Skip to main content
Full access
Articles
Published Online: 15 September 2017

The Role of Neighborhood Factors and Community Stigma in Predicting Community Participation Among Persons With Psychiatric Disabilities

Abstract

Objective:

This study examined the association between neighborhood characteristics, stigma related to mental illness reported by local community members, and measures of perceived stigma and community participation among individuals with psychiatric disabilities living in independent scattered-site housing or in congregate housing in three neighborhoods in the New York City metropolitan area.

Methods:

Neighborhood characteristics were drawn from the 2010 U.S. Census. Surveys focusing on attitudes and intended behavior toward people with mental illness were administered to 608 general community members, and clinical interviews were conducted with 343 persons with psychiatric disabilities.

Results:

Of neighborhood characteristics, both greater socioeconomic disadvantage and more “suburban values” (lower housing density and greater political conservativism) predicted more perpetrated stigma reported by community members. There was no significant relationship between stigma reported by community members and perceived stigma among participants with psychiatric disabilities. Community stigma predicted vocational involvement and demonstrated interaction effects with housing, such that persons living in congregate housing demonstrated more community participation in communities with more stigma, whereas persons living in scattered-site housing demonstrated less participation in these communities. Perceived stigma was significantly negatively related to community participation.

Conclusions:

Findings suggest that effects of neighborhood characteristics and community stigma on people with psychiatric disabilities are complex and are partly conditioned by housing context.
As psychiatric services have shifted from institutions to communities, interest has burgeoned in predictors of community participation (1), defined as “natural engagement with others in a community-based context” (2). Neighborhood characteristics are a relevant consideration given evidence that housing for people with psychiatric disabilities tends to be disproportionately located in economically disadvantaged areas (3). The contribution of neighborhood factors (such as concentrated disadvantage) to individual-level outcomes has been well documented (4,5), and it is plausible that community characteristics influence the community participation of people with psychiatric disabilities. Early research in this area found that people with psychiatric disabilities demonstrated more community participation in “liberal, nontraditional” (6) versus “conservative, middle-class” neighborhoods. Recent research has found that neighborhood factors are associated with sense of community among people with psychiatric disabilities (7,8) but has not specifically examined community participation.
Community stigma—that is, community members’ stigmatizing beliefs about and attitudes toward mental illness—is important when considering neighborhood characteristics that may affect community participation. Research has consistently documented the persistence of stigma related to mental illness (9,10), including recent identification of microaggressions, or subtle discriminatory behaviors, as additional forms of stigma experienced by people with psychiatric disabilities (11). Neighborhood factors, including socioeconomic disadvantage and political conservatism, have been associated with community members’ endorsement of stigmatizing attitudes (12). There is also evidence that stigmatizing attitudes are associated with discrimination that can restrict opportunities for community participation (13,14). Furthermore, individuals with psychiatric disabilities’ perceptions of community stigma have been shown to be associated with diminished community participation (15).
This study sought to address several unanswered questions regarding the relationship between neighborhood characteristics, community stigma, and community participation among individuals with psychiatric disabilities. First, does stigma (both endorsed by community members and perceived by persons with psychiatric disabilities) vary as a function of neighborhood disadvantage? Second, is there a relationship between the stigmatizing behaviors endorsed by local community members, and stigmatizing behaviors perceived by persons with psychiatric disabilities living in those communities? Third, does the degree of local community stigma and stigma perceived by individuals with psychiatric disabilities have an impact on their community participation? We hypothesized that more stigma would be endorsed and perceived in neighborhoods with greater disadvantage, that the degree of stigma endorsed by community members would be associated with the degree of stigma perceived by persons with psychiatric disabilities, and that more stigma endorsed and perceived would be associated with lower reported community participation.

