Skip to main content

Abstract

Importance:

Major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder, and anxiety disorders are highly comorbid and have shared clinical features. It is not yet known whether their clinical overlap is reflected at the neurobiological level.

Objective:

To detect transdiagnostic convergence in ab‐normalities in task-related brain activation.

Data Source:

Task-related functional magnetic resonance imaging articles published in PubMed, Web of Science, and Google Scholar during the last decade comparing control individuals with patients with mood, posttraumatic stress, and anxiety disorders were examined.

Study Selection:

Following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses reporting guidelines, articles were selected if they reported stereotactic coordinates of whole-brain–based activation differences between adult patients and control individuals.

Data Extraction and Synthesis:

Coordinates of case-control differences coded by diagnosis and by cognitive domain based on the research domain criteria were analyzed using activation likelihood estimation.

Main Outcomes and Measures:

Identification of transdiagnostic clusters of aberrant activation and quantification of the contribution of diagnosis and cognitive domain to each cluster.

Results:

A total of 367 experiments (major depressive disorder, 149; bipolar disorder, 103; posttraumatic stress disorder, 55; and anxiety disorders, 60) were included comprising observations from 4507 patients and 4755 control individuals. Three right-sided clusters of hypoactivation were identified centered in the inferior prefrontal cortex/insula (volume, 2120 mm3), the inferior parietal lobule (volume, 1224 mm3), and the putamen (volume, 888 mm3); diagnostic differences were noted only in the putamen (χ3 2 = 8.66; P = .03), where hypoactivation was more likely in bipolar disorder (percentage contribution = 72.17%). Tasks associated with cognitive systems made the largest contribution to each cluster (percentage contributions >29%). Clusters of hyperactivation could only be detected using a less stringent threshold. These were centered in the perigenual/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (volume, 2208 mm3), the left amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus (volume, 2008 mm3), and the left thalamus (volume, 1904 mm3). No diagnostic differences were observed (χ3 2 < 3.06; P > .38), while tasks associated with negative valence systems made the largest contribution to each cluster (percentage contributions >49%). All findings were robust to the moderator effects of age, sex, and magnetic field strength of the scanner and medication.

Conclusions and Relevance:

In mood disorders, posttraumatic stress disorder, and anxiety disorders, the most consistent transdiagnostic abnormalities in task-related brain activity converge in regions that are primarily associated with inhibitory control and salience processing. Targeting these shared neural phenotypes could potentially mitigate the risk of affective morbidity in the general population and improve outcomes in clinical populations.
(Appeared originally in JAMA Psychiatry 2020;77(2):172-179)
Mood disorders (major depressive disorder and bipolar disorder), posttraumatic stress disorder, and anxiety disorders (generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, agoraphobia, and specific and social phobia) are highly comorbid (1) and collectively account for more than 65% of nonfatal disease burden attributable to psychiatric disorders (2). Up to 90% of patients with an anxiety disorder meet criteria for a concurrent mood disorder (2, 3), and as many as 70% of individuals with mood disorders meet criteria for an anxiety disorder during their lifetime (4, 5). Negative affective states are shared and central clinical features of these disorders (6), including bipolar disorder, where depressive symptoms are the dominant psychopathology (7).
Meta-analyses of brain imaging studies on mood, posttraumatic stress, and anxiety disorders have shown that each of these disorders is associated with abnormalities in task-related brain engagement (summarized in eTable 1 in the Supplement). The findings of these diagnosis-specific meta-analyses show conspicuous divergence (eTable 1 in the Supplement) that has been attributed to low numbers of contributing studies, reporting bias from region-of-interest (ROI) analyses, and inadequate correction for multiple comparisons (8, 9). Of note, methodological improvements over time have led to a progressive reduction in the number clusters of case-control differences reported in diagnosis-specific meta-analyses (eFigure 1 in the Supplement). Using data from task-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies published in the last 15 years, we demonstrated that diagnostic differences in the brain regions implicated in mood and anxiety disorders largely reflected the association with ROI analyses (9). By contrast, when only whole-brain analyses were considered, there were large pairwise correlations between the diagnosis-specific profiles (ρ range, 0.79-0.82; all P < .001) (9).
Here, we extend this line of research in 2 distinct ways. First, we sought to identify brain regions where aberrant task-related activation was most likely to show transdiagnostic convergence across major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, and anxiety and posttraumatic stress disorders. To achieve this, we capitalized on activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analytic tools (10-13) to synthesize coordinates of case-control differences in what is, to our knowledge, the largest sample of fMRI articles comprising the body of the relevant literature over the last 15 years. Second, we anchored the analysis plan to the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework (14) proposed by the US National Institute of Mental Health. The RDoC framework is the best approximation to a criterion approach to the classification of the array of activation tasks used in the primary studies and enables a principled interpretation of results in terms of dysfunction in clearly defined cognitive processes. Based on current neurobiological (15, 16), we predicted that transdiagnostic clusters of aberrant brain activity would converge in regions within the prefrontal, insular, and anterior cingulate cortex and in subcortical regions (particularly the amygdala/hippocampus and striatum) that support the adaptive regulation of cognition and affect.

