Skip to main content

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Although major depression is thought to have substantial negative effects on work performance, the possibility of recall bias limits self-report studies of these effects. The authors used the experience sampling method to address this problem by collecting comparative data on moment-in-time work performance among service workers who were depressed and those who were not depressed. METHOD: The group studied included 105 airline reservation agents and 181 telephone customer service representatives selected from a larger baseline sample; depressed workers were deliberately oversampled. Respondents were given pagers and experience sampling method diaries for each day of the study. A computerized autodialer paged respondents at random time points. When paged, respondents reported on their work performance in the diary. Moment-in-time work performance was assessed at five random times each day over a 7-day data collection period (35 data points for each respondent). RESULTS: Seven conditions (allergies, arthritis, back pain, headaches, high blood pressure, asthma, and major depression) occurred often enough in this group of respondents to be studied. Major depression was the only condition significantly related to decrements in both of the dimensions of work performance assessed in the diaries: task focus and productivity. These effects were equivalent to approximately 2.3 days absent because of sickness per depressed worker per month of being depressed. CONCLUSIONS: Previous studies based on days missed from work significantly underestimate the adverse economic effects associated with depression. Productivity losses related to depression appear to exceed the costs of effective treatment.
Rising health care costs have led to growing interest in the effects of untreated health problems on work performance and in the design of health insurance plans that optimally balance the direct costs of treatment against the indirect workplace costs of lost performance (13). Depression has become the focus of special interest in this regard for two reasons. First, epidemiologic studies show that depression not only is a common occurrence among employed people (4, 5) but also is associated with substantial work impairment (6). Second, experimental studies show that adequate depression treatment can substantially reduce work impairment (7). Both of these lines of evidence are limited, however, because they are based largely on retrospective self-reports that require respondents to summarize information about their work performance over recall periods ranging from 1 week to 1 month. There is good reason to believe that recall bias associated with negative depressive cognitions could distort these reports, leading to upwardly biased estimates of both the work impairment associated with depression and the effects of depression treatment on work performance (810).
An obvious way to address this concern would be to base epidemiologic and experimental studies on objective measures of work performance (11). However, such measures are lacking for most occupations, and they are very difficult to obtain even when they exist. Alternatively, simulation can be used to collect objective performance data by approximating the work conditions in a single occupation (12). However, simulations are difficult and expensive to create. Simulations also limit investigation to the occupations for which the simulations have been created. Furthermore, because simulations are tests, workers typically do their best to perform as well as they possibly can, which means that simulations are much better at assessing abilities than performance. In the case of depression, where work impairment is arguably attributable to lack of motivation and effort as to lack of ability, simulation might be an especially poor measurement approach.
A technique known as the experience sampling method (1315) has been developed to address the problem of self-report recall bias in situations where objective data are not available and where simulation is inappropriate. The experience sampling method uses a diary and a pager to collect concrete self-report data on moment-in-time work performance (i.e., performance at the very moment of being paged) for a randomly sampled set of moments-in-time for each respondent over the time interval of interest. The summation of the multiple “snapshots” captured in the diaries is used to build up a portrait of the typical work performance of each respondent in a way that avoids recall bias. However, it should be kept in mind that the experience sampling method relies on self-reports and may still be vulnerable to other information bias, such as might occur if depressed individuals systematically devalue their performance (10).
The aim of the current report is to present data gathered by using the experience sampling method on the relationship between major depression and measures of work performance among service workers in two large corporations. Several previous reports have been published regarding the impaired performance of workers with individual conditions, such as migraine headaches (16, 17), chronic fatigue (18), diabetes (19), asthma (20), hay fever (21), and arthritis (22). These previous studies focused on single conditions in clinical samples. The current report, in comparison, presents data from nonclinical subjects on the work impairments associated with major depression compared with impairments associated with a range of chronic physical conditions.

Method

Subjects

The experience sampling method study was embedded within a larger survey of health and productivity among two types of service workers: reservation agents working for a major airline and customer service representatives working for a major telecommunications company. Most of the individuals in both groups were women, their median ages were in the 30–44-year range, and most had completed at least some college. As described in more detail elsewhere (23), the larger survey was carried out by telephone as part of the calibration of the World Health Organization Health and Work Performance Questionnaire. A probability subsample of 286 respondents from this larger survey, including 105 reservation agents and 181 customer service representatives, was recruited into the experience sampling method study. Respondents who reported either major depression or any of several other commonly occurring chronic conditions in the survey were oversampled. A weight was used to adjust for this oversampling during data analysis.

