this book is primarily directed at practicing clinicians. Its aim is to show how the model should guide evidence-based practice. Most of the references in this book consist of research reports published in scientific journals, but it is impossible to review a complex literature without picking and choosing. Wherever my conclusions seem controversial, readers should pursue their own inquiries, making use of the extensive reference list provided at the end of this book.
In science, difficult problems are rarely settled by single studies. Only the overall weight of evidence allows firm conclusions. This is why I have referenced many of my conclusions with review articles or books, which are themselves summaries of the research literature. Wherever individual papers shed particular light on questions, I have highlighted them. But as I tell my students, no matter how impressive a research finding is, one needs to wait for a meta-analysis.

THE ARGUMENT OF THE CHAPTERS

Part I: Theory

The first five chapters provide an overview of theories affecting how we study genes, environment, and their interactions and how empirical evidence has shed light on all these issues. They also examine how the theory is transdiagnostic and goes beyond DSM definitions of mental disorders.
Chapter 1: Historical Overview
Over the past 20 years, psychiatry has become much more biological, and contemporary practice is dominated by the use of medication. I highlight studies showing that psychiatrists in practice make much less use of psychotherapy than they did in the past. I argue that the field has moved too radically from one extreme to the other and has failed to integrate nature and nurture, both in theory and practice. Interactive gene-environment models are needed to do justice to the current state of research.
Chapter 2: Genetic Predispositions
Chapter 2 reviews some of the advances in psychiatric genetics over the past two decades. These include such issues as the use of genome-wide association studies and the development of polygenic risk scores. I also review the problems of reductionism in genetics and neuroscience.
Chapter 3: Environmental Stressors
This chapter examines recent research on stress and resilience. Newer ideas that expand the predisposition-stress model, such as differential susceptibility to the environment, are considered. I review a large amount of major longitudinal research that has been carried out in the past two decades, such as the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study, the E-Risk Study, and the Children in the Community Study. These studies show that stressors cannot be understood without considered predispositions, but that environmental events can also make an independent contribution to risk.
Chapter 4: Gene-Environment Interactions
We now have much more evidence, both from behavior genetics and longitudinal research, to shed light on gene-environment interactive mechanisms. I review the various forms of interaction and examine the significance of the contribution of the new discipline of epigenetics.
Chapter 5: Disorders, Diagnoses, and Traits
Chapter 5 addresses some of the unsolved problems in psychiatric diagnosis. Given our current state of knowledge, DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association 2013) can only be considered as temporarily useful. Categorical diagnoses can be enriched by an understanding of underlying traits. However, dimensional alternatives (the personality disorder model in DSM-5, Section III, and of ICD-11 [World Health Organization 2019], as well as the Hierarchical Taxonomy of Psychopathology [HiTOP; Kotov et al. 2017] system for all mental disorders) seem to be somewhat premature. Along the same lines, I offer commentary on the Research Domain Criteria system (Cuthbert and Insel 2013), which attempts to use neural connectivity to account for psychopathology.

Part II: Mental Disorders

The next eight chapters review evidence on how genes and environment shape the most important mental disorders. I focus on those categories that are frequently seen in practice and that have been extensively researched.
Chapter 6: ADHD and Conduct Disorder
ADHD, often considered to be mainly biological, cannot be understood without considering how heritable temperament interacts with psychosocial adversities and social influences. Conduct disorder arises in childhood, indicating that it is associated with genetic vulnerability, as shown by a large literature.
Chapter 7: Schizophrenia
Psychoses such as schizophrenia are primarily biological in origin. Yet recent evidence shows that they can be triggered by reactions to psychosocial stressors, such as “social defeat” in immigrant populations, and are associated with adverse or traumatic childhood experiences.
Chapter 8: Depressive Disorders
Depressed mood should not be considered just a reflection of chemical imbalances or abnormal neuroconnectivity. Rather, mild to moderate depressions are intimately linked to life events that trigger predispositions, helping to account for the limitations of psychopharmacology in this domain. Chapter 8 also makes brief mention of bipolar disorder, but it does not fit the model, given that research has found it to be highly genetic.
Chapter 9: Anxiety Disorders and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
Research on panic disorder and generalized anxiety disorder shows that research on these disorders supports the model. Patients susceptible to anxiety have genetic-temperamental vulnerabilities that often date back to childhood. These tendencies can be amplified by adverse life experiences. OCD is another condition that fits the model. Although we now see this disorder as reflecting a problem in neurocircuitry, it can be triggered by environmental adversities.
Chapter 10: Posttraumatic Stress Disorder
PTSD is not just a response to stressful events but reflects components related to heritable predispositions and environmentally sensitive personality traits. There is also an important social component to the disorder.
Chapter 11: Eating Disorders
The eating disorders, which have greatly increased in prevalence, have been studied intensively over the past two decades. Research provides important support for a model in which heritable predispositions interact with psychological stressors as well as social forces.
Chapter 12: Substance-Related and Addictive Disorders
This group provides an example par excellence of how gene-environment interactions shape psychopathology. A large body of research from the past 20 years is reviewed and shown to support the model.
Chapter 13: Personality Disorders
Personality disorders are my own area of research, and I have a good deal more to say than I did 20 years ago. Gene-environment interactions are essential for understanding these disorders and the traits that underlie them. The development of efficacious specialized therapies for borderline personality disorder demonstrates that even when heritability is fairly strong, an interactive model tends to support specifically designed treatment methods.

Part III: Implications

The final section of the book examines implications of the general model for research and clinical practice.
Chapter 14: Clinical Implications
Psychiatric research has suffered from a failure to study predispositions and stressors in the same populations, effectively preventing investigators from examining their interactions. I provide examples of this problem, drawn from several domains of psychopathology.
Chapter 15: Implications for Prevention and Further Research
Clinical practice, whether psychopharmacological or psychotherapeutic, can benefit from the application of an integrative model. I argue for a model that makes use of both traditions. The chapter also reviews the prospects of prevention for mental disorders, the results of which have been so far discouraging. Here I examine programs for early intervention in mental disorders that begin in youth in which, however aware one is of predispositions and heritability, one can change the course and prognosis of severe mental disorders.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Lise Laporte read the entire manuscript and offered many useful suggestions for improvement. Laura Roberts, Erika Parker, and the American Psychiatric Association Publishing staff offered crucial help and support.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Nature and Nurture in Mental Disorders
Nature and Nurture in Mental Disorders: A Gene-Environment Model
Pages: i - xxi

History

Published in print: 6 October 2020
Published online: 5 December 2024
© American Psychiatric Association Publishing

Authors

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share