BOX 1. Key Points

Question Is the clinical overlap seen in major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, anxiety disorders, and posttraumatic stress disorder reflected at the neurobiological level?
Findings In this meta-analysis of 226 task-related functional imaging studies, transdiagnostic clusters of hypoactivation were identified in the inferior prefrontal cortex/insula, inferior parietal lobule, and putamen.
Meaning Across mood and anxiety disorders, the most consistent transdiagnostic abnormalities in task-related brain activity converge in regions that are primarily associated with inhibitory control and salience processing.

Method

Literature Search and Article Eligibility

We applied the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses criteria (http://www.prismastatement.org/) to identify articles that used whole-brain analyses of task-related fMRI to compare healthy adults with adult patients who received a diagnosis of major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, agoraphobia, specific and social phobias, and posttraumatic stress disorder (details of the search and article eligibility criteria in the eMethods and eFigure 2 in the Supplement). Because we used data from published studies, no institutional review board approval was sought and patient consent was not obtained.

Database Construction

We use the term article to denote the published manuscript and the term experiment to denote the coordinates of case-control differences reported in each article. Accordingly, from each article, we extracted coordinates of case-control differences derived from whole-brain analyses only. These were then coded according to the strength of the magnetic field of the scanner, the diagnostic classification system, symptom severity, the direction of change in brain activity in patients compared with healthy individuals (hypoactivation or hyperactivation), and the corresponding RDoC domain and construct. The coding of tasks according to their corresponding RDoC domain and construct is described in the eMethods and shown in eTable 2 in the Supplement. For example, tasks such as the n-back and the Sternberg were assigned to the construct of working memory and the domain of cognitive systems where as various facial affect processing tasks were assigned to the construct of social communication and the domain of social processes. This allowed us to create 3 groupings of tasks labeled by their type (eg, facial affect processing) and their RDoC construct and domain. For each article, the symptom severity of the clinical sample was coded based on the mean psychopathology rating reported. To accommodate the use of different instruments across studies and clinical populations, symptom levels were labeled as minimal/mild, moderate, or severe (details in eMethods in the Supplement). Furthermore, for each experiment, but separately for patient and control groups, we coded their diagnostic status, sample size, age, and sex (percentage of men). In patients, medication status was coded as the percentage of patients receiving any psychotropic medication in each study sample. Further details of the database construction are provided in the eMethods in the Supplement.