Experience Sampling Method Procedures

Respondents were recruited by telephone to participate in what was described as a health-at-work diary study in which they would have to carry a pager and make diary entries about moment-in-time experiences at five randomly paged moments each day over 7 consecutive days. Once informed consent was obtained, the start day of the diary period was selected and an appointment was made with the respondent for a telephone interview the evening before the start day. The day of the week of the start day was randomized so that the day of the week would not be confounded by the number of days in the study.
A special delivery mail packet containing a pager, experience sampling method diaries, return envelopes, and a pen was sent to respondents for delivery the day before the start day. The telephone interview the evening before the start day instructed respondents in how to use the pager and diaries that were included in this packet. Information was also obtained at this time about each respondent’s waking time, work schedule, and bedtime. On the basis of this information, a computerized autodialer was programmed to page each respondent at five random times each day beginning at the start of the workday (or, on regularly scheduled days off, 1 hour after the respondent usually woke up on that day of the week) and ending 2 hours before the respondent usually went to bed on that day of the week. The random time points varied across days within respondents and also varied among respondents.
A separate diary book was provided for each day. Respondents were asked to mail back each day’s completed book the following morning in an effort to avoid retrospective completion. Reminder phone calls made on the evening of the first, third, and fifth diary days reinforced these procedures. A debriefing interview was administered the day after the end of the diary week.

Chronic Conditions

Seven conditions were considered sufficiently common in this group for study: allergies, arthritis, asthma, back pain, headaches, high blood pressure, and major depression. The first five of these seven were assessed with simple symptom checklists. High blood pressure was assessed with questions about whether a doctor ever told the respondent that he or she had high blood pressure and, if so, whether the respondent was under treatment for high blood pressure at the time of the interview. Major depression was assessed with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short Form (24), a screening scale found to have good sensitivity (90%) and specificity (94%) for depression diagnoses made with the full Composite International Diagnostic Interview.

Work Performance Measures

Each daily diary included five identical sets of structured questions that focused on the moment in time when the respondent was paged. The questions began by asking respondents whether they were at work and, if so, whether they were on break or on the job at the moment of the page. If on the job, further questions using 7-point self-anchoring scales were asked about work performance. Exploratory factor analysis found two dimensions of performance in these reports. The first dimension, which we refer to as task focus, is indicated by responses to questions about concentration and daydreaming (reversed), coded by using a self-anchoring 1–7 response scale with anchors of “not at all” for the lowest score and “very much” for the highest score (Cronbach’s alpha=0.63). The second dimension, which we refer to as productivity, is indicated by responses to questions about quality, speed, and efficiency (coded by using a 1–7 self-anchoring scale with responses of “low” for the lowest score and “high” for the highest score) and responses to a question about the extent to which the respondent was succeeding at the moment of the page (coded by using a 1–7 self-anchoring scale with responses of “not at all” for the lowest score and “very much” for the highest score) (Cronbach’s alpha=0.76). The scale ranges of 2–14 for task focus and 4–28 for productivity were changed to a new range of 0–100 (25) by using simple linear transformation (N=[scale range–2]100/12 for task focus and N=[scale range–4]100/24 for productivity)

Analysis Procedures

The GLIMMIX macro and Mixed procedure in SAS (26) were used to estimate a two-level random effects linear regression model with the assumption of an unstructured covariance matrix among the 2,742 data records that were completed while the respondents were working. The 0–100 task focus and productivity outcomes were centered and regressed on dichotomous measures of the seven health problems plus person-level controls (age, sex, occupation, percent of beeps completed, percent of completed beeps at work, and percent of completed beeps at work while working) and within-person controls (day of the week, sequence of the observation among the five observations in a day). Interaction terms were used to identify whether the effects of the health problems varied significantly by time of day, day of week, or demographics. Statistical significance was assessed with two-sided tests.

