What Is Obesity?
Obesity refers to an excess of body fat. In the broadest sense, obesity results from an imbalance between energy intake and energy expenditure; that is, when an individual ingests more than the body expends, excess energy intake is stored in body fat cells that enlarge or increase in number. At present, there is no clear division between normal and abnormal levels of fat.
29 However, body mass index (BMI), a ratio of weight to height calculated by weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters, is widely utilized to define obesity operationally given its robust associations with adiposity (i.e., BMI <18.5, underweight; BMI 18.5–24.9, normal weight; BMI 25–29.9, overweight; BMI ≥30, obese)
30 and medical comorbidity.
The prevalence of obesity has been increasing worldwide, and the WHO has estimated that globally there are 300 million obese individuals.
31 In the United States, the prevalence of obesity in adults aged 20 years and older doubled between 1980 and 2002, whereas rates in children aged 6–19 years tripled.
32 In 2003–2004, 32.2% of US adults and 17.1% of US children and adolescents were obese.
32 The prevalence of obesity in the United States varies by racial/ethnic group in women (rates for Non-Hispanic White, Mexican American, and Non-Hispanic Black women are 30%, 42.3%, and 53.9%, respectively), but not men.
32 As documented below, the worldwide increase in rates of obesity will have profound health consequences.
Is Obesity Harmful?
There is little question that obesity is harmful for most individuals [but see Ref. 33]. The medical consequences of excess body fat are manifold and can be divided into those due to the added weight of the extra fat (e.g., osteoarthritis) and the metabolic changes associated with adiposity (e.g., diabetes).
34 Obesity is a risk factor for cardiovascular disease, diabetes, hypertension, kidney disease, obstructive sleep apnea, osteoarthritis, and several forms of cancer (i.e., colon, breast, esophageal, uterine, ovarian, kidney, and pancreatic).
35 The increased rate of mortality among obese persons, relative to lean individuals, is well-documented.
36Obesity also is associated with significant psychosocial impairment. Obese individuals are subject to weight-based stigmatization in a variety of settings,
37 and generally report poorer quality of life compared with lean individuals.
38 With respect to psychiatric correlates, epidemiologic studies have found positive associations between obesity and mood disorders, anxiety disorders, eating disorders, and personality disorders.
8–11,39 These relationships appear to be especially strong for women
40,41 and for individuals with more severe obesity (i.e., BMI ≥ 35),
8,10 although associations between excess body fat and psychiatric illness also have been documented in men
11 and in more moderately overweight individuals.
9 Finally, prospective longitudinal studies have shown that obesity predicts the onset of psychiatric symptoms
42–45 and vice versa.
46–48At a societal level, there are profound economic consequences associated with obesity in the form of direct medical costs and indirect costs (e.g., income lost from decreased productivity, worker absenteeism, restricted activity).
49 The health care costs related to obesity were estimated to account for 9.1% of US medical expenses in 1998,
50 and if current trends continue, obesity will account for 16% of US health care expenditures by 2030.
51 Studies conducted in European Union states also have documented that obesity-related health care accounts for a significant proportion of national health expenditures.
52 In consideration of the morbidity and mortality associated with obesity,
53 the Council of The Obesity Society recently concluded that obesity should be declared a disease.
54Is There Evidence That Obesity Is caused by Mental Dysfunction?
To consider whether obesity is caused by mental dysfunction, it is necessary first to provide a brief overview of current thinking about the etiology of obesity. Obesity research encompasses work in economics, sociology, epidemiology, nutrition, psychology, and biology, and a complete review is beyond the scope of this article. Nevertheless, there is consensus that the etiology of obesity is multifactorial such that a genetic diathesis interacts in an intricate fashion with individual and environmental factors that promote the expression of a heterogeneous condition. Moreover, there is agreement that given the primacy of nutrition for survival, humans have developed diverse and redundant mechanisms that favor energy intake. In the current US socioeconomic context, which is characterized by the availability of cheap, palatable (i.e., high in sugar and fat) food and a decrease in the amount of physical energy required to sustain survival, the multiple mechanisms that promote energy ingestion are no longer adaptive, but rather promote the development of obesity.