Activation Likelihood Estimation

We used ALE, implemented in MATLAB (MathWorks), to test whether the whole-brain coordinates of case-control differences across experiments and disorders converged into discrete clusters with a nonrandom spatial distribution (10-13). The fundamental assumption of the ALE is that each voxel has the same a priori chance of differentiating patients from control individuals (null hypothesis). Consequently, ROI analyses were excluded because they violate this assumption and their inclusion would artificially bias results in favor of voxels within these regions. The main outcome of the ALE analysis are the clusters (ie, grouping of brain regions) in which the coordinates of the experiments converge. Per best-practice standards (10-13) statistically significant clusters were identified using a cluster-level family wise error–corrected threshold of P less than .05 (cluster-forming threshold at voxel-level P < .001). Additionally, for each suprathreshold cluster, we extracted the per voxel probability of functional change from the modeled activation maps. These values represent the probability of identifying a functional change for a mean voxel within the clusters derived from the modeled activation maps. Details of the procedures involved are described in the eMethods in the Supplement.
We analyzed coordinates of hypoactivation or hyperactivation in patients compared with healthy individuals separately to enhance interpretability. First, we identified suprathreshold clusters of hypoactivation and hyperactivation by pooling coordinates from all diagnoses and tasks and then conducted follow-up analyses to identify the effect of moderators. For the follow-up analyses, we extracted per-voxel probabilities of functional change for each cluster and conducted nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis tests and Spearman correlations to calculate the contribution of age, sex, RDoC domain/construct, diagnosis, symptom severity, and medication.
In generating the modeled activation maps, we pooled coordinates across diagnoses for 2 reasons. First, the disorders considered here are highly comorbid and hence the pooled analyses accommodate uncertainty about their symptomatic and syndromal boundaries. Moreover, comorbidity is not always reported in primary studies and therefore it is difficult to estimate its prevalence in the samples examined and its potential contribution to the neuro imaging results. Second, pooling results across diagnoses balances power, specificity, and sensitivity and allows for a data-driven quantification of the diagnosis-specific contribution to each suprathreshold cluster. We conducted supplemental diagnosis-specific analyses, which are presented in the eMethods and eResults in the Supplement.
In generating the modeled activation maps, we pooled coordinates from all the tasks used in the primary experiments based on 2 considerations. First, traditional neuropsychologic formulations tend to consider cognitive tasks as relatively specific to a particular process. Advances in cognitive and affective neuroscience have led to the recognition that the association between brain structure and function is pluripotent (one-to-many) and degenerate (many-to-one) (17, 18). Therefore, any given task engages brain regions outside those predicted by the cognitive mechanisms attributed to that particular task, while a single brain area may be activated by disparate tasks that may not share cognitive components (17, 18). Our approach accommodates pluripotency and offers a more realistic representation of the relevance of cognitive domains to case-control differences. Following the identification of suprathreshold clusters from the pooled analyses, we estimated the contribution of tasks to each cluster. For these follow-up analyses, tasks were grouped according to their assigned RDoC domain/construct; the use of the RDoC frame-work provided an organizing principle for the multitude of tasks used in the primary studies. Compared with other classifications that are primarily driven by convention, the RDoC framework has a clearly defined origin and rationale (14).
Finally, we used an alternate meta-analytic algorithm to confirm the reproducibility of the results of the main analyses and conducted several ancillary meta-analyses focusing on each diagnosis separately and using alternate classification of tasks (described in the eMethods, eResults, and eTable 8 in the Supplement).