Results

The sum of 35 moment-in-time assessments for 286 respondents yielded 10,010 logically possible data records, of which 6,607 (66.0%) were completed (N=2,254 [61.3% of 3,675] among reservation agents and N=4,353 [68.7% of 6,335] among customer service representatives). In the total group, 3,460 (52.4%) of entries were made while the respondent was at work (1,101 [44.8%] among reservation agents and 2,450 [56.3%] among customer service representatives), and 2,742 (79.2%) of these were made while working (816 [80.8%] among reservation agents and 1,926 [78.6%] among customer service representatives).
Although agreement to participate in the experience sampling method study was not related to reports of depression in the baseline telephone interview, nonresponse to individual diary entries was higher among depressed workers than other respondents (280 [42.1%] versus 3,123 [33.4%]) (p<0.001).

The Distribution of Moment-in-Time Work Performance

The task focus and productivity scores were both strongly skewed to the right (Table 1), with means of 67.2 and 73.2, respectively. There was substantial variation among reports, with interquartile ranges (i.e., the difference in the scores at the 25th and 75th percentiles on the scale distributions) of 45.6 for task focus and 27.0 for productivity. Scores were somewhat higher, but with a similar distribution, among reservation agents than customer service representatives.

The Effects of Control Variables

Regression analyses were carried out among reservation agents and customer service representatives combined because of low statistical power in the two separate groups. Between-person control variable analysis (results not shown but available on request) showed that scores for both task focus and productivity were higher among women than men, among older than younger workers, and among reservation agents than customer service representatives. Productivity scores were also significantly related to the three characteristics of the interview process (percents of beeps completed, completed beeps at work, and completed beeps at work while working; results not shown but available on request); therefore, we adjusted for these scores in subsequent analyses. Within-person control variable analysis showed that scores on task focus were lower on weekends than on other days of the week and lower in the afternoon and evening than in the morning, while productivity scores were highest on Mondays, lowest on weekends, and generally unrelated to time of day. With regard to the within-person effects, workers in both occupations had rotating shifts that included weekday and weekend shifts and that covered the morning, afternoon, and evening hours.

Effects Associated With Chronic Conditions

Adjusting for the effects of the control variables, we found that task focus was negatively related to four of the seven conditions, the exceptions being arthritis, headaches, and high blood pressure, and that productivity was negatively related to six of the seven conditions, the exception being allergy. (The study was not conducted during allergy season.) Major depression was the only condition that was significantly associated with a decrement in task focus; back pain and major depression (at the p<0.10 level, two-sided test) were the only conditions that were significantly associated with decrements in productivity (Table 2). Major depression was associated with decrements of approximately 12 points in task focus and approximately 5 points in productivity on their 0–100 scales. These effect sizes are equivalent to a 0.4 standard deviation decrease in task focus and a 0.3 standard deviation decrease in productivity. Because these effects are based on experience sampling method evaluations at random moments in the workday, they can reasonably be assumed to describe the average decrements in these outcomes at all times across a typical workweek.
We investigated whether collinearities between individual conditions and either demographic characteristics or other conditions may have adversely affected the interpretation and statistical power of models containing all of these variables. To do so, we developed basic models and introduced demographic characteristics first and then other conditions; we observed little change in the size or significance of coefficients representing depression’s effects on either productivity or task focus, suggesting that collinearities with either demographic characteristics or other conditions did not materially affect our results (results not shown but available on request).
The possibility of variation in the magnitude of the significant effects was evaluated across both between-person and within-person control variables. Substantial consistency was found, with effects not varying meaningfully by age, sex, occupation, or day of the week. The only indication of meaningful effect size variation was for major depression by time of day (Table 3), with the effects on both outcomes largest late in the day.