55There have been important advances in genetics and basic work serving to explicate the manifold central and peripheral signals that are involved in the regulation of energy balance in humans (for further information, the reader is referred to reviews of the biology of obesity
56 and to the extensive literature that details neural networks
57–59 and adipose tissue functions
60,61). Research from family history and twin studies has documented that 40%–70% of the variance in obesity can be accounted for by genetic factors.
62 The identification of several monogenic mutations that cause obesity has helped elucidate etiologic pathways associated with genetic obesity syndromes
63; however, these monogenic disorders account only for a small fraction of cases with early onset obesity. The preponderance of evidence indicates that a larger number of genes with small effects, but which occur commonly in the general population, explain most weight variation in humans.
64,65 The introduction of genome-wide association studies has promoted the identification of genes associated with ordinary obesity
66; for example, common variants in the fat mass and obesity-associate (FTO) gene are related to BMI and hip circumference.
67,68 Nevertheless, the impact of these common genetic variants probably will be in influencing individual responses to environmental variables such as type of diet.
64 Thus, risk may ultimately be understood best by explicating gene environment interactions that impact appetite, satiety, activity levels, and the biology of fat storage.
Conceptualizations of the complex circuitry that comprises the internal system that regulates energy balance have advanced significantly. Central factors include homeostatic mechanisms in the hypothalamus and brainstem, reward circuitry in limbic and paralimbic structures, and cognitive mechanisms in the prefrontal and association cortices, which interact with peripheral signals from the gut, pancreas, liver, adipose tissue, and muscle.
69 Although specific examples of central factors that may relate to potential psychiatric aspects of obesity are considered below, it is important to acknowledge the diversity and redundancy of signals involved in energy balance. The caudal brainstem receives nutrition information from the taste buds and gut and controls the machinery involved in food ingestion. The hypothalamus is the major site for integration of nutrient balance information and coordination of adaptive responding. Cortico-limbic pathways are involved in the interaction with the environment in the procurement and consumption of food.
Finally, it may be useful to think of the etiology of obesity as involving more broadly conceived host and environmental factors.
70 Additional host factors may include intrauterine development,
71 gut microbiota,
72 and sleep.
71 Environmental factors include the availability of inexpensive, palatable foods and decreases in physical activity. Recent research also has considered an array of novel environmental factors that may contribute to increasing rates of obesity including viruses,
73 toxins,
74 drugs,
70 and stress.
55In summary, regulation of energy intake is crucial to human survival and consequently is protected by numerous redundant and overlapping central and peripheral processes. Clinical obesity reflects alterations in remarkably complex internal and environmental milieus that combine to form multiple pathways that result in obesity. Given the multiplicity of factors implicated in the etiology of obesity and the diversity of the resulting phenotypes, there is little evidence that supports a conclusion that obesity is a mental disorder. In fact, epidemiological observers have concluded that changes in prevalence rates of obesity may be largely due to modest changes in calorie intake and energy expenditure leading to a shift in population weights.
75 For example, if a person’s daily energy intake exceeded expenditure by 100 kcal (e.g., a medium apple), the excess calories would lead to a weight gain of more than 10 pounds in a year.
76Homeostatic Eating, Stress, and Obesity
The notion that stress-related eating contributes to the development of obesity is not new,
81 and there is a diverse body of literature that provides indirect support for this idea. For purposes of this review, we consider evidence that the effects of chronic stress on central functioning may be a driver of human obesity. Observers have hypothesized that the combination of ongoing stress and the wide availability of inexpensive, highly palatable foods promote the development of overeating and obesity, in particular, abdominal obesity, in vulnerable individuals. Specifically, some people may overeat to modulate activity of the chronic stress network and reduce negative affect.
82 Thus, although the central mechanisms to cope with stress may be operating as designed, there may be a mismatch between these stress-response mechanisms and the current environment. Here we briefly examine evidence that stress effects on central functioning provide a plausible pathway that contributes to the development of obesity. Almost all available evidence is suggestive, rather than confirmatory, and the human literature is rife with methodological difficulties (in particular, the operationalization of chronic stress, which varies widely across studies). Nevertheless, selected findings from the animal literature and human studies linking stress and obesity are considered in turn.
