Site maintenance Wednesday, November 13th, 2024. Please note that access to some content and account information will be unavailable on this date.
Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Articles
Published Online: 24 October 2017

Exploring the Spectrum of Subcortical Hyperintensities and Cognitive Decline

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

White matter hyperintensities (WMHs) include periventricular WMH (pvWMH) and deep WMH. When hyperintensities in the basal ganglia or brainstem are included, the collective term is subcortical hyperintensities. Both WMH and medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) are risk factors for cognitive decline. This prospective study enrolled participants aged 50–85 years and followed their neuropsychological assessments annually for 2 years to explore the interactive effects of WMH and MTA on longitudinal clinical decline. Brain MRI was performed at the beginning of enrollment. Of the 200 participants, 57 were “normal” individuals, 40 had dysexecutive mild cognitive impairment, 53 had amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and 50 had Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Overall, MTA significantly correlated with pvWMH (p=0.0004) but not with deep WMH, as defined by criteria using the Scheltens’ Scale. Total Scheltens’ score was specifically associated with the domain of semantic fluency (beta=−0.4, 95% CI=–0.7 to –0.2, p=0.002), which remained significant when adjusting for MTA (beta=−0.3, 95% CI=–0.5 to –0.1, p=0.017). The pvWMH was significantly higher in AD subjects than in normal control subjects (beta=0.3, 95% CI=0.1 to 0.4, p=0.001), especially the periventricular occipital caps (beta=0.2, 95% CI=0.1 to 0.3, p=0.0003). Cox proportional hazards model showed that the periventricular bands (PVB) predicted 1-year clinical decline (hazard ratio [HR]=5.3, 95% CI=1.8 to 15.7, p=0.002), which remained significant when further adjusting for MTA (HR=4.0, 95% CI=1.3 to 12.1, p=0.013). In summary, pvWMH, especially the occipital caps, was correlated with MTA and the AD subgroup. Assessment of semantic fluency may be useful for the clinical evaluation of the degree of subcortical hyperintensity burden. Visual rating of PVB could be an independent predictor for 1-year clinical decline.
The pathological changes seen in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) consist of brain atrophy, particularly hippocampal and medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA), enlarged ventricles, and white matter hyperintensities (WMHs). Previous studies have shown that the WMH loci and burden were disproportional to gray matter atrophy in AD and that the occurrence of WMH may occur even earlier than does gray matter atrophy.1,2 WMH has been shown to be an independent predictor of the severity of cognitive impairment in patients with AD,3 as well as the conversion rate of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) to AD.4,5 Individuals with β-amyloid (Aβ) deposition were more likely to exhibit clinical manifestation of dementia if they had substantial WMH burden,6 and the effects of Aβ and WMH burden on dementia were likely to be independent.7 The mechanism of WMH initiation is largely unknown. Synergistic interactions between elevated levels of Aβ and comorbid vascular insufficiency are believed to result in WMH.6,8
Clinically, MCI is defined as a transitional clinical state from normal cognition to dementia9 and can be separated into two subtypes according to the involved cognitive area. Whereas amnestic MCI (aMCI) predominantly involves memory, dysexecutive MCI (dMCI) involves nonmemory domains such as executive functions. Although dMCI was less explored in previous research, it accounted for more than 40% of MCIs in community-based studies.3,10 Because white matter subserves rapid and efficient processing speed, the initial clinical manifestations of high WMH burdens involves the nonmemory domains. A longitudinal study5 has shown that WMH burden and entorhinal cortex volume predict rapid cognitive decline among MCI individuals, but how WMH interact with MTA in terms of visual scores and how WMH loci predict clinical decline in patients with aMCI and dMCI is less well established. The present prospective study examined the interactive effects between the visual scores of MTA and WMH to determine whether visual rating of WMH can be an independent predictor of clinical cognitive decline in a clinical spectrum of the “normal,” dMCI, aMCI, and AD participants.