Monetizing the Effects of Depression

Previous research has shown that depression is associated with roughly a doubling of annual sickness absence days, from an average of approximately 1 day per month among most workers to approximately 2 days per month among depressed workers (27, 28). This effect is typically monetized by calculating the daily salary and fringe benefits of depressed workers (29, 30), a conservative estimate in the light of the fact that it does not count lost profit or the inefficiencies in the workplace created by the absence of a depressed worker (31, 32).
It is less clear how to monetize the effects of depression on on-the-job performance. One approach is to treat the metrics of the task focus and productivity scales as representing absolute levels of work performance. Given means of 67.2 and 73.2 on these scales, respectively, the decrements of 12 points in task focus and 5 points in productivity represent proportional reductions of 18% and 7%, respectively, in these two outcomes. If we assume that the mean of these proportions, 12.5%, represents the proportional decrement in overall performance due to an episode of major depression, it is possible to convert these effects into sickness absence day equivalents by saying that the lost productivity during 8 workdays (the multiplicative inverse of 12.5%) is equivalent to 1 day absent for sickness. On a base of 225 workdays each year (i.e., a typical 250-day work year minus approximately 25 days of absence for sickness per year among depressed workers with episodes that persist the entire year), this is equivalent to somewhat more than 2 days of lost productivity per month of being depressed (225/[8×12]=2.3). Consequently, the estimate of lost productivity related to depression on days at work (2.3 days per month) is considerably greater than the lost productivity found in previous studies from sickness absence (approximately 1 day per month). Even with the relatively low salaries of the service workers in this study, the combined salary-equivalent effect of major depression on absenteeism and lost productivity is greater than $300 per month.

Discussion

These results should be interpreted with four sets of limitations in mind. First, although the experience sampling method removes recall bias by focusing on assessments across a random sample of moments in time, the consistency of these assessments with actual performance is largely unknown (33). Some previous studies of idle time at work have found that experience sampling method rates are higher than rates recorded in official workplace records (34, 35), and this has been interpreted as evidence of the greater sensitivity of experience sampling method data (13). If discrepancies between experience sampling method reports and actual work performance at the sampled moments in time are random, associations with depression would in any case be largely unaffected. However, systematic discrepancies could still lead to overestimation of illness effects (36, 37). A plausible systematic discrepancy is a “pessimism bias” associated with depression (10), although this would seem on the face of it to be less likely for reports about task focus than for reports about productivity. Although doubtlessly imperfect, then, reports based on the experience sampling method may be the best measures we can obtain of actual behavior on the job, especially for task focus, where the estimated effect of depression was most pronounced.
Second, some of the comparator conditions that were sufficiently prevalent to examine may have been clinically mild, causing depression to appear to be inordinately impairing. Depression was also the only condition assessed with a screening instrument (others were assessed by symptom checklists and reports of earlier diagnoses), further raising the possibility that depression cases were more severe or acute than cases of other conditions. It may be worth noting that of the seven conditions examined here, four (arthritis, headaches, asthma, and depression) were significantly associated with absenteeism in the parent study that gave rise to our experience sampling method study (38). Furthermore, among medically serious conditions (e.g., cancer, diabetes, gastrointestinal ulcers, and heart disease) assessed in the parent study that were not sufficiently prevalent in our subgroup to examine, only chronic obstructive pulmonary disease/emphysema was significantly associated with absenteeism.
Third, we did not have the information necessary to determine whether conditions preceded work impairments or vice versa. For this reason, it is not possible to say with certainty whether depression caused impairments or vice versa. Finally, it is not clear to what extent our results on service workers generalize to other types of workers. In addition, some predesignated respondents refused to participate, and others who participated failed to complete moments-in-time responses. However, it should be pointed out that nonparticipation was unrelated to depression in the baseline survey. We also found that nonresponse (typical in experience sampling method studies because of the high demands [39]) was higher among depressed workers, confirming previous experience sampling method findings that nonresponse is related to both psychopathology and worse functioning (37, 40, 41). If low response is an indication of failure to perform tasks while at work, our results regarding the effects of depression on work performance may be conservative.
Within the constraints of these limitations, the results reported here show that major depression is more consistently related to poor work performance than any of the commonly occurring chronic physical conditions found in the group studied. Focused experience sampling method studies of depression have found similar results. In particular, depressed primary care patients have been shown to be less engaged in work and more likely to report “doing nothing” during work hours than healthy control subjects (42), and depressed adolescents have been shown to spend less time in schoolwork than either recovered patients or healthy control subjects (43).
Our finding that effects of depression are more pronounced late in the day was not examined in previous experience sampling method studies. This finding is consistent with a diurnal pattern of greater symptom severity later in the day that is often found among patients with the milder forms of depression likely to be found in working populations (44). It is also possible that depression exacerbates the fatigue and reduction in cognitive abilities that have been found to increase naturally over the workday (4548).
An important implication of these results is that the cost-effectiveness of depression treatment from the perspective of the employer might be substantially greater than previously thought. An earlier simulation (27) based on data from two nationally representative general population samples of workers suggested that depression treatments with effect sizes found in recent effectiveness trials would lead to decreases in work loss and work cutback that would yield a value of $1,100–$1,800 in salary equivalents per year of treatment. These savings exceed the average costs of depression treatment, even though they focus exclusively on absenteeism. We found that the effects of depression on on-the-job work performance, at least for these service workers, were twice as large as the average effects on absenteeism. If the same effect sizes were shown to hold in the general population, then the estimated salary-equivalent value of guideline-concordant depression treatment might be two to three times as high as the estimate based on the earlier simulation.
An important caution with regard to the last point is that real-world treatment of depression often uses suboptimal regimens (29, 49). Employers are consequently reluctant to accept indirect evidence of cost-effectiveness. As a result, efforts to increase employer enthusiasm for expanded depression treatment will require effectiveness trials to be carried out that estimate the cost-effectiveness from the employer’s perspective of usual care as well as the perspective of enhanced depression care. A new effectiveness trial known as the Work Outcomes Research and Cost-Effectiveness Study, sponsored by the National Institute of Mental Health, is currently under way to obtain such estimates (28). This new trial as well as future initiatives aimed at evaluating cost-effectiveness from the employer’s perspective need to consider not only the effects of absence because of sickness but also the effects on work performance in order to capture the full extent to which depression affects work performance and the full extent to which depression treatment has value for the employer.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3