There is an extensive animal literature that documents that the HPA axis is a crucial mediator of the vertebrate stress response system, which serves to maintain homeostasis and coordinate the organism’s behavioral responses to internal and external threats.
83 Findings from this work indicate that although increased secretion of glucocorticoids (GCs) during acute stress inhibits HPA activity and decreases eating, the chronic effects of GCs are excitatory and associated with increased food consumption.
82 High concentrations of GCs increase expression of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) in the HPA to enable the chronic stress network. Elevated GCs in the presence of insulin drive the intake of highly palatable foods, which mitigates unpleasant affect consequent to stress in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex.
84 Finally, elevated GCs promote the development of abdominal fat depots.
82 Animal studies that have manipulated GCs and insulin have documented that the joint presence of high cortisol and high insulin drives preference for fat intake.
85 Thus, animal data provide suggestive evidence that stress-induced eating occurs in animals and a similar phenomenon may occur in humans.
Numerous human studies have examined the impact of stress on eating. Greeno and Wing,
86 in a widely cited review, presented evidence for individual differences in stress-induced eating, and concluded that restrained eating predicts stress-induced eating in women. However, they documented the absence of direct tests of the hypothesis that stress leads to weight gain or the development of obesity. Epel et al. in a series of studies (see Ref. 87 for review), documented that individuals who responded to laboratory stressors with high levels of cortisol were likelier than low cortisol responders to ingest more calories after the stressor. In a subsequent field study, they demonstrated that daily stressors were related to greater intake of snack foods among high cortisol responders.
88 However, if stress-related eating drives human obesity one would expect differences between obese and lean individuals in stress-related eating. Torres and Nowson
89 reviewed several early studies comparing obese and lean individuals. Only one investigation
90 documented increased calorie intake in response to stress among obese compared with lean persons.
Several prospective studies have examined whether stress is associated with weight gain over time. For example, Van Strien et al.
91 examined the hypothesis that low emotional eaters would gain less weight in response to negative life events than high emotional eaters. Only male emotional eaters who experienced several negative life events exhibited greater increases in BMI over a 6-month period. In a study of more than 5,000 Finnish twin pairs, Korkeila et al.
92 examined the effects of baseline stress on the odds of substantial weight gain (more than 10 kg) at 6- and 15-year assessments. High levels of stress at baseline predicted major weight gain in men whereas neuroticism and low life satisfaction predicted weight gain in women. These effects were observed at 6 years, but were attenuated at 15 years. Finally, data from the Whitehall II study, which included almost 8,000 male and female British workers, were utilized to evaluate whether work stress had a differential effect on weight gain among lean and overweight individuals.
93 Results indicated that work stress was associated with subsequent weight gain among overweight and obese men, and weight loss among lean men. These bi-directional associations were not observed in women.
In another line of research, Bjorntorp and colleagues
94–96 have hypothesized that psychosocial stress may lead to chronic arousal of the HPA axis and increased cortisol secretion, which in turn promote increased insulin resistance, disturbed lipid and glucose metabolism, and accumulation of visceral fat. The approach taken in this work has been to identify psychosocial or socioeconomic handicaps and then to document increased cortisol secretion or HPA perturbations, and visceral and overall adiposity (see Ref. 97 and 98 for reviews). A review of this literature
97 concluded that although most of the available data are circumstantial, there is evidence that the HPA and other central regulatory systems are involved in the development of obesity.
Taken collectively, there is suggestive evidence that there may be a sub-group of stress-responsive obese individuals with a diathesis to overeat palatable food and gain weight over time. However, there is scant evidence that such obesity-related overeating should be considered a mental disorder. Rather, it probably is most useful to consider whether obese individuals with frankly aberrant eating patterns, such as those with binge eating disorder, differ in stress-responsivity and associated overeating from obese individuals without aberrant eating. Although early work comparing obese binge eaters to obese nonbinge eaters on dexamethasone suppression found little support for this hypothesis,
99,100 studies using neuroimaging or other more recently developed laboratory paradigms may shed additional light on the associations among stress-responsivity, obesity, and aberrant eating.
