Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 June 2004

Neuropsychological Dysfunction in Antipsychotic-Naive First-Episode Unipolar Psychotic Depression

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: The profile of neuropsychological impairment associated with unipolar psychotic depression remains unclear. The authors used a neuropsychological test battery to characterize the neuropsychiatric profile of patients with unipolar psychotic depression, relative to that of patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression, patients with schizophrenia, and healthy comparison subjects. METHOD: Study subjects included antipsychotic-naive patients with a first episode of psychotic unipolar depression (N=20), antipsychotic-naive and unmedicated patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression (N=14), antipsychotic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia (N=86), and healthy volunteers (N=81). Groups were matched on age, sex, race, education, parental socioeconomic status, and estimated premorbid intelligence. Psychotic patients were followed clinically for 2 years to confirm diagnosis. All participants completed a standard neuropsychological battery, including tests of general intelligence, executive function, attention, verbal memory, motor skills, and visual-spatial perception. RESULTS: Patients with psychotic depression had a pattern of neuropsychological dysfunction that was similar to but less severe than that of patients with schizophrenia. In contrast, patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression had a neuropsychological profile that was similar to that of healthy individuals but that included mild dysfunction on tests of attention. Neuropsychological test performance was generally independent of acute clinical symptoms, but some pairwise group differences were attenuated by covariation for symptom severity. CONCLUSIONS: The similar neuropsychological profiles for schizophrenia and psychotic depression suggest that these psychotic disorders may have common pathophysiological features. The dramatic differences in performance between the patients with psychotic depression and those with nonpsychotic depression point to a marked distinction in neurocognitive function associated with the expression of psychosis in depressed patients.
Psychotic depression has been distinguished from nonpsychotic depression by its association with poorer premorbid function, longer depressive episodes, less responsiveness to antidepressant medication, and worse prognosis (1, 2). Empirical evidence also suggests that, compared with nonpsychotic depression, psychotic depression is associated with a higher rate of neurobiological alterations, such as structural brain abnormalities (35) and excessive hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis activity (6, 7). Yet the neuropsychological profile of psychotic depression, especially in early and mid-adulthood, has received little systematic investigation. Existing data are inconsistent, often methodologically limited, and potentially confounded by the effects of chronic disease and exposure to antipsychotic treatments.
One avenue to better understanding of the neurocognitive dysfunction associated with psychotic depression involves comparison with clinically related conditions, particularly schizophrenia, with which psychotic depression shares psychotic features, and nonpsychotic depression, with which it shares affective symptoms. Prior studies of patients with nonpsychotic depression have reported executive and attention difficulties (811) that are relatively benign, compared to the diffuse and severe neuropsychological dysfunction that is often characteristic of schizophrenia (1214). Whether the neuropsychological profile of psychotic depression is more similar to one or the other of these disorders is not known and would have implications for differential diagnosis, treatment, and prognosis. A similar pattern of neurocognitive deficits in psychotic and nonpsychotic unipolar depression would indicate a relatively benign pattern of cognitive and brain dysfunction, even when psychotic features accompany unipolar mood disorders. On the other hand, a neuropsychological profile of psychotic depression that is more consistent with that of schizophrenia would suggest common features of pathophysiology across these psychotic disorders. A neuropsychological profile markedly different from both disorders would provide further support for considering psychotic depression as a separate diagnostic classification and potentially suggest directions for future investigations of the disorder.
Psychotic depression has been associated with more severe dysfunction affecting a wider range of cognitive abilities than the mild and selective impairments observed in nonpsychotic depression (811). For example, in a previous study, patients with psychotic depression (N=12) showed impairments in memory and visual-spatial abilities that were associated with structural abnormalities found through magnetic resonance imaging (3). In another study, patients with psychotic depression (N=46) performed worse than matched patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression (N=34) on measures of verbal and nonverbal ability, attention, and manual dexterity (15). However, the majority of participants in both groups were receiving medication at the time of testing, and the effect of past and current treatments on cognitive function in this population is not well understood. To address potential medication effects, Schatzberg et al. (16) tested subjects after a 2-week washout period (6 weeks for antipsychotic medications) and found greater deficits in attention, response inhibition, and verbal memory for patients with psychotic depression (N=11), compared to healthy individuals (N=23) and patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression (N=32).
Less consistent findings have resulted from investigations comparing the neuropsychological profiles of patients with psychotic depression to those of patients with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. On measures of attention, memory, and problem solving, no differences were found between patients with schizophrenia and a heterogeneous group of patients with psychotic and nonpsychotic affective disorders (17). These findings led the authors to conclude that similar cognitive impairments in schizophrenia and affective disorders represent “a final common pathway.” However, it is unclear what proportion of the affective disorder group had a history of psychosis and what effect this history may have had on test performance. Goldberg and colleagues (18) found that schizophrenia patients scored significantly more poorly in several cognitive areas, compared with a combined group of patients with unipolar and bipolar affective illness. Similarly, chronic schizophrenia patients performed significantly more poorly on neuropsychological tests than patients with unipolar or bipolar psychotic affective disorder (19). However, the degree to which these findings can be attributed to the course of illness or to previous treatment with psychoactive medication is unclear because most of the patients were chronically ill and were receiving medication at the time of testing. In a study that specifically assessed nonverbal problem solving, Rossi et al. (20) found that patients with chronic psychotic bipolar illness scored between patients with chronic schizophrenia and healthy comparison subjects. Other studies have found that patients with psychotic affective disorder are more comparable to patients with schizophrenia. For example, an investigation of neuropsychological function in patients with first-episode schizophrenia and patients with first-episode affective disorders found that the schizophrenia patients performed worse than the affective disorder patients in some areas but that in other areas the level of impairment in the schizophrenia patients was similar to that in the patients with psychotic unipolar and bipolar depression (21). In a study of older patients, both patients with schizophrenia and those with psychotic unipolar depression showed greater dysfunction in motor abilities, attention, and learning, compared to patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression (22). Again, the majority of participants in these studies were receiving medication at the time of testing, and there was no clinical follow-up of first-episode patients to confirm diagnosis.
The lack of consistent findings regarding psychotic depression and other psychiatric disorders may reflect confounding variables and methodological limitations. In many previous studies, group comparisons were potentially confounded by chronic illness, effects of medication and other treatments, illness severity, and age-related effects. Methodological limitations have included small numbers of subjects, lack of healthy comparison groups, and a focus on limited neuropsychological domains. The present study was designed to characterize the profile of neurocognitive dysfunction in psychotic depression by 1) comparing groups of patients with psychiatric disorders that share the clinical features of either psychosis or depression, 2) assessing patients before any lifetime exposure to antipsychotic treatment, 3) including longitudinal clinical follow-up to confirm subjects’ diagnoses, 4) evaluating a broad range of cognitive abilities, 5) including a comparison group of healthy patients, and 6) minimizing potential confounds by matching all groups demographically.

Method

Subjects

After hospitalization for a first lifetime episode of psychosis, with no prior antipsychotic treatment, 106 consecutively admitted patients who met the research diagnostic criteria for a psychotic disorder on the basis of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R or DSM-IV (SCID) were recruited at the University of Pittsburgh Medical Center. Diagnosis was based on clinical data and was derived by trained raters and experienced clinicians during multidisciplinary consensus conferences. Scheduled follow-up evaluations at 12 and 24 months confirmed that 20 individuals met the criteria for unipolar depression with psychotic features. A diagnosis of a schizophrenia spectrum disorder (schizophrenia [N=71] or schizoaffective disorder [N=15]) was confirmed for the remaining 86 patients with a first episode of psychosis who were included in the study. Fourteen patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression confirmed by a SCID interview were recruited, as were 81 healthy individuals from the community who had no history of psychiatric treatment and no axis I disorder, as confirmed by a SCID interview. The patients with psychotic depression and schizophrenia had no prior lifetime treatment with antipsychotic medication. The patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression were antipsychotic naive and free of antidepressant medication for at least 3 weeks before enrollment. Several patients with psychotic symptoms (N=21) were previously treated with antidepressant medications and were free of those agents for more than 6 weeks on average. Treatment with antipsychotic or other medications was initiated after administration of the neuropsychological battery. All participants had no known history of neurologic disorders or of substance dependence or abuse within 6 months, no psychiatric disorders other than those previously specified, and no history of head injury with loss of consciousness, other medical disorders that may affect brain function, or recent/current ECT or treatment with anticonvulsants or benzodiazepines. Verbal and written consent were provided by all participants, and the study was approved by the University of Pittsburgh Institutional Review Board.
All groups were matched demographically on age, sex, and parental socioeconomic status, as measured with the Hollingshead Index of Social Position. Demographic and clinical data are summarized in Table 1. Analysis of demographic information revealed that the healthy comparison subjects had higher personal socioeconomic status, compared to the nonpsychotic unipolar depression and schizophrenia groups. However, there were no statistically significant group differences for mean estimated premorbid intelligence level, as measured by the Wide Range Achievement Test, Revised reading subtest (25).

Clinical and Neuropsychological Evaluation

Participants in the three psychiatric groups underwent diagnostic examination with the SCID. The severity of their psychiatric symptoms was rated with the the 24-item Hamilton Depression Rating Scale, the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS), the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) (23), and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) (24). The Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (GAF) score (DSM-III-R, p. 12; DSM-IV, p. 32) was obtained as part of the SCID. Of the 20 participants with psychotic depression, 80% (N=16) displayed mood incongruent psychotic features.
All participants completed a standard neuropsychological battery before beginning treatment with antipsychotic agents. At the time of testing, the examiners were unaware of the patients’ diagnoses, which typically had not yet been determined. The battery included tests of general intelligence, executive function, attention, verbal memory, visual memory, motor skills, and visual-spatial perception. The tests used to evaluate function in each of these neuropsychological domains, determined on an a priori basis, are listed in Table 2.

Data Analysis

To provide a standard metric for comparison across neuropsychological tests and domains, test scores were standardized (by using z scores) to the performance of the healthy comparison group. To meet the assumptions for parametric analysis, each of the neuropsychological variables was assessed for normality of distribution by calculating two-tailed tests of skewness and kurtosis. The scores for both the Judgment of Line Orientation and California Verbal Learning Test recognition discrimination were negatively skewed in a manner that was not amenable to normalization by using routine transformations. For these two scores only, non-normalized reference means and standard deviations were computed by using the model of a one-tailed z distribution. Outliers resulted in nonnormal distributions for the percentage of perseverative errors on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Trail Making Test part B, the Wechsler Memory Scale—3rd ed. delayed visual reproduction test, the bilateral Finger Tapping Test, and the dominant-hand Grooved Pegboard Test. The distribution of these variables normalized when the outliers were omitted, and the z scores were computed by using the normalized distribution. Examination of individual variable scores revealed some extreme scores (five to eight standard deviations below the mean) that were truncated to z=–5.0 to avoid undue influence of outliers. After normalization and truncation, scores for each neuropsychological function were computed as the mean of the variables that compose each function (14, 34). Specifically, summary measures were calculated for executive function, attention, verbal memory, visual memory, motor skills, and visual-spatial perception. A composite of global neuropsychological function was computed as the mean of all six summary scores.
To assess group differences in neuropsychological function, one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted by using the global composite scores. In the presence of group differences in global neuropsychological function, simple pairwise group contrasts were conducted to clarify group differences. To assess potential profile differences among groups, two-way repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted with neuropsychological function (executive function, attention, verbal memory, visual memory, motor skills, and visual-spatial perception) as the within-subjects factor and diagnostic group (schizophrenia, psychotic depression, nonpsychotic depression, and healthy comparison) as the between-subjects variable. Because of unequal cell size, Pillai’s test was used to compare the groups. Significant interactions were clarified by using a series of univariate ANOVAs for each neuropsychological function. To maintain acceptably low type I error rates, we employed a conservative approach to a priori planned analyses by utilizing Bonferroni corrections for simple contrasts and post hoc group comparisons. The unequal group sizes may have differentially influenced power, particularly for comparisons involving the two groups of depressed patients, and the probability of type II error is unclear. Demographic covariates were not used during analysis of neuropsychological performance because the groups did not differ significantly in age, sex, education, parental socioeconomic status, or premorbid intelligence.

Results

Clinical Features

Compared to the two groups with unipolar depression, the schizophrenia group was rated as having more positive symptoms of psychosis and lower global (daily) functioning (Table 1). Negative symptom ratings were greater for the schizophrenia patients than for the patients with nonpsychotic depression. There was no difference in the severity of negative symptoms between the schizophrenia group and the psychotic depression group.

Neuropsychological Comparisons

To assess the possibility that neuropsychological differences existed between the 15 patients with schizoaffective disorder and the remaining participants in the schizophrenia group, a two-way repeated measures MANOVA was conducted (group [schizoaffective disorder versus schizophrenia] by neuropsychological domain). The results showed no omnibus difference in neuropsychological performance between the subjects with schizoaffective disorder and those with schizophrenia (F=0.93, df=1, 83, p>0.05). Thus, data for the schizoaffective disorder patients and schizophrenia patients were pooled in the following analyses, consistent with the a priori data analytic plan.
Global neuropsychological performance differed significantly among the diagnostic groups (F=25.76, df=3, 197, p<0.001) (Figure 1). Simple contrasts among groups made with a Bonferroni correction revealed that global performance was significantly better for the healthy participants, compared to the schizophrenia and psychotic depression groups. Schizophrenia patients also performed significantly worse overall, compared to the patients with nonpsychotic depression. The results of a repeated-measures MANOVA to clarify differences in global neuropsychological function are illustrated in Figure 2. Significant main effects were found for both diagnosis (F=26.6, df=3, 196, p<0.001) and neuropsychological function (F=5.93, df=5, 192, p<0.001), as well as for the interaction of diagnosis and neuropsychological function (F=2.54, df=15, 582, p=0.001). Univariate ANOVAs were conducted separately for each neuropsychological function to clarify the two-way interaction (Table 3). With the exception of having lower attention scores than the healthy comparison subjects, the patients with nonpsychotic depression performed similarly to the healthy comparison group in all areas. The schizophrenia patients performed more poorly than the healthy participants in all domains and more poorly than the patients with nonpsychotic depression in all domains but attention. Significantly impaired performance was noted for the patients with psychotic depression, relative to the healthy participants, in all areas but verbal memory and visual-spatial perception. The patients with psychotic depression performed more poorly than their nonpsychotic counterparts in visual memory and visual-spatial perception. The psychotic depression and schizophrenia groups differed only on measures of executive function and attention.
As Table 1 shows, several group differences were noted in clinical symptoms. To assess the potential effect of clinical state on neuropsychological performance, correlations were computed among clinical variables and each of the six neuropsychological domains. Despite differences in the severity of positive symptoms among the patient groups, no relationship was found between SAPS scores and neuropsychological performance in any domain for any clinical group. Similarly, no relationship between GAF scores and neuropsychological performance was found for any patient group. A significant negative correlation was found between SANS scores and executive function for schizophrenia patients (r=–0.35, df=83, p=0.001). Among patients with psychotic depression, BPRS scores were significantly correlated with global neuropsychological performance (r=–0.52, df=17, p<0.05), visual memory (r=–0.57, df=17, p=0.01), motor skills (r=–0.46, df=17, p=0.05), and visual-spatial perception (r=–0.56, df=17, p<0.05).
To examine the extent to which profile differences among clinical groups could be attributed to the level of overall psychiatric (BPRS) or negative symptoms (SANS), univariate ANOVAs were rerun with covariation of BPRS and SANS scores to analyze the significant clinical group contrasts reported in Table 3. For executive function, the schizophrenia patients no longer differed from the patients with nonpsychotic depression (F=1.35, df=1, 90, p>0.05) or the patients with psychotic depression (F=3.35, df=1, 97, p>0.05) when symptom severity was covaried. In contrast, differences in attention between the schizophrenia group and the psychotic depression group were robust even after the covariation of clinical symptoms (F=5.95, df=1, 97, p<0.05). For verbal memory, the difference between the schizophrenia group and the nonpsychotic unipolar depression group dissipated when the comparison controlled for variance in BPRS and SANS scores (F=2.46, df=1, 90, p>0.05). The patients with nonpsychotic depression displayed a robust advantage over the schizophrenia patients in visual memory when clinical symptoms were covaried (F=6.76, df=1, 90, p=0.01), but their advantage over the patients with psychotic depression was lost (F=0.67, df=1, 23, p=0.42). Differences among clinical groups for visual-spatial perception were robust after covariation of clinical symptoms: the patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression outperformed both the schizophrenia patients (F=4.27, df=1, 89, p<0.05) and the patients with psychotic depression (F=4.08, df=1, 23, p<0.05). Finally, when the severity of clinical symptoms was covaried, the patients with nonpsychotic unipolar depression and the patients with schizophrenia no longer differed on motor skills (F=2.89, df=1, 23, p>0.05).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first study to provide data that document the neuropsychological profile of psychotic depression in young adults at the time of the first episode of illness, before treatment with antipsychotic medication. Furthermore, the neuropsychological status of these patients was compared with that of patients with other psychiatric disorders that share clinical features with psychotic depression—first-episode schizophrenia, which shares the feature of psychosis, and nonpsychotic unipolar depression, which shares the feature of depressive illness. Visual inspection of the neuropsychological profiles for those groups, represented in Figure 2, indicates remarkably similar patterns of cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychotic depression. Absolute levels of dysfunction for all cognitive areas were less severe for patients with psychotic depression than for those with schizophrenia, and statistically significant differences in attention between those groups were robust after covariation of symptom severity. In contrast, the cognitive profile for nonpsychotic unipolar depression differed dramatically from that for psychotic depression and was more consistent with the cognitive profile for the healthy comparison group. The level of performance was generally lower in psychotic versus nonpsychotic unipolar depression, and statistically significant differences between those groups were present for visual-spatial perception. These data not only provide additional support for psychotic depression as a distinct mood disorder but also document the considerable neuropsychological morbidity associated with the disorder. Further, the similarity in the profiles of neuropsychological dysfunction in schizophrenia and psychotic depression suggests that similar, cognitively relevant brain systems are affected in both psychotic disorders. Ultimately, while these two psychotic disorders are likely to result from fundamentally different mechanisms at the level of molecular processes and synaptic physiology, their similarity at the systems level suggests that the disorders share a final common pathway (17) in terms of their effect on neurocognitive abilities.
In this study, the potential effects of chronic psychotic symptoms and the potential acute or chronic effects of antipsychotic medications were reduced by inclusion of participants with psychotic symptoms during their first episode of psychosis and before antipsychotic treatment. All groups were also matched for age, sex, education, premorbid intellectual abilities, and parental socioeconomic status. Consequently, the observed similarities and differences in cognitive profiles are not attributable to demographic features, medication status, premorbid abilities, or duration of illness. However, differences in clinical symptoms were observed among the clinical groups (Table 1). Although the correlation analysis revealed relative independence of cognitive performance and clinical state, covariation of BPRS and SANS scores attenuated some group differences, particularly the differences in executive function, verbal memory, visual memory, and motor skills. These findings offer further evidence that psychotic depression is neuropsychologically more similar to schizophrenia than to nonpsychotic unipolar depression (1921, 22, 35). The similarity of neuropsychological profiles observed in psychotic depression and schizophrenia, when considered in conjunction with the results of investigations assessing psychotic and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder patients (1719, 21), suggests that psychosis may be related to a similar global neuropsychological dysfunction regardless of the presence or absence of affective symptoms, albeit perhaps a less severe dysfunction in affective psychotic disorders. Studies that more directly assess neurophysiology and both functional and structural brain abnormalities among these groups are warranted to clarify the underlying similarities and distinctions.
The patients with schizophrenia displayed a profile of executive, attention, and memory deficits consistent with previously documented frontal-temporal dysfunction (1214). These cognitive impairments are enduring features of schizophrenia (3639) that disrupt occupational function and cause long-term disability (40, 41). The current data suggest a somewhat less severe clinical manifestation and level of neuropsychological dysfunction in psychotic depression. However, several questions remain. Is the course of neuropsychological dysfunction in psychotic depression similar to that observed in schizophrenia? If the neuropsychological dysfunction associated with psychotic depression is enduring, then can cognitive behavior therapy, psychopharmacologic treatments, or other interventions reduce neuropsychological or functional deficits?
The mild difficulties in executive function and attention observed in the nonpsychotic unipolar depression group were consistent with previous characterizations of depression that have implicated attention and executive systems (811, 42). Despite the small number of participants with mood disorders in this study and the need for replication of the current data, the findings augment the growing support for the presence of mild disruption of frontal-striatal circuitry in nonpsychotic depression (43, 44).
In summary, the study found that the neuropsychological dysfunction associated with psychotic depression is similar in quality and level of dysfunction to that associated with schizophrenia (when the effects of symptom severity were controlled statistically). On the other hand, the neuropsychological profile for psychotic depression was markedly different in shape and severity, compared to that for nonpsychotic depression. The deficits associated with psychotic depression were not attributable to previous exposure to antipsychotic medication, chronic disease effects, potential diagnostic errors at index examination, or demographic characteristics. Thus, psychosis appears to be associated with a common profile of diffuse neurocognitive dysfunction, regardless of whether it occurs in the context of schizophrenia or depression. Additional studies are needed to assess the long-term stability of neuropsychological impairment in both early- and late-onset psychotic depression, relative to the persistent deficits found in schizophrenia, and ultimately to identify etiological factors underlying the systems-level brain disturbance implicated by the neuropsychological data for patients with psychotic disorders.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
TABLE 3
Figure 1. Global Neuropsychological Performance of Healthy Comparison Subjects and Patients With Nonpsychotic Depression, Psychotic Depression, and Schizophreniaa
aHealthy comparison subjects: mean=<–0.01 (SD=0.52); patients with nonpsychotic depression: mean=<0.01 (SD=0.47); patients with psychotic depression: mean=–0.59 (SD=0.91); patients with schizophrenia: mean=–0.95 (SD=0.86).
Figure 2. Performance in Six Neuropsychological Domains of Healthy Comparison Subjects and Patients With Nonpsychotic Depression, Psychotic Depression, and Schizophrenia

Footnote

Received March 14, 2003; revision received Oct. 7, 2003; accepted Oct. 10, 2003. From the Center for Cognitive Medicine, Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois at Chicago; and Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh. Address reprint requests to Dr. Hill, Center for Cognitive Medicine, Department of Psychiatry (M/C 913), University of Illinois at Chicago, 912 South Wood St., Suite 235, Chicago, IL 60612; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression; NIMH grants MH-45156, MH-41884, MH-01433, MH-62134, and MH-42968; and General Clinical Research Center grant RR-00056 from the NIH National Center for Research Resources. The authors thank Dawn Boarts, Jill Brown, Stephen Hegedus, and Christine Krisky for assistance with patient testing and Drs. Gretchen Haas, Ph.D., Cameron Carter, M.D., and the clinical core staff of the Center for the Neuroscience of Mental Disorders in Pittsburgh for their assistance in diagnostic and psychopathological assessments.

References

1.
Meyers BS: Late-life delusional depression: acute and long-term treatment. Int Psychogeriatr 1995; 7(suppl):113–124
2.
Sands JR, Harrow M: Psychotic unipolar depression at follow-up: factors related to psychosis in the affective disorders. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:995–1000
3.
Lesser IM, Miller BL, Boone KB, Hill-Gutierrez E, Mehringer CM, Wong K, Mena I: Brain injury and cognitive function in late-onset psychotic depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1991; 3:33–40
4.
Rothschild AJ, Benes F, Hebben N, Woods B, Luciana M, Bakanas E, Samson JA, Schatzberg AF: Relations between brain CT scan findings and cortisol in psychotic and nonpsychotic depressed patients. Biol Psychiatry 1989; 26:565–575
5.
Shiraishi H, Koizumi J, Hori M, Terashima Y, Suzuki T, Saito K, Mizukami K, Tanaka Y, Yamaguchi N: A computerized tomographic study in patients with delusional and non-delusional depression. Jpn J Psychiatry Neurol 1992; 46:99–105
6.
Anton RF: Urinary free cortisol in psychotic depression. Biol Psychiatry 1987; 2:24–34
7.
Schatzberg AF, Rothschild AJ, Stahl JB, Bond TC, Rosenbaum AH, Lofgren SB, MacLaughlin RA, Sullivan MA, Cole JO: The dexamethasone suppression test: identification of subtypes of depression. Am J Psychiatry 1983; 140:88–91
8.
Bench CJ, Friston KJ, Brown RG, Scott L, Frackowiak RSJ, Dolan RJ: The anatomy of melancholia: abnormalities of regional cerebral blood flow in major depression. Psychol Med 1992; 22:607–615
9.
Dolan RJ, Bench CJ, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Grasby PM, Frackowiak RSJ: Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction in the major psychoses: symptom or disease specificity? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1992; 56:1290–1294
10.
Drevets WC, Videen TO, Price JL, Preskorn SH, Carmichael T, Raichle ME: A functional anatomical study of unipolar depression. J Neurosci 1992; 12:3628–3641
11.
Fossati P, Amar G, Raoux N, Ergis AM, Allilaire JF: Executive functioning and verbal memory in young patients with unipolar depression and schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 1999; 89:171–187
12.
Bilder RM, Bogerts B, Ashtari W, Wu H, Alvir JM, Jody D, Reiter G, Bell L, Lieberman JA: Anterior hippocampal volume reductions predict frontal lobe dysfunction in first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1995; 17:47–58
13.
Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Pliskin NH, Podd MH: Further evidence for dementia of the prefrontal type in schizophrenia? a controlled study of teaching the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987; 48:1008–1014
14.
Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE, Kester DB, Mozley LH, Stafiniak P, Gur RC: Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:124–131
15.
Basso MR, Bornstein RA: Neuropsychological deficits in psychotic versus nonpsychotic unipolar depression. Neuropsychology 1999; 13:69–75
16.
Schatzberg AF, Posener JA, DeBattista C, Kalehzan BM, Rothschild AJ, Shear PK: Neuropsychological deficits in psychotic versus nonpsychotic major depression and no mental illness. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1095–1100
17.
Zihl J, Gron G, Brunnauer A: Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia and affective disorders: evidence for a final common pathway disorder. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1998; 97:351–357
18.
Goldberg TE, Torrey EF, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Ragland RD, Taylor E, Weinberger DR: Genetic risk of neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia: a study of monozygotic twins discordant and concordant for the disorder. Schizophr Res 1995; 17:77–84
19.
Mojtabai R, Bromet EJ, Harvey PD, Carlson GA, Craig TJ, Fennig S: Neuropsychological differences between first-admission schizophrenia and psychotic affective disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1453–1460
20.
Rossi A, Arduini L, Daneluzzo E, Bustini M, Prosperini P, Stratta P: Cognitive function in euthymic bipolar patients, stabilized schizophrenic patients, and healthy controls. J Psychiatr Res 2000; 34:333–339
21.
Albus M, Hubmann W, Wahlheim C, Sobizack N, Franz U, Mohr F: Contrasts in neuropsychological test profiles between patients with first-episode schizophrenia and first-episode affective disorders. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1996; 94:87–93
22.
Jeste DV, Heaton S, Paulsen JS, Ercoli L, Harris MJ, Heaton RK: Clinical and neuropsychological comparison of psychotic depression with nonpsychotic depression and schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:490–496
23.
Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1983
24.
Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1983
25.
Jastak S, Wilkinson GS: The Wide Range Achievement Test, Revised. