The majority of empirical studies on the importance of attachment patterns in individual psychotherapy with adults have dealt with the ways in which client attachment patterns affect the therapeutic process. It has been hypothesized that clients with different attachment patterns will behave differently and will make different use of psychotherapy. This in turn might have predictable effects on the quality of the therapeutic alliance and on how therapists react to clients. Although theoretically just as important, fewer studies have looked into the effect of therapist attachment patterns on the therapeutic process.
Client attachment patterns and client in-treatment behaviour
The most immediate effects of client attachment patterns that have been studied in a psychotherapy process context are differences in client behaviour in therapy.
Dozier (1990) studied 42 psychiatric patients' involvement in treatment, and
Korfmacher, Adam, Ogawa, and Egeland (1997) studied the way that 55 mothers at-risk made use of a preventive intervention programme. In both cases, attachment patterns of clients were measured with the AAI, but in the work of Dozier the interview was scored using Kobak's Q-sort method. These studies yielded several findings corresponding to theoretical predictions.
Secure clients were judged to be more compliant with treatment than other groups (
Dozier, 1990). They were more emotionally committed to treatment and accepted more help with their problems (
Korfmacher et al., 1997). When comparing the more deactivating (i.e. dismissing) to the more hyperactivating (i.e. preoccupied) clients, Dozier found that higher scores on deactivation predicted clients to be less likely to seek out help and more likely to reject it, when it was offered. More deactivating clients were also generally less likely to self-disclose, and were judged by therapists to be poorer at making use of treatment. Similarly, Korfmacher et al. found that dismissing clients were less emotionally committed to treatment than secure clients were and favoured simple companionship over therapeutic interventions. Finally, Korfmacher et al. found that unresolved clients were less emotionally committed to treatment than secure clients were and were more likely than other groups to require crisis intervention during treatment.
The above-mentioned studies lend some support to the notion that attachment patterns of clients are reflected in their behaviour in treatment. Secure clients appear to be better at making use of treatment than insecure clients. Dismissing clients seem to show most difficulty engaging in a treatment relationship, whereas preoccupied clients engage emotionally but are less compliant than secure clients are. However, the empirical evidence of systematic differences in in-treatment behaviour is still quite limited. In the increasingly rich literature with theoretical descriptions and case stories to illustrate how clients with different attachment patterns behave in therapy it is often assumed that the traits characterizing attachment patterns on measuring instruments are similarly evident in a therapeutic context. But just as the home behaviour of infants can deviate importantly from their behaviour in the Strange Situation (
Ainsworth et al., 1978), there might also be differences between for instance communication style on the AAI and communication in therapy. Thus, more research is needed on how clients with different attachment patterns act and present themselves in therapy.
Client attachment patterns and the therapeutic alliance
A frequent topic in the empirical literature is the effect of client attachment patterns on the formation of the therapeutic alliance. If clients with different attachment patterns approach interpersonal relationships differently, their attachment patterns can be expected to colour the therapeutic alliances they form with therapists, both in terms of quality and development over time. Another reason for inquiring into the effect of client attachment patterns on alliance formation is the empirically established relation between alliance and ultimate treatment outcome (
Horvath & Bedi, 2002). Thus, possible connections between client attachment patterns and alliance might also point to outcome differences between clients with different attachment patterns.
Some of the first studies on alliance and attachment patterns used the AAS, which yields measures on comfort with intimacy, ability to depend on others, and fear of abandonment. In a sample of 76 clients at university and community counselling centres,
Mallinckrodt, Coble, and Gantt (1995) found that comfort with intimacy correlated positively with alliance, whereas fear of abandonment correlated negatively with alliance. Also investigating clients from university counselling centres,
Kivlighan, Patton, and Foote (1998) found that comfort with intimacy and ability to depend on others predict better therapeutic alliances (
n=40), whereas
Satterfield and Lyddon (1995) only found a significant relation between ability to depend on others and a positive alliance (
n=60). Comfort with intimacy and ability to depend on others correlate moderately with each other and are both related to secure attachment (
Collins & Read, 1990). Thus, these studies point to the overall conclusion that clients with secure attachment patterns form more positive therapeutic alliances than clients with insecure attachment patterns do, which is what one would expect based on theory. However, because of the use of the AAS, the results are not directly translatable to the more widespread categorical or two-dimensional attachment measures.