Methods

Participants

Participants included two samples: 608 general community members and 343 community members with psychiatric disabilities who were receiving housing treatment services as defined by the New York State Office of Mental Health (NYSOMH) (16). For brevity, the samples are referred to below as “community members” and “participants with psychiatric disabilities.” Samples were recruited from three neighborhoods in the New York City metropolitan area: East/Central Harlem in Manhattan (zip codes 10027, 10029, 10030, and 10035), Crown Heights/East Flatbush in Brooklyn (11212, 11213, 11225, and 11226), and Yonkers/Mount Vernon in Westchester (10550, 10701, and 10705). Neighborhoods were chosen because they represent a range of socioeconomic characteristics and include high concentrations of people with psychiatric disabilities living in scattered-site and congregate housing, as determined by a prior feasibility study.
There were no statistically significant differences in demographic characteristics between community members and participants with psychiatric disabilities (Table 1). Compared with population data from the 2010 U.S. Census, the overall sample (community members and participants with psychiatric disabilities) had a greater proportion of males and a smaller proportion of persons identifying as white or Hispanic/Latino. In addition, both samples had a higher median age than the general population, which was probably attributable to the study’s being restricted to adults. The characteristics of participants with psychiatric disabilities were similar to those of public mental health service recipients in New York City, as reported by NYSOMH in its 2015 survey of patient characteristics (17).
TABLE 1. Characteristics of community members and participants with psychiatric disabilities
 Community members (N=608)Participants with psychiatric disability (N=343)
CharacteristicN%aN%a
Gender    
 Male3515921162
 Female2464112938
Race-ethnicity    
 African American3485919758
 Hispanic/Latino110184212
 White/European American59104313
 Asian American1421<1
 Native American7151
 Arab/Middle Eastern4<11<1
 Other5295315
Diagnosis    
 Schizophrenia  9537
 Schizoaffective disorder  4618
 Bipolar disorder  4016
 Major depressive disorder  3614
 Posttraumatic stress disorder  42
 Personality disorder  31
 Anxiety disorder  1<1
 Other  21
Age (M±SD years)41.98±15.71 47.9±11.4 
Education (M±SD years)13.1±3.0 12.0±3.1 
Age at first hospitalization (M±SD years)  24.6±12.3 
N of prior hospitalizations  10.3±15.7 
a
Percentage are based on the total N and do not account for missing values.

Procedure

Community member participants.

Institutional review board approval was received from all participating institutions. We recruited community member participants between March 2014 and December 2015. Approximately 200 community members were surveyed in each area, with a minimum of 50 respondents per zip code (with the exception of one area where safety concerns limited street outreach) by trained research assistants, who distributed flyers in public locations. Eligibility criteria included being 18 or older, speaking English well enough to complete interviews, and living or working within the targeted area. After providing informed consent, participants completed questionnaires, either administered orally by the research assistant or completed independently—depending on preference. Interviews lasted approximately five to ten minutes, and compensation was $10.

Participants with psychiatric disabilities.

Within each site, 50–60 participants were recruited from two types of housing treatment facilities: independent scattered-site and congregate housing. Participants were recruited from four public-sector clinics and eight housing agencies between March 2014 and December 2015 via announcements at common areas at the site (for example, housing programs’ community meetings) and flyers distributed to potentially eligible participants by housing agencies. When potential participants were not recruited at their residence, they were asked the name and address of their housing agency; its location within a targeted zip code was confirmed with a map and its category confirmed by using New York City’s housing vacancy update (18). Interviews were scheduled after eligibility was determined and informed consent was obtained. Of the persons approached who met inclusion criteria, 13% refused to participate. In addition, 6% of persons who were likely aware of the study at the recruitment sites did not express interest in participating. We estimated that roughly 80% of eligible individuals participated. Interviews lasted from one to two hours, and compensation was $30.

Measures

Community members.

Demographic data were collected with a questionnaire including age, gender, race-ethnicity, education, zip code of residence, and residence or employment within the targeted area.
Stigmatizing attitudes were measured with the Attitudes Toward Mental Illness Scale (AMIS) (19), an 11-item measure scored on a 1- to 5-point scale, with higher overall scores indicating more stigma. AMIS consists of two factors, negative stereotypes (Cronbach’s α=.66) and recovery and outcomes (Cronbach’s α=.69).
Stigmatizing behaviors were measured with the Reported and Intended Behavior Scale (RIBS) (20) and Mental Illness Microaggressions Scale–Perpetrator Version (MIMS-P) (11). The RIBS includes eight items of intended interaction with individuals with mental illness, with four items regarding intended behavior (for example, “In the future, I would be willing to live with someone with a mental health problem”). Internal consistency in our sample was adequate (Cronbach’s α=.74). The MIMS-P includes 17 items measuring microaggression experiences perpetrated toward individuals with mental illness (a 14-item version of the scale was included in this study). Each item is rated on a scale from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating more microaggressions. Internal consistency was high in the sample (Cronbach’s α=.85).

Participants with psychiatric disabilities.

Demographic data included age, race-ethnicity, education, length of time in the current residence, age at first hospitalization, and number of previous hospitalizations. Psychiatric diagnosis was assessed, with consent, via a review of the clinical face sheet from participant’s charts.
Psychiatric symptoms were measured with the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (21), a rating scale for 24 different psychiatric symptoms. We used a subscale-scoring approach (22), dividing items into five factors: affect, positive symptoms, negative symptoms, resistance, and activation.

Community participation.

Physical community integration was measured with a modified 19-item version of the External Community Integration Scale (ECI) (23), assessing number of days in the past two weeks when activities were completed outside the household (for example, going to a movie and attending religious services). Internal consistency was acceptable in our sample (Cronbach’s α=.57). Social community integration was measured with a 12-item scale (24) assessing frequency of interactions between the participant and community members (for example, borrowing items and social outings). Two versions were administered: one related to interactions with neighbors (Cronbach’s α=.85) and the other to interactions with family and friends (Cronbach’s α=.88). Civic engagement was measured with the Social Capital Short Form (25), assessing involvement in activities indicative of citizenship (for example, community service and voting). Internal consistency was poor (Cronbach’s α=.45), with low item endorsement; thus we dichotomized this variable (none versus any involvement).
Social functioning was assessed with the Quality of Life Scale (QLS) (26), a 21-item rating scale completed after a semistructured interview. For this study, we were interested in three of the four QLS factor scores most related to social functioning: interpersonal relations measures the frequency of recent social contacts, instrumental functioning measures vocational functioning, and common objects and activities measures participation in common societal activities (for example, reading a newspaper).
When a reported response reflected the impossible or improbable (for example, length of time in the community that exceeded the participant’s age), responses were considered missing. Mean imputation was used to include scales in cases when specific item values were missing. Scales were not included in cases when more than 50% of items were missing.

Perceived microaggressions scale.

Items were developed based on five identified categories from a focus group study of microaggression experiences: invalidation, assumption of inferiority, fear of mental illness, shaming of mental illness, and second-class citizen (27). Items were created on the basis of participant responses related to interactions with community members (for example, “When I am in public, other community members avoid being physically close to me because of my mental illness”) and scored on a 1–4 scale, with higher scores indicating more microaggression experiences. The scale demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α=.83).

Analyses

Community participation data reduction.

We first conducted an exploratory factor analysis to reduce the large number of community participation variables. We included all community participation variables except for citizenship, which was excluded because of low internal consistency. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was .71, above the recommended value of .5 (28), indicating that the sample size was appropriate. The scree plot indicated a three-factor solution, and principal-components analysis with direct oblimin rotation was employed. Using a cutoff factor loading of .4, factor 1 (social community participation) included QLS interpersonal relationships, SCI with neighbors, and SCI with friends and family; factor 2 (vocational involvement) consisted of the QLS instrumental functioning subscale only; and factor 3 (physical community participation) included the QLS common objects and activities factor and the ECI. We combined scales that loaded on the same factors by converting them to standard scores and summing them.

Neighborhood composite variables.

Neighborhood-level data were gathered linking participants to characteristics drawn from the 2010 U.S. Census. For a large number of participants (14% of the community sample), we did not have enough data to identify census tract because of inaccuracy of reported cross streets or unwillingness to disclose addresses. Because zip code–level data were available for all participants, all neighborhood data were gathered at the zip code level. Neighborhood characteristics were first analyzed with exploratory factor analysis to create composite variables. Variables were divided into two sets for factor analysis to obtain adequate indicators of sampling adequacy.
Set 1 included unemployment rate, median income, percentage of families below the federal poverty level, percentage of high school graduates, percentage receiving public assistance, and percentage of female-headed households for each zip code. These characteristics were included together because they have typically been used in previous neighborhood disadvantage research (29,30). The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy was adequate (28). Because of low communality, unemployment rate was removed. Communalities for all remaining items were above .6, indicating that individual items shared common variance with other items. Principal-components analysis with direct oblimin rotation using Kaiser normalization was employed with a two-factor solution (following examination of the scree plot), and the resulting factors were named socioeconomic disadvantage and vocational disadvantage.
Set 2 included the percentage of vacant housing units, residential stability (percentage of residents moving within the past five years), percentage of foreign-born individuals, GINI index, population density by square mile, and conservative voting habits. The GINI index is a measure of unequal income distribution, ranging from 0 to 1, with higher values indicating greater inequality. Vacant housing, residential stability, and percentage of foreign-born individuals have been used as indicators of neighborhood disadvantage in previous studies (29,31). Population density, conservative voting, and the GINI index were chosen on the basis of prior work indicating that political affiliation and “family orientation” (high proportion of single-family units) are predictors of interest (6). All variables were drawn from the 2010 census at the zip code level, except for conservative voting, which was determined at the congressional district level with the percentage of individuals who voted for the 2012 Republican presidential nominee. Because of low communality, residential stability was removed from the analysis. A two-factor solution was determined, and the factors were named suburban values and income inequality.