Methods

Subjects were recruited from the Dementia Clinic of Chang Gung Memorial Hospital (CGMH) and followed annually for 2 years. Brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed when the patient was enrolled, and the measurement of the visual scores was based on the baseline MRI. Yearly neuropsychological assessments were performed for 2 years. The inclusion criteria included the following: age between 50 and 85 years, modified Hachinski ischemic score of <4, and good general health with no additional diseases that could interfere with the study. Patients with a previous clinical history of neurological, psychiatric, somatic, stroke, or toxic causes of dementia were excluded. Exclusion criteria included cognitive impairment resulting from cerebral trauma, subdural hematoma, or hypoxic cerebral damage regardless of etiology; vitamin deficiency states, including folate, vitamin B12, and other B-complex deficiencies, such as thiamine deficiency in Korsakoff’s syndrome; central nervous system infection, brain neoplasm, and significant endocrine or metabolic disease, including thyroid or pituitary disease, Cushing’s syndrome, severe renal failure, or mental retardation; and meeting DSM-IV criteria for vascular dementia and the requirements of the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke-Association Internationale pour la Recherche et l’Enseignement en Neurosciences criteria for probable vascular dementia.
A battery of neuropsychological assessments11 was performed at baseline and each year for 2 years, which included the following: the Taiwanese version of the Wechsler Memory Scale (third edition) (mainly logical memory and visual reproduction subtests)12 and Word Sequence Learning Test to assess memory;13 the Semantic Association of Verbal Fluency;14 the Three-Dimensional Block Construction Models to assess visuospatial function;15 the digit span and digit symbol substitution subtests of the Taiwanese version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (version 3) to assess attention;16 and the Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and Trail-Making Test, Part B, to assess executive function.17 The Mini-Mental State Examination, Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument, Chinese Version,18 and Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR)-sum of box were also used.19 Significant clinical decline that may influence activities of daily living (ADL) was defined as a 0.5-point CDR increase above the baseline.
The normal control subjects were defined when memory function was normal for age; CDR and memory box score were both zero; subjects were cognitively normal, on the basis of an absence of significant impairment in cognitive functions or ADL; an informant was available from people who had frequent contact with the subject (an average of 10 hours per week or more) and could be reached by the investigators in person or by phone. The aMCI group was defined by using the following criteria: memory complaints and memory difficulties that could be verified by an informant; abnormal memory function (≤1.5 standard deviations [SD] below the lower age-adjusted norms) on the Selective Reminding Test or comparable memory test for episodic memory; CDR score equal to .5 and memory box score of .5; cognitively normal, on the basis of an absence of significant impairment in cognitive functions or ADL; and general cognitive and functional performance sufficiently preserved such that a diagnosis of AD could be made by the physician at the time of the screening visit. The dMCI group was defined by using the following criteria: abnormal executive function ≤1.5 SDs below the lower age-adjusted norms on at least one of four executive function tests (WCST, Digit Span, Trail-Making Test, Part B, and the Semantic Association of Verbal Fluency); and preservation of memory and cognitive function. The AD group was defined by using the following criteria: CDR score equal to .5, 1.0, or 2.0; probable AD as defined by Alzheimer’s criteria from the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Diseases and Stroke/Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association;20 and Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia score of <8. For those with a CDR of .5, the diagnosis of incipient AD depended on the combined consensus of the investigators at a clinical level, ADL, and neuropsychological impairment.21 This study was approved by the institutional review board of CGMH. All subjects gave written informed consent for the study.
Brain MRI was conducted with a 3-Tesla MR scanner (Magnetom Trio with TIM; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany). The MTA was rated on coronal T1-weighted images on a slice through the corpus of the hippocampus at the level of the anterior pons (Scheltens’ score) (Figure 1). Visual rating of WMHs was based on the criteria of Fazekas scale22 and Scheltens’ Scale23 to grade the severity of WMH on T2-weighted and fluid-attenuated inversion recovery scans (see Figure 2). The investigators for visual rating were blinded to the clinical information of the participants. Fazekas grade 1 was defined by WMH <9 mm for a single lesion and <20 mm for grouped lesions. Fazekas 2 was early confluent lesions of 10–20 mm for a single lesion and >20 mm for grouped lesions without connecting bridges between lesions. Fazekas grade 3 was a single lesion or confluent lesion ≥20 mm. Scheltens’ Scale was evaluated and summed by visual rating with 0- to 6-point severity ratings in the periventricular (frontal caps, PVBs, and occipital bands), deep (frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital), basal ganglia (caudate, putamen, globus pallidus, thalamus, internal/external capsule), and infratentorial (cerebellum, mesencephalon, pons, medulla) areas.23,24 As is classified using the recommendations of STRIVE [standards for reporting vascular changes on neuroimaging],25 WMH included periventricular and deep WMHs. When hyperintensities in the subcortical gray matter (basal ganglia as defined by Scheltens’ Scale) or brainstem were included, the collective term was subcortical hyperintensities. Silent lacune was defined when lacune lesion was discovered on MRI with no clinical history of stroke.
FIGURE 1. The Medial Temporal Lobe Atrophy (MTA) Scoring Using the Scheltens’ Scalea
a The MTA was rated on coronal T1-weighted images on a slice through the corpus of the hippocampus at the level of the anterior pons.
FIGURE 2. The Fazekas Scoring and Examples of Periventricular Scoring of the Scheltens’ Scalea
a The three grades of the Fazekas scoring and the examples of periventricular scoring of the Scheltens’ Scale are demonstrated by utilizing the sequence of fluid-attenuated inversion recovery. The white matter hyperintensities (WMHs) within the white frame are the examples of periventricular scoring of the Scheltens’ Scale, including periventricular WMH at the frontal area, band area, and occipital area.
The chi-square test, Fisher’s exact test, or t test was utilized to compare demographic data and the distributions of risks between groups, where appropriate. Two-tailed p values were derived from the tests. Association analyses were examined by general linear models (GLM) and logistic regression, where appropriate. Under the assumption that the effect of increase on each point of the score was the same, GLM was used for comparisons of visual rating scales between participants. Confounding factors included age, sex, education, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, smoking, and the presence of the APOE4 allele. Survival analysis was performed with the Kaplan-Meier estimate, and the Cox proportional hazards model was used to test the WMH effect on the time of clinical functional decline (arbitrarily defined as a .5 point of CDR increase), adjusting for MTA and confounding factors.