Results

Samples and Experiments

In total, 226 articles were selected (major depressive disorder, 83; bipolar disorder, 66; posttraumatic stress disorder, 35; generalized anxiety disorder, 6; panic disorder and agoraphobia, 6; specific phobias, 8; and social phobia, 22) comprising observations from 4507 patients and 4755 healthy individuals. Full citations and details of the selected articles are provided in eTables 3 to 5 in the Supplement. Given the small number of studies on generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, agoraphobia, and specific and social phobias, we used a single coding of “anxiety disorders” for experiments arising from these patient groups. The selected articles yielded a total of 367 experiments (major depressive disorder, 149; bipolar disorder, 103; posttraumatic stress disorder, 55; and anxiety disorders, 60) (Table). The percentage of patients receiving medication differed by diagnosis (χ2 3 = 77.03; P < .001), being higher for bipolar disorder and major depressive disorder followed by posttraumatic stress disorder and anxiety disorders (eTable 6 in the Supplement). There were no statistical differences in the number of experiments per diagnosis (F 3 = 2.54; P = .10) or per RDoC domain (F 4 = 0.60; P = .66) and no significant case-control differences in age or sex.
TABLE. Experiments and Samples Included in the Databasea
  PatientsHealthy Individuals
DiagnosisExperiments, Total No.Sample, No.Age, Mean (SD), yMen, Mean (SD), %Sample, No.Age, Mean (SD), yMen, Mean (SD), %
MDD149b165636.2 (9.85)41 (17)175933.7 (9.41)43 (15)
BD103c148637.9 (10.52)48 (21)164236.4 (10.26)47 (19)
PTSD55d55735.0 (8.65)44 (40)57434.5 (8.25)43 (40)
ANX60e80829.6 (7.4)38 (23)78028.96 (7.07)39 (23)
Abbreviations: ANX, anxiety disorders; BD, bipolar disorder; CD, cross domain; CS, cognitive systems; MDD, major depressive disorders; NVS, negative valence systems; PVS, positive valence systems; PTSD, posttraumatic stress disorder; RDoC, research domain criteria; SP, social processes.
a
 There were no experiments that could be mapped to the domain of arousal. Experiment indicates set of coordinates of case-control differences originating from specific task contrasts; some published articles contributed more than 1 experiment (details in the eMethods and eTables 2-5 in the Supplement).
b
 Of the 149 MDD experiments, CS was studied in 27; NVS, 41; PVS, 43; SP, 17; and CD, 21.
c
 Of the 103 BD experiments, CS was studied in 49; NVS, 20; PVS, 11; SP, 7; and CD, 16.
d
 Of the 55 PTSD experiments, CS was studied in 13; NVS, 12; PVS, 1; SP, 14; and CD, 14.
e
 Of the 60 ANX experiments, CS was studied in 2; NVS, 20; PVS, 1; SP, 15; and CD, 22.

Activation Likelihood Estimation

Coordinates of hypoactivation (179 experiments) or hyperactivation (188 experiments) in patients compared with healthy individuals were entered in separate meta-analyses. Each meta-analysis had more than 80% power to detect clusters of brain regions if they showed convergent case-control differences in at least 10 experiments (10-13). Robust estimation of moderator effects was possible for age, sex, strength of the magnetic field of the scanner, symptom severity, and medication status. The peak coordinates of the supra threshold clusters are presented in Talairach space.

Transdiagnostic clusters of hypoactivation in patients.

We identified 3 reproducible (eFigure 4 in the Supplement) transdiagnostic clusters of hypoactivation in patients compared with healthy individuals centered on the right inferior prefrontal cortex/insula (peak coordinates: x = 40, y = 30, z = −10; volume, 2120 mm3), the right inferior parietal lobule (peak coordinates: x = 38, y = −48, z = 46; volume, 1224 mm3), and the right putamen (peak coordinates: x = 24, y = 8, z = −6; volume, 888 mm3) (Figure 1A; eFigure 3A in the Supplement). The effects of the moderator variables, including medication status and symptom severity, were not significant for any cluster (eResults in the Supplement).
FIGURE 1. Transdiagnostic Clusters of Hypoactivation in Patients Relative to Healthy Individuals
A color version of the figure, as originally published, appears in the online version of this article (focus.psychiatryonline.org).
A, Activation likelihood estimation identified 3 transdiagnostic clusters of relative hypoactivation in patients centered on right inferior prefrontal cortex/insula (IFG/INS), the right inferior parietal lobule (IPL), and the right putamen.
B, Percentage contribution of each research domain criteria (RDoC) to each cluster. Additional details are in eTable 7 in the Supplement.
There was no significant association of diagnosis with the prefrontal/insula (χ2 3 = 6.22; P = .10) and inferior parietal clusters (χ2 3 = 3.54; P = .31); an association of diagnosis was noted for the putamen (χ2 3 = 8.66; P = .03), for which the contribution of bipolar disorder (72.17%) was greater than that of major depressive disorder (17.35%; z 3 = 2.28; P = .02), posttraumatic stress disorder (4.55%; z 3 = 1.82; P = .06) and anxiety disorders (5.93%; z 3 = 2.07; P = .03); all other pairwise comparisons were not significant.
Differences in the contribution of RDoC domains/constructs did not reach statistical significance for any cluster, although processes associated with cognitive systems (Figure 1B), and particularly the construct of cognitive control, made the largest contribution to each cluster (eTable 7 in the Supplement). Of note, hypoactivation in patients in the right inferior front algyrus/insula was also identified in anancillary meta-analysis restricted only to tasks that involve affective (ie, acute or potential threat, reward attainment, approach motivation, and frustrative nonreward) and social (ie, social communication and perception of threat) processing (eResults in the Supplement), thus confirming the importance of this cluster across multiple domains of cognition. No additional diagnosis-specific clusters were identified (eResults in the Supplement).

Transdiagnostic clusters of hyperactivation in patients.

Despite adequate power, there were no statistically significant clusters of hyperactivation in patients compared with healthy individuals at cluster-level familywise error–corrected threshold of P less than .05 (cluster-forming threshold at voxel-level P < .001). At the same threshold, no suprathreshold clusters were detected when we repeated the analyses including only those experiments involving affective and social processing (eResults in the Supplement). No diagnosis-specific clusters were identified either (eResults in the Supplement). This was unexpected given that current models emphasize hyperactivation, primarily during the processing of emotionally valenced stimuli, in the patient populations considered here (15, 16).
Clusters of hyperactivation could only be detected using uncorrected voxel-level thresholding (P < .01) combined with an extent threshold of greater than 200 mm3. This level of statistical inference increases sensitivity at the cost of consistency because it magnifies contributions originating from only a few studies; nevertheless, it can still be considered acceptable if more than 20 experiments are modeled, as is the case here (10-13). The clusters thus identified were centered in the left amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus (peak coordinates: x = −22, y = −2, z = −15; volume, 2208 mm3), the left thalamus (peak coordinates: x = −2, y = −12, z = 4; volume, 2008 mm3) and the perigenual/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (peak coordinates: x = 0, y = 34, z = 12; volume, 1904 mm3) (Figure 2A; eFigure 3B in the Supplement). For the latter cluster, there was a negative correlation with the percentage of men (ρ = −0.68; P = .004), but no other moderator effect (including medication and symptom severity) was significant for this or the other clusters (details in the eResults in the Supplement). There was no significant difference in the degree to which each diagnosis contributed to the left amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus (χ2 3 = 2.13; P = .54), the left thalamus (χ2 3 = 1.26; P = .73), or the perigenual/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (χ2 3 = 3.06; P = .38). Differences in the contribution of RDoC domains did not reach statistical significance for any cluster, although experiments associated with negative valence systems (Figure 2B), and particularly the construct of acute threat (eTable 7 in the Supplement), made the largest numerical contribution to each of these 3 clusters.
FIGURE 2. Transdiagnostic Clusters of Hyperactivation in Patients Compared With Healthy Individuals
A color version of the figure, as originally published, appears in the online version of this article (focus.psychiatryonline.org).
A, Activation likelihood estimation identified 3 transdiagnostic clusters of hyperactivation in the left amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), the left thalamus, and the perigenual/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC).
B, Percentage contribution of each research domain criteria (RDoC) to each cluster. Additional details are in eTable 7 in the Supplement.