Level 2 variance.

Hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) was first employed because of expected variation at the neighborhood level. Unconditional models were estimated to examine intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs); ICCs provide the proportion of variance at level 2 (neighborhood) to the total variance of the model and are used to assess whether HLM is necessary. Across hypotheses, ICCs ranged from .01 to .1; this determined that there was not enough variance at the neighborhood level to continue with HLM, and the model was reduced to a linear regression analysis. Summary scores on the AMIS, RIBS, and MIMS-P at the zip code level are provided in Table 2.
TABLE 2. Characteristics at the zip code level of three neighborhoods in the New York City metropolitan areaa
 HarlemYonkers/Mt. VernonBrooklyn
Characteristic1002710029100301003510550107011070511212112131122511226
Unemployment rate (%)4.17.59.36.311.558.27.26.2138.8
Residential stability (%)b51.252.450.145.460.051.659.049.344.248.744.7
Median income43,26639,37541,45344,49437,43639,97240,69333,89235,71541,08234,610
Families below poverty level (%)25.6252535.216.620.316.731.727.417.819.5
High school graduates (%)81.973.777.064.081.675.176.975.078.883.179.8
Families receiving public assistance (%)5.28.45.76.58.36.65.77.97.94.35.6
Female-headed households (%)23.927.226.427.430.723.323.743.026.825.129.2
Vacant housing units (%)11.58.510.49.913.015.510.28.510.06.78.8
Foreign-born individuals (%)27.826.821.025.336.632.437.033.336.740.850.4
GINI coefficientc.5854.6080.5566.5584.4486.4873.4938.5028.4853.4655.4601
Population densityd68,51392,11696,79024,32918,69615,00717,52154,94458,44864,29578,775
Conservative voting habits (%)4.64.64.621.525.525.525.513.910.313.913.9
Reported and Intended Behaviors Scalee3.823.963.813.843.694.033.653.843.803.843.78
Attitudes Toward Mental Illness Scalef2.362.212.352.422.352.242.532.352.582.462.43
Mental Illness Microaggressions Scale–Perpetrator Versiong2.552.752.782.692.702.972.72.902.872.792.73
a
East/Central Harlem (N=109 participants with psychiatric disabilities); Yonkers/Mt.Vernon (N=111); Crown Heights/East Flatbush, Brooklyn (N=123)
b
Percentage of residents moving within the past five years
c
A measure of unequal income distribution, ranging from 0 to 1, with higher values indicating greater inequality.
d
N of residents per square mile
e
Possible scores range from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating higher levels of desired social distance.
f
Possible scores range from 1 to 5, with higher scores indicating more stigmatizing attitudes.
g
Possible scores range from 1 to 4, with higher scores indicating more microaggression behavior.

Results

Hypothesis 1: Neighborhood-Level Predictors of Stigma

Community member–endorsed stigma.

Table 3 presents results of a regression analysis examining the relationship between neighborhood factors and endorsed stigma by community members. Partial support was found for the hypothesis that neighborhood disadvantage would be associated with more community stigma. Socioeconomic disadvantage was associated with more microaggressions endorsed by community members (MIMS-P total). Community members in areas with more “suburban values” (less dense and more politically conservative) also reported fewer recovery-oriented attitudes (AMIS recovery) and more microaggressions.
TABLE 3. Neighborhood characteristics as predictors of perpetrated stigma among community members
 AMISa 
 TotalNegative stereotypesRecoveryRIBS totalbMIMS-P totalc 
VariableβSEβSEβSEβSEβSEFd
Constant.58.91.74.83.45
Socioeconomic disadvantage–.09.01–.04.02–.07.02.03.02.11*.012.1*
Vocational disadvantage.15.030.05.20*.04–.11.05–.16.031.3
Suburban values–.12.02.05.03–.21*.03.10.03.21*.021.8
Income inequality–.24*.04–.06*.06–.26*.05.15.05.17.031.9
Unemployment–.41.02.02.03–.07.02.01.02.05.01.7
Residential stability.11.01–.09.02.22*.02–.15.02–.23*.012*
a
Attitudes Toward Mental Illness Scale
b
Reported and Intended Behaviors Scale
c
Mental Illness Microaggressions Scale–Perpetrator Version
d
df=8 and 588
*
p<.05
Conversely, vocational disadvantage was associated with more positive community member attitudes toward mental illness (AMIS recovery), and residential instability was also associated with more positive attitudes and fewer microaggressions endorsed by community members. A significant negative relationship was also found between income inequality and scores on the AMIS, such that individuals living in areas with greater inequality reported fewer negative stereotypes. However, community members in these areas also endorsed fewer recovery-oriented attitudes toward mental illness.

