Skip to main content
Full access
Brief Report
Published Online: 1 October 2002

A Study of Parallel Implicit and Explicit Information Processing in Patients With Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: This study examined implicit sequence learning in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) under dual-task conditions. Frontal-striatal networks support implicit learning and are implicated in the pathophysiology of OCD. Neuroimaging data suggest that during implicit learning, OCD patients use neural systems normally active during explicit learning to compensate for striatal dysfunction. METHOD: The authors examined implicit sequence learning in 25 OCD patients and 25 healthy comparison subjects using a dual-task paradigm, with subjects simultaneously engaged in an explicit memory task and an implicit learning task. They predicted that implicit learning in OCD subjects would be disrupted because concurrent explicit information-processing demands would prevent use of compensatory processes. RESULTS: OCD patients failed to show evidence of implicit learning and exhibited a significant deficit in comparison with healthy subjects. CONCLUSIONS: These results are consistent with the hypothesis that concurrent explicit and implicit information-processing demands interfere with implicit learning in OCD patients.
Current neurobiological models of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) emphasize frontal-striatal system dysfunction (1). The striatum supports some aspects of “implicit learning,” which refers to the process by which knowledge is acquired by means of repetition or exposure and expressed without conscious reference to the learning episode. In contrast, “explicit learning” refers to memory functions requiring conscious encoding or retrieval of facts and relies on the integrity of the frontal and medial temporal lobes (2).
A well-characterized measure of implicit learning is the serial reaction-time task (3). In this task, participants are presented with sequences of visual cues in which the locations of the cues are predictable or unpredictable. Implicit learning is demonstrated by the reaction-time advantage in response to predictable versus unpredictable sequences after the subject has been exposed to repeated trials of the predictable sequences.
In positron emission tomography and functional magnetic resonance imaging studies (4, 5), OCD patients have exhibited normal implicit reaction-time task learning. However, whereas healthy subjects showed activation in the right striatum when the predictable versus the unpredictable conditions were contrasted, the OCD patients failed to show right striatal recruitment. Instead, they exhibited activation in medial temporal regions (i.e., the hippocampal/parahippocampal cortex)—brain regions normally involved in explicit memory. This abnormal pattern of activation along with normal behavioral performance on the reaction-time task suggests that OCD patients may use medial temporal networks to compensate for striatal dysfunction (4).
The current study was designed to test this hypothesis of compensatory neural processing in OCD by using a dual-task reaction-time task that entails a concurrent explicit memory load in addition to the reaction-time task-sequence learning task. We predicted that a simultaneous explicit memory load might disproportionately interfere with implicit sequence learning in OCD patients if it prevents the compensatory use of brain systems normally involved in processing explicit information.

Method

Study participants were 25 right-handed subjects who met DSM-IV criteria for OCD (19 women) and 25 right-handed healthy comparison subjects (15 women) who were recruited through Massachusetts General Hospital’s OCD unit. All participants rendered written informed consent before participation, in accordance with Massachusetts General Hospital’s Subcommittee on Human Studies. Current comorbid (secondary) diagnoses in OCD subjects included major depression (N=5), dysthymia (N=2), body dysmorphic disorder (N=2), panic disorder (N=3), social phobia (N=3), tic disorder not otherwise specified (N=1), and anorexia nervosa (N=1). Fifteen OCD subjects were taking psychotropic medication at the time of testing: selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (N=9), benzodiazepines (N=5), risperidone (N=2), and phenelzine (N=1). In the OCD group, Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (6) and Beck Depression Inventory scores indicated moderate OCD severity (mean=21.3, SD=5.8) and low levels of depression (mean=13.2, SD=9.0). All subjects were otherwise medically healthy by self-report and reported no history of significant head injury, seizure, neurological condition, or current major medical condition. OCD and comparison subjects did not differ with regard to age (t=–1.13, df=48, p=0.27) or education (t=–0.11, df=48, p=0.91).
For a detailed description of the version of the reaction-time task that we used, see Whalen et al. (7). Briefly, in the dual-task reaction-time task, participants respond to visual cues on a screen by pressing one of four buttons that corresponds to the location of the cue. Each trial consists of a sequence of three such cues; the third cue in the sequence is either predictable or unpredictable on the basis of the location of the first two cues. Each participant completes three blocks of 36 trials (1×12 unpredictable sequences, and 3×8 predictable sequences). In addition, at the beginning of each block, subjects are given a seven-letter explicit working memory load (e.g., “GFMSVRJ”) and instructed to maintain the letters in working memory throughout the reaction-time task for a free-recall test after each block. Participants were instructed to give attentional priority to the working memory components of the dual-task reaction-time task.
After completion of the third block, subjects were tested for explicit knowledge of the cue sequences by using a frequency-recognition task in which subjects rated the frequency with which predictable and unpredictable sequences occurred in the third block.
Data were analyzed by using mixed-model analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group (OCD versus comparison) as the between factor, block and (for reaction time only) type of trial (predictable versus unpredictable sequences) as within factors, followed by unpaired and pairwise t tests. Dependent variables were 1) the mean reaction time to the third cue in predictable and unpredictable sequences, 2) the number of correctly recalled letters from the working memory task, and 3) the frequency rating for the recognition test.

