Skip to main content

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Although it is well established that cerebral activation increases with higher task load, the potential effects of training have been investigated over brief periods only. Training is of potential clinical relevance since training programs are an essential part of psychiatric therapy. METHOD: Cerebral activation during a visual spatial working memory task was examined before, during, and after 4 weeks of daily training in nine healthy subjects using functional magnetic resonance imaging. RESULTS: All subjects showed a pronounced activation mainly involving the right inferior frontal gyrus and the right intraparietal sulcus. While the respective regions showed activation increases with improved performance after 2 weeks of training, the activation values decreased at the time of consolidation of performance gains after 4 weeks. CONCLUSIONS: Our results suggest that training-related cerebral activation changes follow an inverse U-shaped quadratic function and raise the prospect of investigating cerebral mechanisms underlying training effects.
Neuroimaging studies have indicated that training over brief periods leads to a decreased activation of cortical regions related to working memory (13). However, the length of training was restricted to hours in order to complete the respective functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) experiments on a single day. In contrast to decreased activation in efficiently learned processes, previous studies have demonstrated that increased activation corresponds to better performance during working memory tasks (4, 5). These results appear to be rather conflicting but may be adequately addressed by investigating training-related activation changes over longer periods.
This is of particular importance since the underlying mechanisms for memory processes on the neuronal level—repetition suppression and enhancement (6)—may depend on training history, in particular, individual base level, or training intensity. Moreover, two clinical studies (7, 8) have described a compensatory increase in cortical activation related to use of working memory in patients with schizophrenia after training over 6 to 12 weeks.
Therefore, in the present study, we examined cerebral activation changes over a 4-week training program. To test the hypothesis that cerebral activation follows a complex time course, with initial activation increases at the time of improved performance but subsequent decreases after consolidation of performance gains, healthy subjects were investigated during three sessions (at baseline and after 2 weeks and 4 weeks of training, respectively) while executing a visual spatial working memory task by using fMRI.

Method

Nine right-handed healthy male subjects (ages 26 to 32 years, with 4 to 6 years of university-level education) were included. The subjects were medication free, task naive, and had no history of drug abuse, head trauma, or neurological or psychiatric illness. All subjects gave written informed consent, according to institutional guidelines (the Ethics Committee of the University of Heidelberg).
The spatial variant of the n-back task used was already applied in several previous fMRI experiments (3, 9). The task is comprised of three conditions (0-back, 1-back, and 2-back, respectively) with increasing difficulty, each consisting of 16 stimuli presented in a randomized sequence over a total duration of 45 seconds. The responses were continuously recorded to allow analyses of percent-correct replies. For the training, the subjects were asked to practice the task on a personal computer twice daily between the fMRI examinations.
A blocked design with four repetitions of each condition was performed at 1.5 T (Magnetom Vision Plus, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) by using the standard head coil and a gradient-echo prepared echo planar imaging sequence (volume of 15 slices, field of view=240×240 mm2, voxel size=1.88×1.88×4 mm3, flip angle=90°). The slice orientation was parallel to the anterior commissure/posterior commissure plane. Image processing and analysis was performed with SPM 99 software (http://www.fil.ion. ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Functional volumes were realigned to correct for motion artifacts, stereotactically normalized, and smoothed with a Gaussian kernel (full width at half maximum=8 mm). Statistical analysis was performed for each condition according to the general linear model approach (10).
At the first level of analysis, a statistical model for each session and each subject was computed to contrast periods during which the 0-back condition was performed with the 1-back and 2-back conditions, respectively. To test our hypothesis, a volume-of-interest analysis was performed for each subject and condition by using the regions identified by the comparison of 2-back versus 0-back over all sessions (k=20 voxels; p<0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons). On the basis of a second-level analysis, all conditions were compared with each other by calculating analyses of variance with testing for linear and quadratic trends (SPSS for Windows, version 11.0.1, SPSS Inc., Chicago).

Results

Even in the first session, the subjects made only a few errors during the 1-back condition (mean rate of relative errors=1.7%, SD=2.3); the improvement after training (second session: mean=1.4%, SD=3.2, third session: mean=0.5%, SD=1.1) did not reach statistical significance (F=1.54, df=1, 8, n.s., linear contrasts). For the 2-back condition, the relative error rate improved significantly (F=10.1, df=1, 8, p≤0.05, linear trend) from a mean of 6.9% (SD=4.1) in the first session to 1.3% (SD=2.6) in the second session and remained stable in the third session (mean=1.4%, SD=2.7). Hardly any errors were recorded in the 0-back condition (first session: 0.2%, SD=0.6; second and third sessions: 0%, SD=0).
With respect to the cortical hemodynamic responses, the group analysis revealed two main activation clusters for the contrast comparing the 2-back versus 0-back tasks: the right inferior frontal gyrus (Brodmann’s area 45) and the right intraparietal sulcus (Brodmann’s area 39/40). Apart from these regions, the respective functional volumes of interest also included parts of the medial frontal gyrus (Brodmann’s area 9/46) and the superior parietal lobe (Brodmann’s area 40). As presented in Figure 1, the changes in effect sizes were used to quantify the impact of training. With respect to the intraparietal sulcus/superior parietal lobe, a significant quadratic trend was found for the mean effect sizes under the 1-back condition (F=6.50, df=1, 8, p<0.05) and under the 2-back condition (F=9.96, df=1, 8, p<0.01). The respective values under the 0-back condition did not reach significance (F=4.37, df=1, 8, p=0.07). In the inferior/medial frontal gyrus, only a nonsignificant trend toward a quadratic function was found for the mean effect size under the 1-back condition (F=4.24, df=1, 8, p=0.07). Repeated analyses of the maximum effect sizes only revealed nonsignificant trends (3.37<F<4.43, df=1, 8, 0.07<p<0.10) toward quadratic functions for the intraparietal sulcus/superior parietal lobe
The mean and maximum effect sizes of both regions showed a significant (34.5<F<104.7, df=1, 8, 0.0001<p<0.001, linear trend) load effect with higher activation values at increased task demands.