Stigma perceived by participants with psychiatric disabilities.

No significant relationship was noted between neighborhood variables and microaggressions perceived by participants with psychiatric disabilities. Exploratory analyses indicated, however, that, despite their being located in the same communities, participants in congregate housing perceived significantly more microaggressions than participants in independent scattered-site housing (β=.19, p≤.01). We thus explored whether there was an interaction between housing and neighborhood factors in predicting perceived stigma with product terms for housing category and each neighborhood predictor. Significant interactions were noted between housing and vocational disadvantage (β=–.36, p<.05) and between housing and suburban values (β=.31, p<.05). Participants in independent scattered-site housing perceived more microaggressions in communities with more vocational disadvantage, while participants in congregate housing perceived more microaggressions in neighborhoods with more suburban values.

Hypothesis 2: Community Member–Endorsed Stigma and Perceived Stigma

The relationship was evaluated by using multiple regression analysis. No statistically significant relationship was found between measures of community member–reported stigmatizing attitudes and behaviors toward people with mental illness and measures of stigma perceived by participants with psychiatric disabilities in their community.

Hypothesis 3: Stigma and Community Participation

Community member–endorsed stigma.

Table 4 provides results from multiple regression analyses. Contrary to expectations, community member microaggressions had a significant positive relationship to vocational involvement when the analysis controlled for negative symptoms, such that individuals with psychiatric disabilities living in areas with more community member–reported microaggressions demonstrated higher levels of vocational involvement. However, when housing type was included in the analyses, a significant interaction between neighborhood microaggressions and housing type was found for physical community participation and vocational involvement. For individuals in congregate housing, living in areas with higher levels of community microaggressions was positively related to vocational outcomes and participation in routine activities in the community, whereas the reverse was found for participants living in independent scattered-site housing in these communities. Figure 1 illustrates the nature of the interaction for physical community participation.
TABLE 4. Community stigma and other variables as a predictors of community participation among participants with psychiatric disabilities
 Social community participationPhysical community participationVocational involvement  
VariableβSEβSEβSE Fa
Constant.35.23.22 
Community microaggressions.08.06.09.04.19**.04 3.12*
Housing categoryb.03.26.21**.17.04.16 5.9**
Microaggressions × housing–.12.1–.14*.07–.18**.06 3.68*
Negative symptomsc.19**1.3.21**.85.15**.81 7.98**
a
df=8 and 327
b
0=scattered site; 1=congregate
c
Reverse coded
*
p<.05, **p<.01
FIGURE 1. Microaggressions reported by community members and total factor scores for physical community participation among participants with psychiatric disabilities, by housing typea
aScores for microaggressions range from 1 to 4 with higher scores indicating more microaggression behavior. Scores for physical community participation range from 0 to 7 with higher scores indicating less impairment in social functioning.

Stigma perceived by participants with psychiatric disabilities.

In multiple regression analysis including perceived microaggressions, housing, and negative symptoms, the overall model significantly predicted community participation (social community participation, F=6.68, df=3 and 324, p<.01, r2=.06; physical community participation, F=14.45, df=3 and 324, p<.01, r2=.06; physical community participation, F=14.45, df=3 and 324, p<.01, r2=.12; and vocational involvement, F=3.99, df=3 and 324, p<.01, r2=.03). Individually, perceived microaggressions had a statistically significant relationship with social community participation (β=–.13, p<.05) and with physical community participation (β=–.15, p<.01) but not with vocational involvement, when the analysis controlled for negative symptoms. Individuals who perceived more community microaggressions demonstrated significantly less social and physical community participation. When housing type (scattered site or congregate) was added to the model, it was significantly associated only with physical community participation, such that individuals living in scattered-site housing demonstrated higher levels of physical community participation (β=–.17, p<.01).

