Skip to main content
Full access
Frontline Report
Published Online: 1 January 2002

Computer-Facilitated Dialogue With Patients Who Have Schizophrenia

Psychotherapy conducted in a conversational mode relies on auditory language processing, which involves concentration, attention, and working memory. All of these faculties may be compromised in people who have schizophrenia. Some case studies that two colleagues and I published in this journal in October 1997 showed that visual representation of spoken words, displayed by typing the conversation between patient and therapist on a computer screen, may compensate for some of these deficits and enhance communication.
At Taunton (Mass.) State Hospital we conducted a pilot study of the efficacy of this approach with ten patients diagnosed as having schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. The study was approved by the research review committee of the Massachusetts Department of Mental Health, and all participants provided written informed consent. We divided the patients into an experimental group and a control group, each comprising four men and one woman.
The mean±SD age of those in the experimental group was 37.8±6.3 years, and they had a mean of 10.6±1.1 years of education; their first psychiatric hospitalization occurred at a mean age of 28.8±7 years. In the control group the mean age was 44.2±14.3 years, the mean educational attainment was 10.4±1.8 years, and the mean age at first psychiatric hospitalization was 25.8±14.7 years.
All participants received eight 20- to 30-minute sessions over two to four months. Therapists followed a guideline to select common themes and issues that were to be discussed in the sessions. Topics included the treatment plan and goals, the patient's current functioning, medication issues, identification of treatment team members and their roles, and reasons for hospitalization. In the experimental group, the patient and the therapist sat in front of a computer, with the therapist at the keyboard. The therapist typed a brief version of each question, followed by the patient's responses verbatim. A transcript was given to the patient after each session. For the control group, identical issues were discussed with the usual conversational modality.
All patients answered a 29-item questionnaire administered by the unit nurse before and after the eight treatment sessions. The answers fell into two broad categories: a "negative response" category that included "No, I don't know" or any type of vague or irrelevant response, and a "productive association" category that included any meaningful or relevant response.
The control and experimental groups did not show any significant difference at the pretreatment assessment. For the control group, a mean±SD of 8.3±5.6 answers were categorized as negative responses and 25.8±9.6 as productive associations; for the experimental group the figures were 8.8±7.4 and 27.9±12.5, respectively.
At the posttreatment assessment, the experimental group showed a significantly lower rate of negative responses (5.3±3.75 compared with 11±3.66; t=−2.61, df=4, p=.01; one-tailed test) and a significantly higher rate of productive associations (34.1±7.8 compared with 26±7.7; t=−2.76, df=4, p=.02; one-tailed test).
In a comparison of within-group pretreatment and posttreatment differences, the predicted improvement for productive associations was significant for the experimental group (t=2.25, df=4, p=.04; one-tailed test). The predicted decrease in the negative response category was not large enough to be statistically significant. For the control group, there was no significant change in either of the two variables.
The results of this pilot study suggest that therapeutic communication accompanied by visual representation of spoken words may be more effective in increasing associations and productive thoughts related to the issues the therapist and the patient discuss. The technique may help compensate for deficits in verbal memory, working memory, and attentional process, which can interfere with communication. Such compensation may help patients with schizophrenia better understand and remember the issues discussed in therapeutic encounters. Controlled studies using a longer time frame and a larger sample could help to validate this hypothesis and assess whether the effect persists.

Footnote

Dr. Ahmed is a consulting psychologist at Northern Rhode Island Community Mental Health Center and a part-time psychologist at Taunton State Hospital, P.O. Box 4007, Taunton, Massachusetts 02780 (e-mail, [email protected] ).

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Psychiatric Services
Go to Psychiatric Services
Psychiatric Services
Pages: 99-a - 100

History

Published online: 1 January 2002
Published in print: January 2002

Authors

Affiliations

Mohiuddin Ahmed, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share