Results

We enrolled 200 subjects in this study, including 57 people with normal cognitive function, 93 people with MCI (dMCI, N=40; and aMCI, N=53), and 50 patients with AD. The demographic data for the enrolled population is shown in Table 1. The sex proportions were similar between the groups. Normal control subjects were younger and had more years of education than did the other three groups. The proportion of hypertension and diabetes mellitus was borderline higher in the MCI and the AD groups than in the normal group. There were more carriers of the APOE4 allele in the aMCI and AD groups than in the control group (p=.003 and .018, respectively). The distribution of body mass index and smoking frequency was not different between the groups. As was expected, neuropsychological examinations showed that global cognitive function and all of the domains examined were significantly different between the controls and each of the other three groups (Table 1), including the domains of memory, language, attention, executive, and visual spatial function. In contrast to the aMCI group, patients with dMCI had relatively preserved global and memory function but impaired executive function (Table 1) (WCST, p=.003). The yearly follow-up rate was 85% and 68% for 2 years (Table 1). There was no difference in the mean survival time to clinical decline between the dMCI and the aMCI groups.
TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of the Study Population at Baselinea
CharacteristicNormal Control Group (N=57)dMCI Group (N=40)aMCI Group (N=53)AD Group (N=50)p Value Comparison
dMCI Compared With NormalaMCI Compared With NormalaMCI Compared With dMCIAD Compared With Normal
Age (years)62.3±7.266.7±8.368.4±7.873.0±9.50.006<0.00010.32<0.0001
Men (%)52.645.058.544.00.460.540.200.37
Education (years)10.3±4.28.0±4.17.7±5.36.8±5.80.0080.0050.790.0005
Body mass index (kg/m2)24.5±2.925.8±3.824.2±3.023.4±3.30.110.640.060.10
Hypertension (%)25.047.549.144.90.020.010.880.03
Diabetes (%)7.125.016.924.50.010.110.340.01
Smoking (%)1.87.59.44.10.170.080.740.49
APOE4 carrier (%)8.823.132.026.00.0510.0030.350.018
Depressionb1.96±2.671.58±2.381.67±2.072.18±2.480.460.540.840.77
Global cognitionc28.5±1.327.3±2.024.2±4.515.8±6.60.0004<0.00010.0001<0.0001
Global cognitiond92.8±3.889.2±4.680.3±14.655.1±20.8<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001
Memory, logical memory11.8±2.810.1±2.57.1±3.83.4±2.30.003<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001
Memory, visual reproduction11.4±2.89.8±2.48.2±2.86.2±1.70.003<0.00010.004<0.0001
Word sequence learning3.4±1.72.1±1.70.48±1.020.05±0.220.0004<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001
Semantic fluency35.0±7.431.4±7.528.8±6.515.9±7.40.02<0.00010.08<0.0001
Attention, digit span11.6±2.710±2.59.8±2.77.6±2.00.0040.00080.73<0.0001
Digit symbol substitution10.6±2.69.4±2.48.7±3.36.2±2.80.020.0010.24<0.0001
Executivee5.3±1.12.4±1.23.5±2.01.6±2.3<0.0001<0.00010.003<0.0001
Trail-Making Test, Part B        
Visual spatial, three-dimensional block construction models28.7±0.727.8±1.927.1±3.619.1±9.90.0020.0010.28<0.0001
Clinical Dementia Rating, sum of box00.3±0.70.9±0.55.9±3.50.001<0.0001<0.0001<0.0001
One-year clinical decline (N=170)0% (0/49)10.3% (3/29)13% (6/46)28.3% (13/46)0.0480.011<0.0001
Two-year clinical decline (N=136)7.3% (3/41)22.2% (4/18)9.8% (4/41)30.6% (11/36)0.1810.230.02
a
Comparisons among normal subjects, subjects with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (amnestic MCI [aMCI] and dysexecutive MCI [dMCI]), and subjects with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) were analyzed using chi-square test, Fisher’s exact test, or t test, where appropriate; data are expressed as percentage or mean±SD.
b
Assessed using the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale.
c
Assessed using the Mini-Mental State Examination.
d
Assessed using the Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument.
e
Assessed using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (completed categories).
Differences in visual WMH scores utilizing Scheltens’ Scale corresponding to the MTA score are shown in Table 2. Visual score of MTA significantly correlated with that of periventricular WMH (pvWMH, beta=0.1, 95% CI=0.06–0.2, p=0.0004), following adjustment for confounding factors. The magnitude of this significance was notably decreasing when adjusting for subgroups (p=.03), suggesting that the correlation between MTA and pvWMH within each subgroup is weak. The associations between MTA and total score of subcortical hyperintensities fell short of statistical significance (p=0.046). There was no association between MTA and hyperintensities in deep white matter, basal ganglia, or brainstem areas.
TABLE 2. Differences (95% Confidence Intervals) in Subcortical Hyperintensity Scores Utilizing Scheltens’ Scale as a Function of the Increase in One Point of the Medial Temporal Lobe Atrophy (MTA) Score in Each Groupa
ItemSubcortical Hyperintensity (White Mater, Deep Gray Matter, and Brainstem)White Mater (Periventricular and Deep White Matter)Periventricular White MatterDeep White MatterDeep Gray Matter and Brainstem pb