Discussion

We found partial support for the hypothesized relationship between neighborhood characteristics and stigma related to mental illness. For stigma endorsed by community members, the level of socioeconomic disadvantage in a community was related to more microaggressions reported by the community members, consistent with previous findings of a relationship between income and socioeconomic status and stigmatizing attitudes (12,32). Community members living in less dense and more conservative areas endorsed more microaggressions, consistent with previous studies that have identified less density (33) and conservative political affiliation (12,34) as correlates of stigma. The hypothesized impact of neighborhood characteristics on stigma perceived by participants with psychiatric disabilities was significant only when the moderating effect of housing type was considered. Participants living in less dense and more politically conservative communities perceived more stigma only when they lived in congregate housing, whereas participants living in scattered-site housing tended to perceive more stigma in areas with more vocational disadvantage.
Contrary to hypotheses, perpetrated microaggression behavior in a community did not correspond to the perceptions of individuals with psychiatric disabilities in those communities. This finding, although unexpected, is consistent with previous research indicating that individuals with psychiatric disabilities anticipate discrimination even when they do not objectively experience it (14). An additional explanation for this finding could be that measurement of community stigma was limited to the zip code level. The lack of findings from these analyses should thus be interpreted with caution, because it does not necessarily mean that no relationship exists between stigmatizing behaviors of community members and perceived stigma by individuals with psychiatric disabilities.
Participants with psychiatric disabilities who lived in communities with more community member–endorsed microaggressions demonstrated better community participation but only when they lived in congregate housing. This finding suggests that people with psychiatric disabilities may experience certain neighborhood characteristics differently depending on their housing context. Residents of congregate housing might benefit from relative anonymity in denser and less conservative areas, because community members might be less likely to become aware of their diagnostic status. Conversely, residents of independent scattered-site housing, who are generally better able to maintain anonymity, may benefit more from the resource opportunities of less dense communities with higher education, income, and employment rates.
Consistent with prior research, perceived stigma predicted community participation of participants with psychiatric disabilities, even when the analysis controlled for negative symptoms (the most consistent predictor of community participation in analyses not reported here). This suggests that community participation of individuals with psychiatric disabilities is influenced not only by psychopathology but also by perceived experiences of social exclusion.
Several study limitations should be acknowledged. Neighborhoods included only three New York City metropolitan areas, and results may not be generalizable to other urban or suburban areas. In addition, street-outreach recruitment methods may limit generalizability of our sample, and self-report survey methods may have included social desirability bias. We were unable to include neighborhood variables at the census tract level in our analyses, instead using characteristics at the zip code level; this limited potential variability, because one zip code may contain several “communities” that are different from one another. Our measure of perceived microaggression experiences, although demonstrating good internal consistency, had not been previously validated. We did not include measures of perceived community support and social climate, constructs associated with community integration that might have further elucidated our findings (35,36).

Conclusions

This study has potential utility for informing antistigma interventions targeting specific neighborhoods in which stigma is more prevalent, including those with lower income, less population density, and more conservative political attitudes. Findings suggest that stigma has an impact on community participation, although the nature of the relationship is conditioned by the housing context. Findings also suggest that independent scattered-site housing might optimally facilitate community participation when located in less economically disadvantaged areas, whereas congregate housing might facilitate such participation in more urban areas with liberal attitudes. However, further research in more diverse settings is needed to replicate this finding.

Acknowledgments

The study was funded by the National Institute on Disability, Independent Living and Rehabilitation Research (H133G130086). The authors acknowledge the hard work of Radoslava Mechkyurova, M.A., George Anderson, M.A., and John Vaccaro, M.A., in completing the project.