Nonhomeostatic Eating, Reward, and Obesity
Several recent models have emphasized the role of the neural reward system in the development and maintenance of obesity. Specifically, this research hypothesizes that dysfunction of brain reward circuitry in response to food cues may predispose some individuals to obesity via an increased likelihood of overeating, particularly excessive consumption of palatable foods. For example, as noted in Devlin’s
101 review of the potential role of obesity in DSM-V, the notion that some human obesity may result from food “addiction” has gained increased credence. Numerous observers have documented parallels between addictive behaviors and particular forms of overeating,
102,103 and more generally have conceptualized addiction as a process that occurs when any habitual behavior (e.g., eating, gambling) co-opts brain reward circuitry.
104 Work by Volkow and colleagues
12,14,105,106 has explicated the similarities between drug addiction and obesity, such as decreased levels of striatal dopamine D
2 receptors, and suggested that obesity, like drug addiction, is linked with exposure to and reward from powerful reinforcers (i.e., food).
Current conceptualizations of food hedonics or reward distinguish between wanting, i.e., the desire for or motivation to consume foods and liking, i.e., the degree of pleasure derived from food consumption (see Ref. 107 for review). In terms of wanting food, obese individuals may differentially seek food or respond in a heightened way to food cues. In terms of liking food, obese individuals may have a heightened hedonic response to food, and thus overeat. Conversely, obese individuals may have less pleasure from food, and consequently eat more highly palatable food to optimize pleasure. Neurobiological models also emphasize the role of learning in reward, noting that cognitive processes interact with motivational and affective mechanisms to influence reward.
108 A comprehensive examination of the research literature that examines food reward is beyond the scope of this article. Much of this work has focused on animals (see Refs. 109–112 for review), but there are several lines of research in human subjects that provide at least indirect support for the proposition that differences in reward neurocircuitry are involved in overeating associated with obesity.
For example, studies have examined human differences in sensitivity to reward (STR), a trait thought to be linked to the mesolimbic dopamine pathway. Davis et al.
113 measured STR by questionnaire and found that it was associated with emotional overeating in response to depressed mood and BMI in women. Similarly, research using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has shown that healthy volunteers who self-report higher STR demonstrate greater activation in the frontal-striatal-amygdala-midbrain network in response to appetizing foods.
114 These findings are suggestive of an association between reward sensitivity and eating behavior. However, investigations that rely on self-report of reward sensitivity or that do not include comparisons of obese and lean individuals do not provide direct evidence that obese persons differ from their normal weight counterparts in hedonic responses to food.
There is a burgeoning literature comparing obese and lean individuals on measures of neural activation in response to food stimuli or neural correlates of food reward using neuroimaging techniques [i.e., positron emission tomography (PET), MRI, fMRI]. As reviewed in Supporting Information Table S1 (see Supporting Information on Wiley-Interscience), in general, this work suggests that relative to lean individuals, obese persons have increased activation to anticipated food reward (i.e., increased wanting) and decreased activation to food consumption (i.e., decreased liking), although specific findings have varied across studies.
Several reports have shown that exposure to palatable food stimuli, which may activate processes related to food wanting, is associated with greater neural activation among obese compared with lean individuals. For example, Karhunen et al.
115 found that obese women had increased activation in the right parietal and temporal cortices relative to normal-weight women in response to viewing, but not eating, a heated meal. Similarly, Rothemund et al.
116 reported that obese women had greater activation in the dorsal striatum, anterior insula, hippocampus, and parietal lobule than did lean women in response to viewing pictures of high-calorie foods. Stoeckel et al.
117 also found evidence of increased activation in the dorsal striatum, insula, and hippocampus, as well several other brain regions, in obese women relative to normal-weight controls while viewing high-calorie food images. Taken together, these findings provide support for the notion that obese individuals experience increased neural reactivity to palatable food cues, which may suggest the presence of heightened motivation for eating in this group. However, the results from one recent study suggest that these differences may be specific to obese individuals with comorbid binge eating,
118 which raises questions about the extent to which increased neural activation to food cues among obese persons is explained by higher rates of eating pathology in this group.