Whereas the above-mentioned studies used single point ratings of therapeutic alliance, several later studies have also inquired into changes in alliance over time. A study by
Kanninen, Salo, and Punamäki (2000) investigated the effects of client attachment patterns upon the development of the therapeutic alliance in trauma therapy with 36 Palestinian political ex-prisoners. They used a paper and pencil version of the AAI to divide the clients into the clusters
autonomous, dismissing, and
preoccupied. Since this way of measuring attachment patterns has not been employed in other studies or compared with the AAI proper, care must be taken in interpreting the results, which were, however, quite interesting.
Kanninen et al. (2000) found no differences between the three groups on the initial ratings of working alliance, but when studying changes in alliance ratings from the beginning to the middle and end of therapy, group differences emerged. Working alliance ratings often show a high–low–high pattern (
Kivlighan & Shaughnessy, 2000), and this was true of both the secure and the preoccupied group. However, both the fall in alliance towards the middle and the rise in alliance towards the end of therapy were steeper in the preoccupied group than in the secure group. The dismissing group showed a different pattern of alliance development, in which alliance was stable from the beginning to the middle of therapy, but decreased towards the end of therapy. The authors discuss the results in terms of a tendency in preoccupied individuals to show a strong emotional involvement in treatment, being extreme in negative as well as positive reactions. They speculate that the drop in alliance shown by the dismissing clients towards the end of therapy might reflect a dismissal of the importance of the therapeutic relationship in view of the coming separation.
Eames and Roth (2000) studied the relation between the attachment patterns of 30 clinical psychology outpatients and ratings of therapeutic alliance and alliance ruptures at four points in the initial phase of therapy. They found that fearful attachment on the RSQ was negatively related to working alliance, whereas secure attachment was positively related to working alliance. However, these associations only reached the level of significance for some sessions and some parts of the alliance ratings. Both dismissing and preoccupied attachment were associated with an increase in alliance over time, but the authors themselves express doubts about the reliability of these results. The clearest result of the study emerged with respect to alliance ruptures. Therapists reported significantly more ruptures with preoccupied clients and significantly fewer ruptures with dismissing clients.
In the most recent study on attachment patterns and therapeutic alliance
Sauer, Lopez, and Gormley (2003) used hierarchical linear modelling to investigate the contribution of client and therapist attachment patterns to the quality of the working alliance measured at three points during the initial phase of psychotherapy at university and community counselling centres. Attachment patterns were assessed with Simpson's Adult Attachment Inventory. Sauer et al. found no effect of client avoidance or anxiety upon working alliance as rated by either the client or the therapist (effects of therapist attachment are reported below). Out of the 28 clients, 11 clients terminated therapy before the third sampling of working alliance, making the sample rather small, but the dropout clients did not differ from the completers in terms of attachment patterns or initial rating of working alliance.
With the
Sauer et al. (2003) study as a notable exception, most of the remaining studies point to the expected conclusion that secure attachment in the client is related to more positive therapeutic alliances than insecure attachment. The studies further indicate that it might be feasible to study differences between the attachment patterns in terms of development and fluctuations in alliance over time, rather than in terms of levels of alliance at single point measurements. For instance, both the study by
Kanninen et al. (2000) and the study by
Eames and Roth (2000) suggest that alliance might fluctuate more strongly with preoccupied clients than with other client groups, although such parallels remain tentative given the large differences between the studies. Thus therapeutic work with preoccupied clients might require much more rupture repair work than therapeutic work with other clients (
Safran, Muran, Samstag, & Stevens, 2002).