Discussion

Meta-analyses of 367 task-related fMRI experiments in mood disorders, posttraumatic stress disorder, and anxiety disorders, comprising data from 4507 patients and 4755 control individuals, detected statistically robust transdiagnostic clusters of hypoactivation in the inferior prefrontal cortex/ insula, the inferior parietal lobule, and the putamen. These regions are part of a right-dominant brain system that supports contextual shifting and stopping of mental operations and behavioral responses (19-26). Specifically, the right inferior prefrontal cortex is critically involved in the inhibition of contextually inappropriate cognitive, affective, and motor responses (19-21); similarly, the putamen, particularly on the right, is essential for terminating contextually inappropriate motor and cognitive processes (22). The anterior insula (23, 24) and the right inferior parietal lobule (25, 26) participate in the generation of salience-related signals that either initiate or terminate the engagement of attentional and working memory networks in response to changing demands. The insula, and particularly the anterior portion on the right, is thought to have a major role in integrating interoceptive information with information from other brain regions, thus supporting the formation of the conscious experience of an embodied self (27, 28). This awareness of negative emotional states may act as a salient trigger for the insula and the adjacent inferior frontal regions to engage mechanisms of cognitive control. Notably, experiments involving domains of nonaffective cognition, affective processing, and social cognition showed a similar range of contributions to these clusters of hypoactivation (respective range: 28%-50% and 29%-56%) (Figure 1B; eTable 7 in the Supplement). We therefore infer that the dominant abnormality in mood disorders, post traumatic stress disorder, and anxiety disorders involves a diagnosis-general disruption in salience processing (including interoceptive processing) and inhibitory control. These results contradict early hypotheses, which stipulated that affective morbidity results from right-sided fronto-parietal hyperactivity in response to negative/withdrawal stimuli (29), but are in line with evidence that emphasizes the role of deficient cognitive control (30-32).
Further support derives from studies showing that deficits in the ability to stop and shift ongoing affective states and thoughts are the most significant predictors of affective symptoms and syndromes (33-35). Neurocognitive studies in mood and anxiety disorders also indicate a general disruption in cognitive control because they consistently report deficits of large effect size in stopping and shifting responses in a range of tasks (32, 36, 37). Thus, impaired engagement of brain regions that subserve salience processing and inhibitory control present a plausible explanatory mechanism for the affective and nonaffective abnormalities observed in patients. In a separate meta-analysis (38) of functional neuroimaging studies that was limited only to tasks of cognitive control, hypoactivation in the right inferior prefrontal/insular cortex was also reported as a transdiagnostic feature of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, anxiety disorders, and substance use (38). When considered together, these findings point to the possibility that abnormalities in brain regions involved in switching and stopping may underpin the vulnerability to develop any and all forms of psychopathology. Interestingly, similar arguments have been put forward for a single dimension of psychopathology, termed factor p, as a main predictor of individuals’ liability for all mental disorders (39). The relationship between the p factor and disrupted engagement in salience/inhibitory control regions presents an intriguing avenue for future research.
We also identified 3 transdiagnostic clusters of hyperactivation in patients compared with healthy individuals centered in the left amygdala/parahippocampal gyrus, the left thalamus, and the perigenual/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex that were attributable mainly to experiments mapping to RDoC domains relating to affective and social processing (Figure 2B). The clusters identified appear plausible because they comprised regions consistently associated with affective morbidity (15, 16). The perigenual/dorsal anterior cingulate cortex is known to exert a regulatory influence on emotional experience and appraisal (40) while the amygdala and parahippocampal gyrus, particularly on the left, are involved in emotional memory formation and retrieval (41). The dorsal anterior cingulate cortex is also closely involved in the generation of internal autonomic and their associated expressive emotional responses (42). Its relative hyperactivation in patients is consistent with the notion of increased arousal in response to stress that may be a trait feature of mood and anxiety disorders but may also reflect increased stress response to the fMRI tasks (43).
Notwithstanding, these clusters were only detectable at a liberal statistical threshold, indicating greater inconsistency across primary studies that may indicate that hyperactivation in patients compared with healthy individuals may be more sensitive to variations in fMRI task design (eg, type or duration of stimuli or task instructions) and neuroimaging acquisition and analysis parameters. Detailed investigations that could directly address these issues would require more data than are currently available in the entire literature corpus.

Limitations

We placed substantial emphasis on the rigor and reproducibility of our methods to address ongoing concerns about the disparity in the number and localization of clusters in previous meta-analyses (eTable 1 in the Supplement). To further enhance reproducibility, we classified experiments based on the RDoC framework, which offers a structured approach to classification for fMRI tasks in future studies. We only included studies in adults, and therefore these findings may not generalize to pediatric or geriatric groups. We did not consider studies that failed to find case-control differences because such practice could only be justified if negative studies were sufficiently powered. We did not find an effect of symptom severity on the transdiagnostic clusters. This observation should be viewed with caution because of the variable instruments used to rate psychopathology and the reliance on group means from each study sample. Medication status did not have a statistically significant moderator association with the results reported. Medication has been shown to have mostly normalizing effects (44) and may have attenuated case-control differences in the primary studies. We examined disorders with significant symptomatic and syndromal overlap for which we had comparable amount of data across diagnoses. We decided to exclude task-related fMRI articles on schizophrenia because the disproportionately larger number of studies (>250)9 would have skewed the results. Given the observed power in this study, the results are statistically robust, but as the literature expands it is possible that additional transdiagnostic or disease specific clusters may emerge.

