Skip to main content
In Reply: Dr. Menuck fires a salvo at the messengers; we address his four darts in order:
First, he takes issue with our statement that there is in fact "no evidence … that clinicians' unaided judgments are better than those of laypersons in assessing violence risk." Better-than-chance accuracy of clinical judgment does not contradict that. Predictions by psychiatrists and laypersons are indistinguishable in basis, substance, and reliability (1). Forensic clinicians' predictions are no different from those of clinicians without forensic training and experience (2). Because clinicians and laypersons alike base their predictions on history of violence, it is unsurprising that accuracies sometimes exceed chance. Dr. Menuck's assertion that clinicians predict violence with the same accuracy as actuarial tools is false. The references he cited do not support his contention, and the one by Gardner and associates demonstrated yet again the practically universal finding from hundreds of studies that actuarial methods are superior.
Second, our review explicitly concerned violence among psychiatric patients, forensic patients, and released prisoners. Among such populations, psychosis is unrelated or inversely related to subsequent violence (3). Psychoses (or more accurately, some symptoms) might be related to violence in the general population, but that was clearly not our topic. The size of the relationship in the general population, if it exists, is small compared with, for example, the relationships for substance abuse and psychopathy. Menuck's comments about the nonapplicability to individuals of results derived from large samples and the unquantifiable nature of individual psychopathology illustrate but two of many well-documented errors to which human judgment is prone (4,5,6).
Third, our point was that a few symptoms might be related to violence or treatment for them might help control it. Zisook and associates found two of 46 patients with violent command hallucinations (albeit toward themselves) committed lethal violence, leading us to suggest that command hallucinations might increase risk. Nowhere did we state that violent patients can be effectively managed with drugs. Allan and associates found that nadolol was unrelated to improvement in aggression, though related to other improvements, leading them to conclude, "Aggressiveness and psychosis can be orthogonal."
Fourth, consider, by analogy, the difference between a treatment target and a beneficial collateral effect for a fractured tibia. Proper treatment (splinting, immobilization with a cast, and so on) inevitably brings pain relief—a collateral benefit. However, treatment targeted only at pain relief obviously brings harm rather than benefit. Research indicates that in treating violent individuals, harm results from therapy aimed at raising self-esteem even though increased self-esteem might be a collateral benefit of appropriate treatment.
We appreciate that these scientific facts might be unwelcome news to practitioners who rely on unaided clinical assessment of dangerousness, drugs to treat psychotic symptoms (and thereby violence), and insight-oriented, emotionally based psychotherapy for violent offenders. Nevertheless, our positive message was that improvements in the prediction and management of violent recidivism among high-risk offenders can be attained when clinicians practice along the lines we suggested.

References

1.
Quinsey VL, Ambtman R: Variables affecting psychiatrists' and teachers' assessments of the dangerousness of mentally ill offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 47:353-362, 1979
2.
Green B, Baglioni, A: Judging suitability for release of patients from a maximum security hospital by hospital and community staff. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry 20:323-335, 1997
3.
Bonta J, Hanson R, Law M: The prediction of criminal and violent recidivism among mentally disordered offenders: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, in press
4.
Einhorn HJ, Hogarth RM: Confidence in judgment: persistence of the illusion of validity. Psychological Review 85:395-416, 1978
5.
Kahneman D, Tversky A: On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review 80:237-251, 1973
6.
Paulos JA: Innumeracy. New York, Viking Press, 1988

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Psychiatric Services
Go to Psychiatric Services
Psychiatric Services
Pages: 388
PubMed: 9525805

History

Published online: 1 March 1998
Published in print: March 1998

Authors

Affiliations

Grant T. Harris, B.Sc., Ph.D.
Marnie E. Rice, M.A., Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share