Beta95% CIpBeta95% CIpBeta95% CIpBeta95% CIp
Group             
 Normal (N=57)0.6–0.4 to 1.50.220.3–0.6 to 1.20.45–0.1–0.3 to 0.10.230.5–0.3 to 1.30.260.07
 dMCI, (N=40)–1.0–2.7 to 0.80.26–1.02–2.7 to 0.70.23–0.2–0.5 to 0.20.29–0.8–2.3 to 0.60.250.45
 aMCI (N=53)0.3–0.5 to 1.10.460.2–0.6 to 1.00.610.05–0.2 to 0.30.610.2–0.5 to 0.80.640.90
 AD (N=50)–0.02–0.8 to 0.70.960.1–0.5 to 0.70.700.1–0.03 to 0.30.120.001–0.5 to 0.50.990.12
Total (N=200)0.40.01 to 0.70.0460.30.002 to 0.60.0480.10.06 to 0.210.00040.2–0.1 to 0.50.180.31
Total (N=200)c0.3–0.1 to 0.70.170.3–0.1 to 0.60.140.10.01 to 0.20.030.2–0.1 to 0.50.250.41
a
Beta (95% CI) derived from general linear models adjusting for age, sex, APOE4, years of education, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, and smoking shows the effect of increase in one point of the MTA score on subcortical hyperintensity; AD=Alzheimer’s disease; aMCI=amnestic mild cognitive impairment; dMCI=dysexecutive mild cognitive impairment.
b
The p value was estimated by using logistic regression, comparing participants with and without hyperintensity in deep gray matter and brainstem.
c
The model additionally adjusted for the normal, MCI, and dementia groups.
In all the participants, the associations between cognitive assessment and total subcortical hyperintensities were analyzed in detail (Table 3). As was expected, MTA was significantly associated with global cognition and all cognitive domains (p<0.0001), except for a borderline significance with the WCST (p=0.02). In contrast, overall subcortical hyperintensity was specifically associated with the domain of semantic fluency (beta=−0.4, 95% confidence interval [CI]=–0.7 to –0.2, p=0.002), which remained borderline significant after further adjusting for MTA (beta=−0.3, 95% CI=–0.5 to –0.1, p=0.017). Specifically, both pvWMH (beta=−2.2, 95% CI=–3.5 to –0.9, p=0.001) and deep WMH (beta=−0.5, 95% CI= –0.8 to –0.15, p=0.004) were related to the performance of semantic fluency. When further adjusting for MTA, pvWMH was no longer associated with semantic fluency (p=0.055), while deep WMH and semantic fluency remained associated (beta=−0.3, 95% CI= –0.6 to –0.04, p=0.027).
TABLE 3. Differences in Cognitive Evaluation as a Function of Visual Rating Scores of Overall Loading of Subcortical Hyperintensity in All Participantsa
MeasureMedial Temporal Lobe AtrophySubcortical Hyperintensity (White Matter, Deep Gray Matter, and Brainstem)
Beta95% CIpbBeta95% CIpbBeta95% CIpc
Hamilton Depression Rating Scale–0.1–0.4 to 0.20.420.01–0.1 to 0.10.740.02–0.1 to 0.10.68
Global cognition         
 Mini-Mental State Examination–1.7–2.0 to –1.3<0.0001–0.1–0.2 to 0.10.530.04–0.1 to 0.20.57
 Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument–5.3–6.3 to –4.2<0.0001–0.2–0.8 to 0.30.36–0.1–0.4 to 0.50.77
Memory         
 Logical memory–1–1.2 to –0.7<0.0001–0.1–0.2 to 0.10.32–0.001–0.1 to 0.10.99
 Visual reproduction–0.6–0.8 to –0.4<0.0001–0.1–0.1 to 0.040.26–0.01–0.1 to 0.10.71
Word sequence learning (recall)–0.2–0.4 to –0.10.0004–0.02–0.1 to 0.030.44–0.01–0.1 to 0.040.79
Semantic fluency–2.3–3.0 to –1.7<0.0001–0.4–0.7 to –0.20.002–0.3–0.5 to –0.10.017
Attention, digit span–0.3–0.5 to –0.10.008–0.02–0.1 to 0.10.57–0.01–0.1 to 0.10.86
Digit symbol substitution–0.6–0.9 to –0.4<0.0001–0.1–0.1 to 0.030.23–0.01–0.1 to 0.10.72
Executive, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test–0.2–0.4 to –0.040.02–0.02–0.1 to 0.04)0.61–0.003–0.1 to 0.10.91
Visual spatial, three-dimensional block construction mode decline2.11.3 to 3.40.002d1.00.9 to 1.10.88d0.990.93 to 1.10.78d
Clinical Dementia Rating0.80.7 to 1.0<0.00010.04–0.1 to 0.10.33–0.01–0.1 to 0.10.89
a
Beta (95% CI) derived from general linear models shows the effect of medial temporal lobe atrophy or periventricular hyperintensity on cognitive function.
b
The model adjusted for age, sex, APOE4, years of education, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, and smoking.
c
The model adjusted for age, sex, APOE4, years of education, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, smoking, and medial temporal lobe atrophy.
d
Estimated by using logistic regression.
Differences in visual rating scores on the MRI scans between the subgroups are shown in Table 4. When adjusting for confounding factors, the MTA score was significantly higher in the AD group than in the control group (p<0.0001). There was no remarkable difference in the MTA score between the control group and the two MCI groups. Silent lacunar lesions discovered in the MRI scans were slightly higher in the aMCI group than in the control group (p=0.04). With regard to WMH, we found that the distribution of the Fazekas score was different between the control group and the AD group (p=0.016). When further estimated using Scheltens’ scoring for WMH, the association between the Fazekas score and AD was mainly contributed to by the lesions at pvWMH (beta=0.3, 95% CI=0.1–0.4, p=0.001), especially the periventricular occipital caps (beta=0.2, 95% CI=0.1–0.3, p=0.0003). The periventricular occipital caps score was found to be slightly higher in the aMCI group than that in the control and dMCI groups (p=0.036 and 0.049, respectively). There was no significant difference between subgroups regarding the hyperintensities of whole subcortical areas. Diabetes mellitus was the only risk factor for pvWMH (beta=0.61, 95% CI=0.23–0.64, p=0.002).