Footnote

Received July 1, 2003; revision received Dec. 2, 2003; accepted Dec. 8, 2003. From the Department of Health Care Policy, Harvard Medical School; the Division of Pharmacoepidemiology and Pharmacoeconomics, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Boston; the Clinical Research Unit, Kaiser Permanente, Denver, Colo.; the Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor; American Airlines, Dallas/Fort Worth, Tex.; HealthPartners, Minneapolis; and the Center for Health Studies, Group Health Cooperative, Seattle, Wash. Address reprint requests to Dr. Wang, Department of Health Care Policy, Harvard Medical School, 180 Longwood Ave., Boston, MA 02115; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Program on Health and Work Performance. Also supported by NIMH grants R01 MH-61941 (Dr. Wang), K01 MH-01651 (Dr. Wang), and K05 MH-00507 (Dr. Kessler), and by grant 048123 from the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (Dr. Wang). The authors thank Paul Greenberg, Kathleen Merikangas, and Paul Stang for helpful comments on an earlier version of this article.

References

1.
Murray CJL, Lopez AD: The Global Burden of Disease: A Comprehensive Assessment of Mortality and Disability From Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors in 1990 and Projected to 2020. Cambridge, Mass, Harvard University Press, 1996
2.
Burner ST, Waldo DR, McKusick DR: National health expenditures projections through 2030. Health Care Financ Rev 1992; 14:1–29
3.
Gold MR, Siegel JE, Russell LB, Weinstein MC: Cost-Effectiveness in Health and Medicine. New York, Oxford University Press, 1996
4.
WHO International Consortium in Psychiatric Epidemiology: Cross-national comparisons of the prevalences and correlates of mental disorders. Bull World Health Organ 2000; 78:413–426
5.
Kessler RC, Greenberg PE, Mickelson KD, Meneades LM, Wang PS: The effects of chronic medical conditions on work loss and work cutback. J Occup Environ Med 2001; 43(suppl 3):218–225
6.
Eaton WW, Anthony JC, Mandel W, Garrison R: Occupations and the prevalence of major depressive disorder. J Occup Environ Med 1990; 32:1079–1087
7.
Mintz J, Mintz LI, Arruda MJ, Hwang SS: Treatments of depression and the functional capacity to work. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:761–768
8.
Bradburn NM, Rips LJ, Shevell SK: Answering autobiographical questions: the impact of memory and inference on surveys. Science 1987; 236:157–161
9.
Ross M: Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories. Psychol Rev 1989; 96:341–357
10.
Morgado A, Raoux N, Smith M, Allilaire JF, Widlocher D: Subjective bias in reports of poor work adjustment in depressed patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1989; 80:541–547
11.
Greenberg PE, Birnbaum HG: Linking administrative claims data with archival productivity measures to inform employer decision making, in Health and Work Productivity. Edited by Kessler RC, Stang P. Chicago, University of Chicago Press (in press)
12.
Howland J, Mangione TW, Laramie A: Simulation for measurement of occupational performance. Ibid (in press)
13.
Csikszentmihalyi M, Larson R: Validity and reliability of the Experience Sampling Method, in The Experience of Psychopathology: Investigating Mental Disorders in Their Natural Settings. Edited by deVries MW. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 1992, pp 43–57
14.
deVries MW (ed): The Experience of Psychopathology: Investigating Mental Disorders in Their Natural Settings. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 1992