Conclusions

This meta-analysis of what is, to our knowledge, the largest data set of fMRI studies currently available identified reduced engagement of brain regions associated with inhibitory control and salience processing as the most consistent neurobiological feature in mood disorders, posttraumatic stress disorder, and anxiety disorders. These shared brain phenotypes have the potential to serve as targets for interventions aiming to improve clinical outcomes and reduce or prevent affective morbidity in the general population. Tracking the trajectory of disruption in these regions across development could provide invaluable information regarding their timing and their association with emerging psychopathology and psychiatric nosology.

REFERENCES

1.
Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, et al ; Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005 ; 62 ( 6 ): 593 - 602.
2.
Whiteford HA, Degenhardt L, Rehm J, et al;. Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet. 2013 ; 382 ( 9904 ): 1575 - 1586.
3.
Wittchen HU, Zhao S, Kessler RC, et al ; DSM-III-R generalized anxiety disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994 ; 51 ( 5 ): 355 - 364.
4.
Kessler RC, Merikangas KR, Wang PS. Prevalence, comorbidity, and service utilization for mood disorders in the United States at the beginning of the twenty-first century. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2007 ; 3 ( 1 ): 137 - 158.
5.
Merikangas KR, Akiskal HS, Angst J, et al;. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of bipolar spectrum disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007 ; 64 ( 5 ): 543 - 552.
6.
Beard C, Millner AJ, Forgeard MJC, et al;. Network analysis of depression and anxiety symptom relationships in a psychiatric sample. Psychol Med. 2016 ; 46 ( 16 ): 3359 - 3369.
7.
Judd LL, Schettler PJ, Akiskal HS, et al;. Long-term symptomatic status of bipolar I vs bipolar II disorders. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2003 ; 6 ( 2 ): 127 - 137.
8.
Müller VI, Cieslik EC, Serbanescu I, et al ; Altered brain activity in unipolar depression revisited: meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017 ; 74 ( 1 ): 47 - 55.
9.
Sprooten E, Rasgon A, Goodman M, et al;. Addressing reverse inference in psychiatric neuroimaging: meta-analyses of task-related brain activation in common mental disorders. Hum Brain Mapp. 2017 ; 38 ( 4 ): 1846 - 1864.
10.
Eickhoff SB, Nichols TE, Laird AR, et al;. Behavior, sensitivity, and power of activation likelihood estimation characterized by massive empirical simulation. Neuroimage. 2016 ; 137 : 70 - 85.
11.
Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, Grefkes C, et al ; Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. HumBrain Mapp. 2009 ; 30 ( 9 ): 2907 - 2926.
12.
Eickhoff SB, Bzdok D, Laird AR, et al ; Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. Neuroimage. 2012 ; 59 ( 3 ): 2349 - 2361.
13.
Turkeltaub PE, Eickhoff SB, Laird AR, et al ; Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in Activation Likelihood Estimation meta-analyses. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012 ; 33 ( 1 ): 1 - 13.
14.
Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, et al;. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2010 ; 167 ( 7 ): 748 - 751.
15.
Etkin A. Functional neuroanatomy of anxiety: a neural circuit perspective. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2010 ; 2 : 251 - 277.
16.
Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, et al ; Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2003 ; 54 ( 5 ): 515 - 528.
17.
Pessoa L. Understanding brain networks and brain organization. Phys Life Rev. 2014 ; 11 ( 3 ): 400 - 435.
18.
Price CJ, Friston KJ. Functional ontologies for cognition: the systematic definition of structure and function. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2005 ; 22 ( 3 ): 262 - 275.
19.
Nee DE, Wager TD, Jonides J. Interference resolution: insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging tasks. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007 ; 7 ( 1 ): 1 - 17.
20.