Results

With regard to explicit working memory performance (recall for letters), ANOVA indicated a main effect for block (F=20.37, df=2, 96, p<0.001), a less-than-significant group-by-block interaction (F=2.99, df=2, 96, p<0.06), and no significant main effect for group (F=0.41, df=1, 48, p=0.53) (Table 1). Follow-up unpaired t tests indicated that OCD and comparison subjects did not differ significantly in the number of letters recalled after the first block (t=1.52, df=48, p=0.14), the second block (t=–0.17, df=48, p=0.87), or the third block (t=–0.61, df=48, p=0.55) (Table 1).
With regard to implicit sequence-learning performance (reaction time), ANOVA indicated a significant main effect for block (F=5.98, df=2, 92, p=0.004), a significant group-by-trial type interaction (F=6.06, df=1, 46, p<0.02), a significant interaction for block-by-trial type (F=4.54, df=2, 92, p<0.02), and a significant group-by-block-by-trial type interaction (F=3.12, df=2, 92, p<0.05). For OCD patients, follow-up pairwise t tests between reaction time and predictable and unpredictable trials within each group indicated no significant differences between predictable and unpredictable trials in the first block (t=–1.14, df=23, p=0.27), the second block (t=–1.67, df=24, p=0.11), or the third block (t=0.53, df=24, p=0.60). For comparison subjects, there was no difference in reaction time to predictable and unpredictable trials in the first block (t=–0.16, df=23, p=0.13) or the second block (t=1.36, df=24, p=0.19). However, there was a significant difference between predictable and unpredictable trials in the third block (t=3.47, df=24, p=0.002). This indicated a learning effect for predictable trials in the comparison subjects but not in the OCD subjects. Moreover, an unpaired t test between comparison and OCD subjects indicated a significantly greater reaction-time advantage (reaction time to unpredictable sequences minus reaction time to predictable sequences) for the comparison subjects than for the OCD subjects in the third block (t=2.73, df=48, p=0.009).
With regard to explicit knowledge for sequences (frequency ratings), ANOVA indicated no main effects for group (F=1.06, df=1, 48, p=0.31) or trial (F=1.44, df=1, 48, p=0.24) and no significant interaction (F=0.50, df=1, 48, p=0.48) (Table 1). Both comparison and OCD subjects estimated that predictable and unpredictable triplets occurred on average only once, indicating that both groups were unaware of the predictable sequences.
With regard to medication and comorbidity, after exclusion of either medicated patients or patients with comorbid diagnoses, replication of the described analyses yielded results equivalent to those for the total OCD group in both the subgroup of medication-free patients as well as the subgroup of comorbidity-free patients.