Research also has found differences between obese and lean individuals with respect to neural activation in response to tasting and consuming food. For example, in a series of studies using PET scans and MRI to examine neural responses to food reward in the context of extreme hunger (i.e., following a 36-h fast), Gautier, Del Parigi and colleagues
119–122 documented that obese individuals exhibit increased activation in the prefrontal cortex (but see Ref. 122) and frontal operculum and decreased activation in the hippocampus, amygdala, cingulate cortex (but see Ref. 120), caudate nucleus, and putamen relative to lean controls in response to consuming a satiating liquid meal (i.e., 50% of estimated daily expenditure in the form of Ensure). Furthermore, in response to tasting a small quantity (i.e., 2 mL) of the liquid meal prior to full consumption, obese individuals showed increased activation in the insular cortex and midbrain compared with lean controls.
119,123 Of note, insular activity in response to anticipated food reward (i.e., a satiating meal in the context of extreme hunger) remained present in formerly obese individuals
119 and was correlated with disinhibition.
123Recent work has distinguished the neural correlates of anticipatory and consummatory food reward in obese and lean individuals. Stice et al.
124 found that obese relative to lean adolescent girls exhibited greater activation in the insular cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and somatosensory cortex (i.e., Rolandic operculum, temporal operculum, parietal operculum) in response to cues signaling impending delivery of palatable food. In response to receipt of the palatable food, obese adolescents showed increased activation in the Rolandic operculum and left-frontal operculum compared to lean controls. Moreover, continuous analyses found a negative association between BMI and activity in the caudate nucleus in response to consummatory food reward, which is consistent with research indicating that obese individuals have reduced striatal dopamine receptor availability relative to lean persons and thus may experience diminished reward from eating.
13,105 Indeed, a follow-up study documented that presence of the
TaqIA A1 allele, which is associated with reduced dopamine D
2 receptor gene binding in the striatum, moderated the relation between blunted striatal activation to consummatory food reward and BMI currently and at 1-year follow-up in adolescent and adult females
125 (Prospective analyses conducted only in adolescent females—see Supporting Information Table S1).
In summary, there is accumulating evidence that obese individuals differ from lean controls with respect to neural correlates of anticipated food reward and food consumption. These findings may suggest that mental mechanisms related to reward processing play a role in the onset and maintenance of obesity. However, as noted in Supporting Information Table S1, a number of questions remain unanswered regarding the presence of mental dysfunction related to reward circuitry in obese persons. First, although studies have documented differences between obese and lean individuals in neural responses to food stimuli, no research to date has provided conclusive evidence that these differences represent dysfunction, i.e., “the inability of some mental mechanism to perform its natural function” (p. 385).
17 Indeed, it is entirely possible that observed differences reflect normal variation in reward sensitivity that historically conferred an adaptive advantage, but no longer is adaptive in an obesogenic environment. Second, with the exception of one recent study
125 that documented that decreased striatal activation to consummatory food reward is associated with increases in BMI among adolescent females with the
TaqIA A1 allele, no research has provided evidence that differences in neural activation in response to food reward are associated with the onset or maintenance of obesity. It seems equally plausible that observed differences may be a consequence or correlate of obesity. Third, although many investigators have hypothesized that activation of reward circuitry is related to overeating among obese individuals, no study has provided direct evidence that differences between obese and lean persons with respect to neural correlates of food reward are associated with in vivo eating behavior. Finally, studies focusing on reward processing in obese versus lean individuals typically have made little or no distinction for other behavioral or physiologic phenotypes that might refine our understanding of differences between these groups.
1 “Obese” and “lean” persons represent large and heterogeneous segments of the population. Moreover, as noted above, at least one report has suggested that differences in neural activation between obese and lean women in response to palatable food cues may be due to higher rates of eating-related psychopathology among obese persons.
118 If replicated, this work might suggest that mental dysfunction, if present among obese individuals, is limited to those with coexisting eating pathology.