Skip to main content
Full access
Editorial
Published Online: 1 July 2002

Major Depression: Causes or Effects?

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry
This issue contains four articles that address a number of key aspects of the causes and effects of major depression. Three suggest that what we have often thought were effects may actually be causes of depression. As a whole, the articles point to a number of important questions that are worthy of further research.
Over the past few years, two major brain areas have been implicated in major depression—the prefrontal cortex/anterior cingulate and the hippocampus. The articles in this issue by Frodl et al. and by Lockwood et al. each point to abnormalities involving one of these areas. Lockwood et al. report on significant neuropsychological impairment in depressed patients, based on comparisons with healthy subjects. Elderly subjects demonstrated impairment in performance of tasks of executive function (suggestive of prefrontal/anterior cingulate dysfunction), and particular impairment was seen in elderly depressed subjects. These findings support a number of other observations involving the prefrontal region in depression (1, 2). Taken together, these data at first glance point to a major consequence or correlate of depression, namely marked cognitive dysfunction involving the prefrontal cortex/anterior cingulate, but also suggest that a performance decrement in the elderly involving this region could be a risk factor for becoming severely depressed.
Frodl et al. explored hippocampal volume in patients with first-episode major depression. Sheline et al. (3) and Bremner et al. (4) reported smaller hippocampal volumes in depressed patients than in healthy subjects. Further, Sheline et al. (3) reported that a smaller hippocampus was associated with greater lifetime duration of depression and suggested that excessive glucocorticoid activity could be a factor in this seeming atrophy. These findings extend Sapolsky’s work with rats (5). The article by Frodl et al. raises serious questions regarding the significance and causes of smaller hippocampal volume. Indeed, the fact that the study involved first-episode patients suggests that a smaller hippocampus, particularly in men, is a risk factor for, rather than a consequence of, major depression.
These data fit nicely with findings from three other recent studies that all point to strong genetic influences on hippocampal volume. My colleagues and I (6) used a model involving paternal half-siblings among squirrel monkeys to explore the relative contributions of early life stress and genetics to hippocampal size in young adulthood. Paternal genetics—but not early stress—appeared to account for much of the variance in hippocampal size. Specific fathers appeared to sire offspring who had smaller hippocampi. These animals also demonstrated greater cortisol responses at the time of weaning, suggesting a risk factor for depression.
Two other studies of human twins by Sullivan et al. (7) and R.K. Pitman et al. (2001 personal communication and presentation at the 2001 annual meeting of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology) have demonstrated rather convincingly that hippocampal size is largely genetically determined, although in the study by Sullivan et al. environment appeared to play a role in hippocampal size as well. In the study by Pitman et al., a smaller hippocampus appeared to be a risk factor for developing posttraumatic stress disorder.
Still, prospective longitudinal studies are needed to determine whether depression and other psychiatric disorders can result in a further diminution in hippocampal volume. Thus, a smaller hippocampus could still reflect both a cause and an effect of major depression.
The articles by Frodl et al. and Lockwood et al. emphasize two different regions, and little attention is paid in each article to the other region. It is, however, probable that both regions are involved and that activity in both of them and the connections between them helps explain both the somatic and cognitive symptoms of the disorder.
One possible common thread affecting both regions is excessive glucocorticoid activity. Administration of cortisol to healthy subjects results in dysfunction in both regions (8, 9). Frodl et al. note that although in rats high cortisol levels can relatively rapidly reduce hippocampal size, excessive glucocorticoids probably do not explain their hippocampal findings in the first-episode patients. On the basis of the time period involved, this seems sensible. However, they also cite the work of Sanchez et al. (10) in which type II glucocorticoid receptors were not appreciably found in the hippocampus of the rhesus monkey, although they were found heavily in the prefrontal cortex, as another reason for not invoking glucocorticoids. A human glucocorticoid receptor probe was used in that study. In contrast, Patel et al. (11), using specific probes for glucocorticoid receptors in the squirrel monkey, found glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus as well as in the frontal cortex. Thus, glucocorticoids could play a key role in the findings of both studies. A smaller hippocampus could be associated with higher glucocorticoid levels during stressful times, which could lead to cognitive dysfunction associated with effects in the prefrontal cortex/anterior cingulate and the hippocampus. In turn, excessive glucocorticoids could result in “atrophy” of specific brain regions. This area is worthy of further investigation and will require longitudinal study.
My colleagues and I have been studying prefrontal cortex/anterior cingulate and hippocampal function or activity in psychotic major depression, a subtype characterized by excessive activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. We have reported (12) that patients with psychotic major depression demonstrate impaired performance on both the color-word portion of the Stroop Color-Word Test and the Paragraph Recall Test (verbal memory). These data suggest impairment in both regions of interest; however, the deficit in performance on the Paragraph Recall Test was not in retention, suggesting that attention, response inhibition, and encoding could account for a large part of the deficit. In another study (13), we reported that patients with psychotic major depression demonstrated errors of commission but not omission on a word-list test, in line with problems of focus and encoding rather than in memory/recall per se. We are currently employing functional magnetic resonance imaging to tease apart these functions and brain regions and are using a specific antagonist for the low-affinity type II glucocorticoid receptor to access whether blocking cortisol improves performance.
The article by Takeshita et al. is another seeming “effect” article and follows several recent articles pointing to an association between depression and a higher rate of mortality. The data are chilling: Takeshita et al. report that depressive symptoms are associated with a high mortality rate in otherwise healthy elderly subjects. The findings suggest that some aspect of the biology of depression is associated with early mortality. But what is this factor? This article does not provide an explanation, and further research is required to ferret out the cause for early mortality. Alterations in blood clotting and a high risk for arrhythmias are possible culprits that could lead to untoward consequences. They would, however, open the way for innovative interventions. For example, if depression is associated with high levels of circulating catecholamines and with arrhythmias, specific treatments could be aimed at preventing early mortality. It may be premature, however, to conclude that depression specifically leads to early mortality. Conceivably, a biological process might be at play that involves a number of somatic symptoms, some of which are depressive in nature, or there could be a biological process, such as a subtle cardiovascular dysfunction, that produces a depressive-like cast.
The study by Kendler et al. tested a hypothesis put forth previously by this group (14) on the multifactoral causes of depression. Using a twin cohort, they elegantly dissected out the roles of early experiences (e.g., childhood abuse), genetics, substance abuse, etc., on early and later episodes of depression. The factors studied are those that we clinicians commonly see when treating depressed patients. This elegant pathway analysis might be strengthened if we had better candidates genes to add to the schematic, as well as baseline hippocampal volumes. Biology could play major roles in the seemingly psychosocial factors.
Taken together, these articles point to a number of possible causative factors as well as untoward consequences or correlates of major depression. Use of cross-sectional techniques can easily obfuscate what are truly causes and effects. Still, such research gives us important clues, and longitudinal studies can ultimately give us a better sense of the causes and effects of a troubling disorder in modern society.