Discussion

Consistent with our hypothesis, OCD subjects failed to exhibit implicit sequence learning in this dual-task reaction-time task. Specifically, only comparison subjects—not OCD patients—showed a reaction-time advantage for predictable sequences compared to unpredictable sequences in the third block. Moreover, comparison subjects developed a significantly greater reaction-time advantage than OCD patients, which is indicative of impaired sequence learning in OCD patients. OCD subjects exhibited no deficit in explicit information processing, as assessed by recall for letters. Furthermore, ratings about the frequency with which the various triplets had appeared in the third block of the experiment indicated that both OCD and comparison subjects had no significant awareness of the predictable sequences. This indicates that preserved sequence learning ability under the dual-task condition in comparison subjects did not reflect explicit knowledge as a basis for better reaction time. Taken together, the finding of impaired sequence learning in OCD subjects but preserved processing of explicit information and absent awareness of the repeated sequences suggests that concurrent processing of explicit information interfered with implicit learning of the sequences presented in the dual-task reaction-time task as originally hypothesized.
However, it is important to acknowledge that we did not assess implicit sequence learning using both the single- and dual-task reaction-time tasks for the same subjects. Consequently, our interpretation of the current findings relies in part on prior findings. In addition, one should be cautious about linking the current cognitive behavior results with interpretations regarding neurocircuitry from prior neuroimaging findings. The results of this study do not allow firm conclusions regarding the neural underpinnings of the parallel-processing deficit exhibited by subjects with OCD.
We originally hypothesized that OCD patients may employ medial temporal networks to learn sequence information as a means of compensating for primary dysfunction in frontal-striatal systems. From this perspective, parallel processing of explicit information may have “preoccupied” frontal-temporal systems, thereby preventing subjects with OCD from employing these structures for sequence learning and hence uncovering a limitation in implicit learning and memory. However, at this time, it cannot be ruled out that concurrent explicit information-processing demands interfere with implicit learning in patients with OCD by some other mechanism, nor does intact letter recall in OCD subjects preclude the possibility of dysfunction involving the medial temporal lobe. Future studies are needed to clarify the neural substrates of normal parallel-processing functions as well as their apparent disruption in OCD.
TABLE 1

Footnote

Presented in part at the 33rd annual meeting of the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Toronto, Nov. 11–14, 1999. Received June 15, 2001; revision received Feb. 13, 2002; accepted April 8, 2002. From the Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston; and the Department of Psychology, University of Colorado, Boulder. Address reprint requests to Dr. Rauch, Department of Psychiatry, 149-9102, Massachusetts General Hospital—East, Bldg. 149, 13th St., 9th Floor, Charlestown, MA 02129; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported in part by MH-60219 (Dr. Rauch), the Judah Research Fund (Dr. Jenike), the Tourette Syndrome Association (Dr. Deckersbach), and the Obsessive-Compulsive Foundation (Dr. Deckersbach).

References

1.
Saxena S, Brody AL, Schwartz JM, Baxter LR: Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 1998; 35:26-37
2.
Schacter DL, Curran T: Memory without remembering and remembering without memory: implicit and false memories, in The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Edited by Gazzaniga M. Cambridge, Mass, MIT Press, 2000, pp 829-840
3.
Nissen MJ, Bullemer P: Attentional requirements of learning: evidence from performance measures. Cognit Psychol 1987; 19:1-32
4.
Rauch SL, Savage CR, Alpert NM, Dougherty D, Kendrick A, Curran T, Brown HD, Manzo P, Fischman AJ, Jenike MA: Probing striatal function in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a PET study of implicit sequence learning. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997; 9:568-573
5.
Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Curran T, Shin LM, Coffey BJ, Savage CR, McInerney SC, Baer L, Jenike MA: Probing striato-thalamic function in OCD and TS using neuroimaging methods. Adv Neurol 2000; 85:207-224
6.
Goodman WK, Price LH, Rasmussen SA, Mazure C, Fleischmann RL, Hill CL, Heninger GR, Charney DS: The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, I: development, use, and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989; 46:1006-1011
7.
Whalen PJ, Curran T, Rauch SL: Neuroimaging studies of implicit information processing, in Psychiatric Neuroimaging Research: Contemporary Strategies. Edited by Dougherty DD, Rauch SL. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 2001, pp 73-100

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1780 - 1782
PubMed: 12359688

History

Published online: 1 October 2002
Published in print: October 2002

Authors

Details

Thilo Deckersbach, Ph.D.
Michael A. Jenike, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share