TABLE 4. Differences in Visual Rating Scores on MRI Among Normal Control Subjects and Patients With Dysexecutive Mild Cognitive Impairment (dMCI), Patients With Amnestic Mild Cognitive Impairment (aMCI), and Patients With Alzheimer’s Disease (AD)
ItemNormal Control Subjects (N=57)dMCI Patients (N=40)aMCI Patients (N=53)AD Patients (N=50)Comparisona
dMCI Compared With Normal BetaaMCI Compared With Normal BetaaMCI Compared With MCI BetaAD Compared With Normal p
Medial temporal lobe atrophy (sum of both sides)0.93±1.150.98±1.031.60±1.593.8±2.240.570.650.18<0.0001
Presence of silent lacune (%)5.2612.522.68.00.210.040.190.72
Fazekas (0, 1, 2, 3) (%)1.75, 94.74, 1.75,1.750, 80, 15, 51.9, 79.3, 15.1, 3.80, 56, 26, 180.140.070.980.016
Subcortical hyperintensity6.18±4.417.4±4.847.53±4.8011.4±7.250.550.680.850.30
 White matter hyperintensity5.93±4.187.25±4.817.34±4.5810.56±5.750.700.800.850.31
    Periventricular2.44±0.952.78±1.073.15±1.233.96±1.40.730.070.080.001
   Frontal1.02±0.351.05±0.321.09±0.451.32±0.470.330.850.380.16
   Band1.0±0.271.10±0.301.13±0.341.32±0.470.360.250.620.08
   Occipital0.42±0.600.63±0.740.92±0.761.32±0.770.580.10.30.0003
  Deep white matter3.49±3.524.48±3.954.19±3.716.6±4.620.590.430.810.81
 Presence of deep gray matter and brainstem (%)15.81015.1320.250.300.790.53
a
Comparisons between the normal group and each of the MCI and dementia groups were analyzed using general linear models or logistic regression that adjusted for age, sex, APOE4, years of education, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, and smoking.
In all the participants, we found that the pvWMH at the PVBs (Figure 2) was a significant predictor for 1-year and 2-year cognitive decline. To explore the effects of PVB on 2-year cognitive decline, we performed a Kaplan-Meier survival analysis and showed that the mean survival period to clinical decline was 1.9 years (95% CI=1.8–1.9) for subjects with a score of 1 for PVB, in comparison with 1.6 years (95% CI=1.4–1.8) for the group with a score of 2 (log-rank test, p=0.005). In those whose MTA score was zero, equal to 1 or 2, and >2, the mean survival was 1.98 years (95% CI=1.95–2.02), 1.85 years (95% CI=1.76–1.93), and 1.73 years (95% CI=1.59–1.86), respectively (p<0.0001). The significant drop rate prohibited us from exploring the exact prediction effect beyond 1 year; the survival analyses were therefore performed utilizing only the data of the first year. Still, in the 1-year follow-up, a Cox proportional hazards model adjusting for confounding factors for evaluating the PVB effect on 1-year cognition showed that the visual PVB score predicted clinical decline in all of the participants (hazard ratio [HR]=5.3, 95% CI=1.8–15.7, p=0.002). This significance remained when further adjusting for MTA (HR=4.0, 95% CI=1.3–12.1, p=0.013). The MTA score was also an independent predictor for 1-year clinical decline (HR=2.2, 95% CI=1.2–4.3, p=0.015). There was no interactive effect between MTA and PVB in predicting clinical decline (p=0.31). Specifically, PVB significantly predicted the 1-year clinical decline (odds ratio [OR]=11.4, 95% CI=2.5–51.5, p=0.002), which was independent of the effects of MTA and the other covariables (Figure 3). When the MTA score was equal to 1 or 2, the rate of 1-year clinical decline was higher in the group with a score of 2 (25%) than it was in the group with a score of 1 (11.5%). Following a similar pattern, when the MTA score was >2, the rate of 1-year clinical decline was higher in the PVB group with a score of 2 (42.9%) than in the group with a score of 1 (20%). Nonetheless, MTA is also an independent risk factor for 1-year clinical decline (OR=3.7, 95% CI=1.7–8.2, p=0.001). Still, there was no interactive effect between MTA and PVB in predicting 1-year clinical decline. When stratified by the clinical subgroups, we found that neither visual rating PVB score nor visual rating MTA score could be a strong predictor for 1-year decline in each of the subgroups, suggesting a ceiling effect of utilizing a visual score to predict 1-year clinical outcome in each subgroup.
FIGURE 3. Predictors for One-Year Clinical Declinea
a In the 1-year follow-up, pvWMH bands (PVB) significantly predicted 1-year clinical decline, which was independent of the effects of medial temporal lobe atrophy (MTA) and other covariables. The rate of 1-year clinical decline was higher in the PVB group that scored 2 (PVB=2) than in the group that scored 1 (PVB=1) in each MTA subgroup. As was expected, MTA was found to be an independent risk factor for 1-year clinical decline. There was no interactive effect between MTA and PVB in predicting 1-year clinical decline.