Robbins TW. Shifting and stopping: fronto-striatal substrates, neurochemical modulation and clinical implications. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2007 ; 362 ( 1481 ): 917 - 932.
21.
Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004 ; 8 ( 4 ): 170 - 177.
22.
Guo Y, Schmitz TW, Mur M, et al ; A supramodal role of the basal ganglia in memory and motor inhibition: meta-analytic evidence. Neuropsychologia. 2018 ; 108 : 117 - 134.
23.
Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010 ; 214 ( 5-6 ): 655 - 667.
24.
Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008 ; 105 ( 34 ): 12569 - 12574.
25.
Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002 ; 3 ( 3 ): 201 - 215.
26.
Buschman TJ, Miller EK. Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science. 2007 ; 315 ( 5820 ): 1860 - 1862.
27.
Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, et al ; Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2004 ; 7 ( 2 ): 189 - 195.
28.
Craig AD. How do you feel—now? the anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009 ; 10 ( 1 ): 59 - 70.
29.
Bruder GE, Stewart JW, McGrath PJ. Right brain, left brain in depressive disorders: clinical and theoretical implications of behavioral, electrophysiological and neuroimaging findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017 ; 78 : 178 - 191.
30.
Ochsner KN, Ray RD, Cooper JC, et al;. For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage. 2004 ; 23 ( 2 ): 483 - 499.
31.
Goeleven E, De Raedt R, Baert S, et al ; Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. J Affect Disord. 2006 ; 93 ( 1-3 ): 149 - 157.
32.
Paulus MP. Cognitive control in depression and anxiety: out of control? Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2015 ; 1 : 113 - 120.
33.
Charles ST, Piazza JR, Mogle J, et al ; The wear and tear of daily stressors on mental health. Psychol Sci. 2013 ; 24 ( 5 ): 733 - 741.
34.
Neumann A, van Lier PAC, Frijns T, et al ; Emotional dynamics in the development of early adolescent psychopathology: a one-year longitudinal study. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2011 ; 39 ( 5 ): 657 - 669.
35.
Zetsche U, Bürkner PC, Schulze L. Shedding light on the association between repetitive negative thinking and deficits in cognitive control: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2018 ; 63 ( April ): 56 - 65.
36.
Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord. 2009 ; 113 ( 1-2 ): 1 - 20.
37.
Bora E, Harrison BJ, Yücel M, et al ; Cognitive impairment in euthymic major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2013 ; 43 ( 10 ): 2017 - 2026.
38.
McTeague LM, Huemer J, Carreon DM, et al ; Identification of common neural circuit disruptions in cognitive control across psychiatric disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2017 ; 174 ( 7 ): 676 - 685.
39.
Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, et al;. The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin Psychol Sci. 2014 ; 2 ( 2 ): 119 - 137.
40.
Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011 ; 15 ( 2 ): 85 - 93.
41.
Markowitsch HJ. Differential contribution of right and left amygdala to affective information processing. Behav Neurol. 1998 ; 11 ( 4 ): 233 - 244.
42.
Gianaros PJ, Derbyshire SWG, May JC, et al ; Anterior cingulate activity correlates with blood pressure during stress. Psychophysiology. 2005 ; 42 ( 6 ): 627 - 635.
43.
Muehlhan M, Lueken U, Siegert J, et al ; Enhanced sympathetic arousal in response to FMRI scanning correlates with task induced activations and deactivations. PLoS One. 2013 ; 8 ( 8 ): e72576.
44.
Hafeman DM, Chang KD, Garrett AS, et al ; Effects of medication on neuroimaging findings in bipolar disorder: an updated review. Bipolar Disord. 2012 ; 14 ( 4 ): 375 - 410.

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

History

Published in print: Spring 2021
Published online: 17 June 2021

Authors

Details

James W. Murrough, MD., PhD
Simon B. Eickhoff, MD., PhD
Sophia Frangou, MD., PhD.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Focus

PPV Articles - Focus

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share