References

1.
Yanos PT: Beyond “Landscapes of Despair”: the need for new research on the urban environment, sprawl, and the community integration of persons with severe mental illness. Health and Place 13:672–676, 2007
2.
Salzer MS, Brusilovskiy E, Prvu-Bettger J, et al: Measuring community participation of adults with psychiatric disabilities: reliability of two modes of data collection. Rehabilitation Psychology 59:211–219, 2014
3.
Byrne T, Prvu Bettger J, Brusilovskiy E, et al: Comparing neighborhoods of adults with serious mental illness and of the general population: research implications. Psychiatric Services 64:782–788, 2013
4.
Sampson RJ, Raudenbush SW: Seeing disorder: neighborhood stigma and the social construction of “broken windows.” Social Psychology Quarterly 67:319–342, 2004
5.
Hill TD, Maimon D: Neighborhood context and mental health; in Handbook of the Sociology of Mental Health. Amsterdam, Springer, 2012
6.
Segal SP, Baumohl J, Moyles EW: Neighborhood types and community reaction to the mentally ill: a paradox of intensity. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 21:345–359, 1980
7.
Brusilovskiy E, Salzer MS: A study of environmental influences on the well-being of individuals with psychiatric disabilities in Philadelphia, PA. Social Science and Medicine 74:1591–1601, 2012
8.
Ecker J, Aubry T: Individual, housing, and neighborhood predictors of psychological integration among vulnerably housed and homeless individuals. American Journal of Community Psychology 58:111–122, 2016
9.
Martin JK, Pescosolido BA, Tuch SA: Of fear and loathing: the role of “disturbing behavior,” labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 41:208–223, 2000
10.
Reavley NJ, Jorm AF: Stigmatizing attitudes towards people with mental disorders: findings from an Australian National Survey of Mental Health Literacy and Stigma. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 45:1086–1093, 2011
11.
Gonzales L, Davidoff KC, DeLuca JS, et al: The Mental Illness Microaggressions Scale–Perpetrator Version (MIMS-P): reliability and validity. Psychiatry Research 229:120–125, 2015
12.
Gonzales L, Chan G, Yanos PT: Individual and neighborhood predictors of stigmatizing attitudes toward mental illness in New York State. Stigma and Health 2:175–181, 2017
13.
González-Torres MA, Oraa R, Arístegui M, et al: Stigma and discrimination towards people with schizophrenia and their family members: a qualitative study with focus groups. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 42:14–23, 2007
14.
Cechnicki A, Angermeyer MC, Bielańska A: Anticipated and experienced stigma among people with schizophrenia: its nature and correlates. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 46:643–650, 2011
15.
Prince PN, Prince CR: Perceived stigma and community integration among clients of assertive community treatment. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal 25:323–331, 2002
16.
Supportive Housing Options NYC: A Guide to Supportive Housing Models for People Living With Mental Illness. New York, Center for Urban Community Services, 2015. http://www.cidny.org/resources/Services/Housing/Supportive%20Housing%20Options%20-2015%20edition.pdf
17.
Patient Characteristics Survey Summary Reports. Albany, New York State Office of Mental Health. https://my.omh.ny.gov/bi/pcs. Accessed Aug 21, 2017
18.
Housing Vacancy Update. New York, Center for Urban Community Services. https://www.cucs.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/08/HP-New-Vacancy-Update.pdf. Accessed Nov 7, 2016
19.
Kobau R, Diiorio C, Chapman D, et al: Attitudes about mental illness and its treatment: validation of a generic scale for public health surveillance of mental illness associated stigma. Community Mental Health Journal 46:164–176, 2010
20.
Evans-Lacko S, Rose D, Little K, et al: Development and psychometric properties of the Reported and Intended Behaviour Scale (RIBS): a stigma-related behaviour measure. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 20:263–271, 2011
21.
Overall JE, Gorham DR: The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychological Reports 10:799–812, 1962
22.
Mueser KT, Curran PJ, McHugo GJ: Factor structure of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale in schizophrenia. Psychological Assessment 9:196–204, 1997
23.
Segal SP, Aviram U: The Mentally Ill in Community Based Sheltered Care: A Study of Community Care and Social Integration. New York, Wiley, 1978
24.
Aubry T, Myner J: Community integration and quality of life: a comparison of persons with psychiatric disabilities in housing programs and community residents who are neighbors. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health 15:5–20, 1996
25.
Putnam RD: Social Capital Community Benchmark Survey Short Form. Cambridge, MA, Harvard Kennedy School of Government, Saguaro Seminar, 2002. www.hks.harvard.edu/saguaro/pdfs/socialcapitalshortform.pdf
26.
Heinrichs DW, Hanlon TE, Carpenter WT Jr: The Quality of Life Scale: an instrument for rating the schizophrenic deficit syndrome. Schizophrenia Bulletin 10:388–398, 1984
27.
Gonzales L, Davidoff KC, Nadal KL, et al: Microaggressions experienced by persons with mental illnesses: an exploratory study. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal 38:234–241, 2015
28.
Stevens JP: Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, 1992
29.
Joongbaeck K: Neighborhood disadvantage and mental health: the role of neighborhood disorder and social relationships. Social Science Research 39:260–271, 2010
30.
Krishan S, Bakeman R, Broussard B, et al: The influence of neighborhood characteristics on police officers’ encounters with persons suspected to have a serious mental illness. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 37:359–369, 2014
31.
Silver E: Race, neighborhood disadvantage, and violence among persons with mental disorders: the importance of contextual measurement. Law and Human Behavior 24:449–456, 2000
32.
Phelan JC, Link BG: Fear of people with mental illnesses: the role of personal and impersonal contact and exposure to threat or harm. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 45:68–80, 2004
33.
Leviten-Reid C, Johnson P, Miller M: Supported housing in a small community: effects on consumers, suggestions for change. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health 33:57–69, 2014
34.
Watson AC, Corrigan PW, Angell B: What motivates public support for legally mandated mental health treatment? Social Work Research 29:87–94, 2005
35.
Kloos B, Townley G: Investigating the relationship between neighborhood experiences and psychiatric distress for individuals with serious mental illness. Administration and Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research 38:105–116, 2011
36.
Townley G, Miller H, Kloos B: A little goes a long way: the impact of distal social support on community integration and recovery of individuals with psychiatric disabilities. American Journal of Community Psychology 52:84–96, 2013