Discussion

The prospective study herein demonstrates that pvWMH, especially the occipital caps, is independently associated with AD. Although the visual score of MTA is the strongest predictor for clinical outcome in the overall participants, the pvWMH at the PVBs is another independent predictor for clinical decline. We did not find an interactive effect between visual scores of MTA and WMH on the clinical outcome. When considering the confounders, the association between the visual scores of MTA and the total subcortical WMH is weak. For the neuropsychological examinations, in contrast to the involvement of the global function and multiple domains of MTA, semantic fluency was the only independent domain correlated to subcortical hyperintensities and therefore may be useful for clinical evaluation of total hyperintensities burden. Although WMH alone might not be a major risk for MCIs and AD, both MTA and pvWMH at the PVBs were independent predictors for 1-year clinical decline, suggesting that treatment targeting WMH may play an important role in the prevention of AD. Moreover, this study also suggests that diabetes mellitus is an important target for the prevention of pvWMH.
The semantic fluency test is frequently used to test a circumscribed aspect of executive function. During examination, participants have to generate as many items of a certain category as possible in a given time.26 Anatomical mapping of semantic fluency in stroke patients suggested the functional loci in the left inferior frontal gyrus and insula, left medial temporal regions, and the right inferior frontal gyrus, which related to application of a visuospatial mental imagery strategy in semantic fluency.26 There was no report addressing the correlation between subcortical hyperintensities and semantic fluency to date. Our study suggests that despite the strong effect of MTA in semantic fluency, subcortical hyperintensities, especially deep WMH, play an independent role in functioning semantic fluency. Further replication of this study, especially functional mapping, is important.
The associations between hippocampal volume reduction27 and clinical decline28 with pvWMH but not deep WMH have been reported. It was discovered that pvWMH shows an almost 30% loss of axons and multiple biochemical marker changes in histological tests.29 Diffusion tensor imaging displays disturbances in track integrity in the bilateral corona radiata, superior longitudinal fasciculus, cingulum, and corpus callosum in patients with AD, and all of these contain fibers that pass through the periventricular region.30 Although having MCIs or AD per se is a risk of 1-year clinical decline in this study, the burden of PVB is similar between the clinical subgroups, suggesting that visual rating of PVB could be an independent predictor for 1-year clinical decline. This study shows that pvWMH at PVBs is an independent predictor for 1-year clinical decline that may support the aforementioned neuropathological findings.
It is worth mentioning, however, that there are limitations to our study. First, the proportion of participant follow-up dropped significantly after 1 year, which prohibited us from exploring the exact prediction effect beyond 1 year. The survival results that are derived from an overall 2-year follow-up in this study should be interpreted with caution until a robust replication is available. Second, the biological relevance of brain integrity and WMH is not clear and remains to be investigated. Thus, further cohort studies with adequate sample sizes are needed to confirm our results before the pvWMH score can be viewed as an independent marker of clinical decline. Nevertheless, this study provides evidence for significant associations of visual WMH scores with semantic fluency and AD risks. Because both MTA and PVB are independent predictors for clinical decline, future treatments targeting WMH may provide an additional prevention role for AD.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Yau-Yau Wai and Dr. Hon-Chung Fung for their valuable contributions to this study. The authors also thank the participants in this study.