Eames and Roth (2000) raise the question of differential reliability of reports of therapeutic alliance by clients with different attachment patterns. For instance, secure clients might generally be reliable in their portrayal of the quality of the alliance. In contrast, dismissing clients might report good alliances in denial of their difficulty and unwillingness to engage emotionally in treatment, in a manner similar to the idealization of parents shown by dismissing persons on the AAI. Such differences would obviously complicate matters and stress the importance of supplementing client alliance rating with ratings by observers and therapists. If such a tendency to hide beneath a surface of seeming cooperation is indeed characteristic of dismissing clients, therapists would obviously do well to attend to this possibility. Future studies could relevantly inquire into whether alliance ratings by the different attachment groups differ in terms of discrepancy from ratings from the perspectives of therapists or observers.
The effect of client attachment patterns on therapist behaviour
If clients with different attachment patterns engage differently in psychotherapeutic treatment, one might also expect client attachment patterns to call forth different behaviour on part of the therapist. Whereas attachment theory generally predicts that people elicit responses from others that confirm their working models of attachment, the results might be different with therapists who have been trained not to confirm the client's worst expectations and might instead respond in an emotionally corrective way. To date two studies have looked into therapist behaviour in response to client attachment patterns. In a study by
Hardy et al. (1999), client attachment patterns were assessed by analyzing client in-session discourse in a manner similar to the analysis of the AAI, and therapist responses to relationship episodes related by clients were noted. In a study by
Rubino, Barker, Roth, and Fearon (2000), videotapes of actors presenting a therapeutic rupture episode as it might be experienced with each of the four Bartholomew attachment styles were shown to 77 therapists-in-training and their suggested response was noted.
Hardy et al. (1999) distinguished between secure, dismissing and preoccupied client communications styles on 10 episodes from psychodynamic-interpersonal therapy chosen by clients as being particularly helpful. They found that therapists were more likely to respond with interpretation to dismissing attachment styles and with reflection of feelings to preoccupied attachment styles. This finding confirmed an earlier finding by this group that therapists tended to react with more cognitive interventions to an
underinvolved client interpersonal stance, and with more affective interventions to an
overinvolved client interpersonal stance (
Hardy, Stiles, Barkham, & Startup, 1998). It should be noted that
Hardy et al. (1999) only evaluated attachment patterns in brief episodes taken from therapy and that client overall attachment patterns were not assessed. However, this could be quite relevant as therapists might react more directly to client attachment patterns as displayed in particular moments of therapy rather than to general attachment patterns. However, since the episodes were not chosen randomly, but based on client assessment of helpfulness, the response patterns found by Hardy et al. might not be representative of the therapeutic process in general.
Rubino et al. (2000) assessed the degree of empathy and depth of intervention shown by the therapists-in-training in their suggested response to the simulated rupture episodes. Intervention depth was defined as the degree to which the therapist goes beyond and elaborates on the information given by the client. They found that therapists responded with more empathy and with greater depth to the fearful and preoccupied than to the secure and dismissing clients. In spite of the adoption of a very different research strategy, these results show some similarities to the results of
Hardy et al. (1999). Thus, the greater use of expressions of empathy with the fearful and preoccupied clients—that is, the clients hypothesized to be highest on attachment-related anxiety—might correspond to the greater use of reflection of feelings with preoccupied attachment styles in the Hardy et al. study. However, the results are more dissimilar with respect to depth of intervention. Rubino et al. found fearful and preoccupied clients to be the targets of the deepest interventions by the therapists, whereas the interpretations received by the dismissing clients in the Hardy et al. study would normally be considered deeper than the reflection of feelings received by the preoccupied clients.
Both research groups discuss their results in terms of appropriate therapist responsiveness or matching of the interpersonal style of the client. To the extent that they do find significant effects of client attachment patterns on therapist behaviour, they seem to be mostly “in style” in that therapists are more feeling-focused and empathic with anxious or preoccupied clients and are more cognitive or distant with avoidant or dismissing clients. Theoretically one might argue in favour of therapist responses “in style” to facilitate rapport, but also in favour of therapist responses “out of style” to correct or challenge the client's insecure attachment pattern, and perhaps in favour of varying response modes between different phases of therapy (
Dolan, Arnkoff, & Glass, 1993). Future studies could profitably take such timing of responses in different phases of therapy into account. Furthermore, therapist responses are obviously never simple products of client attachment patterns, but also reflect the theoretical stance and personality of the therapist —with one relevant factor being the therapist's own attachment pattern.