15.
Stone AA, Shiffman S: Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavioral medicine. Ann Behav Med 1994; 16:199–202
16.
Honkoop PC, Sorbi MJ, Godaert GL, Spierings EL: High-density assessment of the IHS classification criteria for migraine without aura: a prospective study. Cephalalgia 1999; 19:201–206
17.
Sorbi MJ, Honkoop PC, Godaert GL: A signal-contingent computer diary for the assessment of psychological precedents of the migraine attack, in Ambulatory Assessment: Computer-Assisted Psychological and Psychophysiological Methods in Monitoring and Field Studies. Edited by Fahrenberg J, Myrtek J. Seattle, Hogrefe & Huber, 1996
18.
Stone AA, Broderick JE, Porter LS, Krupp L, Gnys M, Paty JA, Shiffman S: Fatigue and mood in chronic fatigue syndrome patients: results of a momentary assessment protocol examining fatigue and mood levels and diurnal patterns. Ann Behav Med 1994; 16:228–234
19.
Schandry R, Leopold C: Ambulatory assessment of self-monitored subjective and objective symptoms of diabetic patients, in Ambulatory Assessment: Computer-Assisted Psychological and Psychophysiological Methods in Monitoring and Field Studies. Edited by Fahrenberg J, Myrtek J. Seattle, Hogrefe & Huber, 1996, pp 393–402
20.
Smyth JM, Soefer MH, Hurewitz A, Kliment A, Stone AA: Daily psychosocial factors predict levels and diurnal cycles of asthma symptomatology and peak flow. J Behav Med 1999; 22:179–193
21.
Kessler RC, Almeida DM, Berglund P, Stang P: Pollen and mold exposure impairs the work performance of employees with allergic rhinitis. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol 2001; 87:289–295
22.
Gorin AA, Smyth JM, Weisberg JN, Affleck G, Tennen H, Urrows S, Stone AA: Rheumatoid arthritis patients show weather sensitivity in daily life, but the relationship is not clinically significant. Pain 1999; 81:173–177
23.
Kessler RC, Barber C, Beck A, Berglund P, Cleary PD, McKenas D, Pronk N, Simon G, Stang P, Ustun TB, Wang P: The World Health Organization Health and Work Performance Questionnaire. J Occup Environ Med 2003; 45:156–174
24.
Kessler RC, Andrews G, Mroczek D, Ustun B, Wittchen H-U: The World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview Short Form (CIDI-SF). Int J Meth Psychiatr Res 1998; 7:171–185
25.
Lépine JP, Lellouch J, Lovell A, Teherani M, Ha C, Verdier-Taillefer MH, Rambourg B, Lempérière T: Anxiety and depressive disorders in a French population: methodology and preliminary results. Psychiatry and Psychobiology 1989; 4:267–274
26.
SAS Version 6.12. Cary, NC, SAS Institute, 1996
27.
Kessler RC, Barber CB, Birnbaum HG, Frank RG, Greenberg PE, Rose RM, Simon GE, Wang PS: Depression in the workplace: effects on short-term work disability. Health Aff 1999; 18:163–171
28.
Wang PS, Simon GE, Kessler RC: The economic burden of depression and the cost-effectiveness of treatment. Int J Meth Psychiatr Res 2003; 12:22–33
29.
Wang PS, Berglund P, Kessler RC: Recent care of common mental disorders in the United States: prevalence and conformance with evidence-based recommendations. J Gen Intern Med 2000; 15:284–292
30.
Greenberg P, Kessler R, Nells T, Finkelstein S, Berndt ER: Depression in the workplace: an economic perspective, in Selective Serotonin Reuptake Inhibitors: Advances in Basic Research and Clinical Practice. Edited by Feighner JP, Boyer WF. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 1996, pp 327–363
31.
Berger ML, Murray JF, Xu J, Pauly M: Alternative valuations of work loss and productivity. J Occup Environ Med 2001; 43:18–24
32.