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Psychiatric Services
Go to Psychiatric Services

Cover: Understanding, by Michael Olszewski, 1984. Dyed and pleated silk plain weave with silk embroidery. Philadelphia Museum of Art, gift of Nancy and David Bergman, 2015.

Psychiatric Services
Pages: 76 - 83
PubMed: 28945180

History

Received: 6 April 2017
Revision received: 30 May 2017
Accepted: 16 June 2017
Published online: 15 September 2017
Published in print: January 01, 2018

Keywords

  1. Neighborhood
  2. Community Participation
  3. Stigma

Authors

Details

Lauren Gonzales, Ph.D.
When this work was done, Dr. Gonzales was at John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York, New York, where Dr. Yanos is affiliated. She is currently a postdoctoral fellow in the Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco. Dr. Stefancic is with the Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York. Dr. Alexander is with the Division of Social Solutions and Services Research, Nathan S. Kline Institute, Orangeburg, New York. Ms. Harney-Delehanty is with the Department of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, New Jersey.
Philip T. Yanos, Ph.D. [email protected]
When this work was done, Dr. Gonzales was at John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York, New York, where Dr. Yanos is affiliated. She is currently a postdoctoral fellow in the Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco. Dr. Stefancic is with the Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York. Dr. Alexander is with the Division of Social Solutions and Services Research, Nathan S. Kline Institute, Orangeburg, New York. Ms. Harney-Delehanty is with the Department of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, New Jersey.
Ana Stefancic, Ph.D.
When this work was done, Dr. Gonzales was at John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York, New York, where Dr. Yanos is affiliated. She is currently a postdoctoral fellow in the Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco. Dr. Stefancic is with the Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York. Dr. Alexander is with the Division of Social Solutions and Services Research, Nathan S. Kline Institute, Orangeburg, New York. Ms. Harney-Delehanty is with the Department of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, New Jersey.
Mary Jane Alexander, Ph.D.
When this work was done, Dr. Gonzales was at John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York, New York, where Dr. Yanos is affiliated. She is currently a postdoctoral fellow in the Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco. Dr. Stefancic is with the Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York. Dr. Alexander is with the Division of Social Solutions and Services Research, Nathan S. Kline Institute, Orangeburg, New York. Ms. Harney-Delehanty is with the Department of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, New Jersey.
Brianna Harney-Delehanty, M.A.
When this work was done, Dr. Gonzales was at John Jay College of Criminal Justice, City University of New York, New York, where Dr. Yanos is affiliated. She is currently a postdoctoral fellow in the Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco. Dr. Stefancic is with the Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University, New York. Dr. Alexander is with the Division of Social Solutions and Services Research, Nathan S. Kline Institute, Orangeburg, New York. Ms. Harney-Delehanty is with the Department of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University, Teaneck, New Jersey.

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Yanos (e-mail: [email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

National Institute on Disability, Independent Living, and Rehabilitation Research10.13039/100009157: H133G130086

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share