References

1.
Fischer FU, Wolf D, Scheurich A, et al: Altered whole-brain white matter networks in preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroimage Clin 2015; 8:660–666
2.
Collins-Praino LE, Francis YI, Griffith EY, et al: Soluble amyloid beta levels are elevated in the white matter of Alzheimer’s patients, independent of cortical plaque severity. Acta Neuropathol Commun 2014; 2:83
3.
Huang F, Shang Y, Luo Y, et al: Lower prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease among Tibetans: association with religious and genetic factors. J Alzheimers Dis 2016; 50:659–667
4.
Sachdev PS, Zhuang L, Braidy N, et al: Is Alzheimer’s a disease of the white matter? Curr Opin Psychiatry 2013; 26:244–251
5.
Li HL, Yang P, Liu ZJ, et al: Common variants at Bin1 are associated with sporadic Alzheimer’s disease in the Han Chinese population. Psychiatr Genet 2015; 25:21–25
6.
Provenzano FA, Muraskin J, Tosto G, et al: White matter hyperintensities and cerebral amyloidosis: necessary and sufficient for clinical expression of Alzheimer disease? JAMA Neurol 2013; 70:455–461
7.
Gordon BA, Najmi S, Hsu P, et al: The effects of white matter hyperintensities and amyloid deposition on Alzheimer dementia. Neuroimage Clin 2015; 8:246–252
8.
Dickie DA, Ritchie SJ, Cox SR, et al: Vascular risk factors and progression of white matter hyperintensities in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Neurobiol Aging 2016; 42:116–123
9.
Petersen RC: Clinical practice. Mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med 2011; 364:2227–2234
10.
Palmer K, Bäckman L, Winblad B, et al: Mild cognitive impairment in the general population: occurrence and progression to Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2008; 16:603–611
11.
Hsu WC, Chu YC, Fung HC, et al: Digit Symbol Substitution Test score and hyperhomocysteinemia in older adults. Medicine (Baltimore) 2016; 95:e4535
12.
Hua MS, Chang BS, Lin KN, et al: Wechsler Memory Scale [Manual], 3rd ed. [In Chinese.] Taipei, Chinese Behavioral Science Corp, 2005
13.
Hamsher Kd, Roberts RJ: Word Sequence Learning Test. Milwaukee, University of Wisconsin Medical School, Department of Neurology, 1983
14.
Hua MS, Chang SH, Chen ST: Factor structure and age effects with an aphasia test battery in normal Taiwanese adults. Neuropsychology 1997; 11:156–162
15.
Benton AL, Sivan AB, Hamsher KD, et al: Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: a Clinical Manual, 2nd ed. New York, Oxford University Press, 1994
16.
Chen JH, Chen HY: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale [Manual], 3rd ed. [In Chinese]. Taipei, Chinese Behavioral Science Corp, 2002.
17.
Nelson HE: A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 1976; 12:313–324
18.
Lin KN, Wang PN, Liu HC, et al: [Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument, Chinese version 2.0 (CASI C-2.0): administration and clinical application]. Acta Neurol Taiwan 2012; 21:180–189
19.
Morris JC: The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurology 1993; 43:2412–2414
20.
McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al: Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984; 34:939–944
21.
Morris JC, Storandt M, Miller JP, et al: Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 2001; 58:397–405
22.
Fazekas F, Chawluk JB, Alavi A, et al: MR signal abnormalities at 1.5 T in Alzheimer’s dementia and normal aging. AJR Am J Roentgenol 1987; 149:351–356
23.
Scheltens P, Barkhof F, Leys D, et al: A semiquantitative rating scale for the assessment of signal hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurol Sci 1993; 114:7–12
24.
Prins ND, Scheltens P: White matter hyperintensities, cognitive impairment and dementia: an update. Nat Rev Neurol 2015; 11:157–165
25.
Wardlaw JM, Smith EE, Biessels GJ, et al: Neuroimaging standards for research into small vessel disease and its contribution to ageing and neurodegeneration. Lancet Neurol 2013; 12:822–838
26.
Biesbroek JM, van Zandvoort MJ, Kappelle LJ, et al: Shared and distinct anatomical correlates of semantic and phonemic fluency revealed by lesion-symptom mapping in patients with ischemic stroke. Brain Struct Funct 2016; 221:2123–2134
27.
Jang JW, Kim S, Na HY, et al: Effect of white matter hyperintensity on medial temporal lobe atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease. Eur Neurol 2013; 69:229–235
28.
de Groot JC, de Leeuw FE, Oudkerk M, et al: Cerebral white matter lesions and cognitive function: the Rotterdam Scan Study. Ann Neurol 2000; 47:145–151
29.
Castaño EM, Maarouf CL, Wu T, et al: Alzheimer disease periventricular white matter lesions exhibit specific proteomic profile alterations. Neurochem Int 2013; 62:145–156
30.
Ouyang X, Chen K, Yao L, et al: Independent component analysis-based identification of covariance patterns of microstructural white matter damage in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0119714