Footnote

Address reprint requests to Dr. Schatzberg, Department of Psychiatry, Stanford University School of Medicine, 3rd Floor, 401 Quarry Rd., Stanford, CA 94305-5717; [email protected] (e-mail).

References

1.
Liotti M, Mayberg HS: The role of functional neuroimaging in the neuropsychology of depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2001; 123:121-136
2.
Drevets WC: Functional anatomical abnormalities in limbic and prefrontal cortical structures in major depression. Prog Brain Res 2000; 126:413-431
3.
Sheline YI, Sanghani M, Mintan MA, Gado MH: Depression duration but not age predicts hippocampal volume loss in medically healthy women with recurrent major depression. J Neurosci 1999; 19:5034-5043
4.
Bremner JD, Narayan M, Anderson ER, Staib LH, Miller HL, Charney DS: Hippocampal volume reduction in major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:115-118
5.
Sapolsky RM: Glucocorticoids and hippocampal atrophy in neuropsychiatric disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:925-935
6.
Lyons DM, Yang C, Sawyer-Glover AM, Moseley ME, Schatzberg AF: Early life stress and inherited variation in monkey hippocampal volumes. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:1145-1151
7.
Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A, Swan GE, Carmelli D: Heritability of hippocampal size in elderly twin men: equivalent influence from genes and environment. Hippocampus 2001; 11:754-762
8.
Newcomer JW, Selke G, Melson AK, Hershey T, Craft S, Richards K, Alderson AL: Decreased memory performance in healthy humans induced by stress-level cortisol treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:527-533
9.
Lupien SJ, Gillin CJ, Hauger RL: Working memory is more sensitive than declarative memory to the acute effects of corticosteroids. Behav Neurosci 1999; 113:420-430
10.
Sanchez MM, Young LJ, Plotsky PM, Insel TR: Distribution of corticosteroid receptors in the rhesus brain; relative absence of glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampal formation. J Neurosci 2000; 20:4657-4668
11.
Patel PD, Lopez JF, Lyons DM, Burke S, Wallace M, Schatzberg AF: Glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptor as RNA expression in squirrel monkey brain. J Psychiatr Res 2000; 34:383-392
12.
Schatzberg AF, Posener JA, DeBattista C, Kalehzan BM, Rothschild AJ, Shear PK: Neuropsychological deficits in psychotic versus nonpsychotic major depression and no mental illness. Am J Psychiatry. 2000; 157:1095-1100
13.
Belanoff JK, Kalehzan M, Sund B, Fleming Ficek SK, Schatzberg AF: Cortisol activity and cognitive changes in psychotic major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1612-1616
14.
Kendler KS, Kessler RC, Neale MC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ: The prediction of major depression in women: toward an integrated etiologic model. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150:1139-1148

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1077 - 1079

History

Published online: 1 July 2002
Published in print: July 2002

Authors

Details

Alan F. Schatzberg, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share