Discussion

The brain regions activated in all sessions during performance of the visual spatial working memory task were generally consistent with previous reports and included right prefrontal and parietal areas (9, 11, 12). In each session, higher task demands corresponded to activation increases in the respective regions. Similar load effects were described for a variety of tasks, including working memory, repetitive movements, and affect recognition (9, 13, 14).
The training-related activation changes in the inferior frontal gyrus and the intraparietal sulcus are best described by an inverse U-shaped quadratic function with a negative exponent. While this function reached the significance level for the right intraparietal sulcus/superior parietal lobe under the 1-back and 2-back conditions only, both regions showed initial activation increases with improved performance, followed by subsequent activation decreases with consolidation of performance gains.
These activation changes support the hypothesis that training of working memory is mediated by different neuronal mechanisms, depending on training history. According to Desimone (6), three neuronal effects in memory-demanding tasks are commonly observed, namely, repetition suppression, enhancement, and delay activity. Following this hypothesis, our results suggest that the prefrontal and parietal cortices may contain at least two parallel mechanisms mediating the training effects of working memory: an enhancement mechanism for active working memory and a suppressive mechanism that is engaged automatically by stimulus repetition. Training, therefore, may lead to a change in predominance of one of these mechanisms triggered by the ongoing requests on manipulation, storage, and attention.
Our study has potential implications for fMRI studies since training effects occur regularly whenever similar tasks are repeatedly given. At this point, the question of whether the inverse U-shaped course of cerebral activation changes also applies to other cognitive domains or may be modified by important individual characteristics, such as age, education level, or gender cannot be addressed. However, our results emphasize the prospect of investigating cerebral mechanisms underlying training effects and optimizing the respective programs.
Figure 1. Training-Related Cerebral Activation Changes in Nine Normal Subjects During Performance of a Working Memory Taska
aMaximum and mean effect sizes of the volumes of interest (intraparietal sulcus/superior parietal lobe, 206 voxels; inferior frontal gyrus/medial frontal gyrus, 42 voxels) during the three different task conditions at baseline and after 2 and 4 weeks (sessions 1–3, respectively) of training.

Footnote

Presented in part at the 11th Biennial Winter Workshop on Schizophrenia, Davos, Switzerland, Feb. 24 to March 1, 2002. Received April 14, 2003, revision received Aug. 19, 2003; accepted Aug. 28, 2003. From the Section of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg; the German Cancer Research Center, Heidelberg, Germany; and the Institute of General and Experimental Psychology, Otto-von-Guericke University, Magdeburg, Germany. Address reprint requests to Dr. Schröder, Section of Geriatric Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg, Voßstr.4, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported in part by the Theodore and Vada Stanley Foundation; Dr. Hempel was supported by a Young Scientist Award/Davos 2002. The authors thank C. Bottmer, M.Sc., for her comments on the manuscript.

References

1.
Büchel C, Coull JT, Friston KJ: The predictive value of changes in effective connectivity for human learning. Science 1999; 283:1538–1541
2.
Garavan H, Kelley D, Rosen A, Rao SM, Stein EA: Practice-related functional activation changes in a working memory task. Microsc Res Tech 2000; 51:54–63
3.
Jansma JM, Ramsey NF, Slagter HA, Kahn RS: Functional anatomical correlates of controlled and automatic processing. J Cogn Neurosci 2001; 13:730–743
4.
Gray JR, Chabris CF, Braver TS: Neural mechanisms of general fluid intelligence. Nat Neurosci 2003; 6:316–322
5.
Bor D, Duncan J, Wiseman RJ, Owen AM: Encoding strategies dissociate prefrontal activity from working memory demand. Neuron 2003; 37:361–367
6.
Desimone R: Neural mechanisms for visual memory and their role in attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1996; 93:13494–13499
7.
Wexler BE, Anderson M, Fulbright RK, Gore JC: Preliminary evidence of improved verbal working memory performance and normalization of task-related frontal lobe activation in schizophrenia following cognitive exercises. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1694–1697
8.
Wykes T, Brammer M, Mellers J, Bray P, Reeder C, Williams C, Cormer J: Effects on the brain of a psychological treatment: cognitive remediation therapy. Br J Psychiatry 2002; 181:144–152
9.
Callicott JH, Mattay VS, Bertolino A, Finn K, Coppola R, Frank JA, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR: Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 1999; 9:20–26
10.
Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KPPJB, Frith CD, Frackoviak RSJ: Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp 1995; 2:189–210
11.
Jansma JM, Ramsey NF, Coppola R, Kahn RS: Specific versus nonspecific brain activity in a parametric n-back task. Neuroimage 2000; 12:688–697
12.
Smith EE, Jonides J: Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 1999; 283:1657–1661
13.
Schröder J, Essig M, Baudendistel K, Jahn T, Gerdsen I, Stockert A, Knopp MV, Schad LR: Motor dysfunction and sensorimotor cortex activation changes in schizophrenia: a study with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 1999; 9:81–87
14.
Hempel A, Hempel E, Schönknecht P, Stippich C, Schröder J: Impairment in basal limbic function in schizophrenia during affect recognition. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging 2003; 122:115–124

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 745 - 747
PubMed: 15056524

History

Published online: 1 April 2004
Published in print: April 2004

Authors

Details

Frederik L. Giesel, M.D.
Nohazarahit M. Garcia Caraballo, M.D.
Torsten Wüstenberg, Ph.D.
Johannes Schröder, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share