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 130 - 138
PubMed: 29061091

History

Received: 14 May 2017
Revision received: 3 July 2017
Accepted: 2 August 2017
Published online: 24 October 2017
Published in print: Spring 2018

Keywords

  1. White matter hyperintensity
  2. medial temporal lobe atrophy
  3. mild cognitive impairment
  4. Alzheimer’s disease

Authors

Details

Yi-Chun Chen, M.D.
From the Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou Medical Center and College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Dementia Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Department of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (HH-T); the Department of Medical Imaging and Radiological Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Medical Imaging Research Center, Institute for Radiological Research, Chang Gung University, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Keelung, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Healthy Ageing Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Neuroscience Research Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Electrical Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JD-L); the Department of Laboratory Medicine, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at LinKou Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C); and the Department of Medical Biotechnology and Laboratory Science, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C).
Hsien-Hao Tsao
From the Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou Medical Center and College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Dementia Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Department of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (HH-T); the Department of Medical Imaging and Radiological Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Medical Imaging Research Center, Institute for Radiological Research, Chang Gung University, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Keelung, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Healthy Ageing Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Neuroscience Research Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Electrical Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JD-L); the Department of Laboratory Medicine, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at LinKou Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C); and the Department of Medical Biotechnology and Laboratory Science, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C).
Yi-Chuan Chu, M.S.
From the Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou Medical Center and College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Dementia Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Department of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (HH-T); the Department of Medical Imaging and Radiological Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Medical Imaging Research Center, Institute for Radiological Research, Chang Gung University, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Keelung, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Healthy Ageing Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Neuroscience Research Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Electrical Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JD-L); the Department of Laboratory Medicine, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at LinKou Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C); and the Department of Medical Biotechnology and Laboratory Science, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C).
Jiun-Jie Wang, Ph.D.
From the Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou Medical Center and College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Dementia Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Department of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (HH-T); the Department of Medical Imaging and Radiological Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Medical Imaging Research Center, Institute for Radiological Research, Chang Gung University, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Keelung, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Healthy Ageing Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Neuroscience Research Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Electrical Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JD-L); the Department of Laboratory Medicine, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at LinKou Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C); and the Department of Medical Biotechnology and Laboratory Science, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C).
Jiann-Der Lee, Ph.D.
From the Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou Medical Center and College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Dementia Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Department of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (HH-T); the Department of Medical Imaging and Radiological Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Medical Imaging Research Center, Institute for Radiological Research, Chang Gung University, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Keelung, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Healthy Ageing Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Neuroscience Research Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Electrical Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JD-L); the Department of Laboratory Medicine, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at LinKou Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C); and the Department of Medical Biotechnology and Laboratory Science, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C).
Pi-Yueh Chang, Ph.D.
From the Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou Medical Center and College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Dementia Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Department of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (HH-T); the Department of Medical Imaging and Radiological Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Medical Imaging Research Center, Institute for Radiological Research, Chang Gung University, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Keelung, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Healthy Ageing Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Neuroscience Research Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Electrical Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JD-L); the Department of Laboratory Medicine, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at LinKou Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C); and the Department of Medical Biotechnology and Laboratory Science, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C).
Wen-Chuin Hsu, M.D., M.P.H., M.S. [email protected]
From the Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou Medical Center and College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Dementia Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Taoyuan, Taiwan (YC-C, YC-C, WC-H); the Department of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (HH-T); the Department of Medical Imaging and Radiological Sciences, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Medical Imaging Research Center, Institute for Radiological Research, Chang Gung University, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Diagnostic Radiology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Keelung, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Healthy Ageing Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Neuroscience Research Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou, Taiwan (JJ-W); the Department of Electrical Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (JD-L); the Department of Laboratory Medicine, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at LinKou Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C); and the Department of Medical Biotechnology and Laboratory Science, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan (PY-C).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Hsu; e-mail: [email protected]

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

Chang Gung Memorial Hospital, Linkou10.13039/501100005795: CMRPG37136, CMRPG37172, CMRPG3E1501, and CMRPG3C17
Supported by CGMH (grants CMRPG37136, CMRPG37172, CMRPG3E1501, and CMRPG3C174).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share