Murray JF, Nicholson S, Pauly M, Berger ML: Investing in health to boost employee productivity: the employer’s perspective, in Health and Work Productivity. Edited by Kessler RC, Stang P. Chicago, University of Chicago Press (in press)
33.
Stone AA, Kessler RC, Haythornthwaite JA: Measuring daily events and experiences: decisions for the researcher. J Pers 1991; 59:575–607
34.
Robinson J: How Americans Use Time: A Social-Psychological Analysis of Everyday Behavior. New York, Praeger, 1977
35.
Szalai A, Converse P, Feldheim P: The Use of Time: Daily Activities of Urban and Suburban Populations in Twelve Countries. The Hague, Mouton, 1972
36.
Kraan H, Meertens H, Hilwig M, Volovics L, Dijkman-Caes CIM, Portegijs P: Selecting measures, diagnostic validity and scaling in the study of depression, in The Experience of Psychopathology: Investigating Mental Disorders in Their Natural Settings. Edited by deVries MW. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 1992, pp 324–338
37.
Wilson KCM, Hopkins R, deVries MW, Copeland JRM: Research alliance and the limit of compliance: experience sampling with the depressed elderly. Ibid, pp 339–346
38.
Wang PS, Beck A, Berglund P, Leutzinger JA, Pronk N, Richling D, Simon G, Stang P, Ustun TB, Kessler RC: Chronic medical conditions and work performance in the Health and Work Performance Questionnaire calibration surveys. J Occup Environ Med 2003; 45:1303–1311
39.
Hormuth SE: The sampling of experiences in situ. J Pers 1986; 54:262–293
40.
Waite BM, Claffey R, Hillbrand M: Differences between volunteers and nonvolunteers in a high demand self-recording study. Psychol Rep 1998; 83:199–210
41.
Dijkman-Caes CIM: Panic Disorder and Agoraphobia in Daily Life. Maastricht, Netherlands, Maastricht University Press, 1993
42.
Barge-Schaapveld DQ, Nicolson NA, Berkhof J, deVries MW: Quality of life in depression: daily life determinants and variability. Psychiatry Res 1999; 88:173–189
43.
Merrick WA: Dysphoric moods in depressed and non-depressed adolescents, in The Experience of Psychopathology: Investigating Mental Disorders in Their Natural Settings. Edited by deVries MW. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 1992, pp 148–156
44.
Rusting CL, Larsen RJ: Diurnal patterns of unpleasant mood: associations with neuroticism, depression, and anxiety. J Pers 1998; 66:85–103
45.
Smith L, Totterdell P, Folkhard S: Shiftwork effects in nuclear power workers: a field study using portable computers. Work Stress 1995; 9:187–197
46.
Totterdell P, Folkhard S: In situ repeated measures of affect and cognitive performance facilitated by use of a hand-held computer. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 1992; 24:545–553
47.
Totterdell P, Spelten E, Barton J, Smith L, Folkhard S: On-shift and daily variations in self-report and performance measures in rotating-shift and permanent night nurses. Work Stress 1995; 9:187–197
48.
Knauth P, Keller J, Schindele G, Totterdell P: A 14-h night-shift in the control room of a fire brigade. Work Stress 1995; 9:176–186
49.
Wang PS, Demler O, Kessler RC: Adequacy of treatment for serious mental illness in the United States. Am J Public Health 2002; 92:92–98

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1885 - 1891
PubMed: 15465987

History

Published in print: October 2004
Published online: 22 December 2014

Authors

Details

Philip S. Wang, M.D., Dr.P.H.
David K. McKenas, M.D., M.P.H.
Nicolaas P. Pronk, Ph.D.
Gregory E. Simon, M.D., M.P.H.
Ronald C. Kessler, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share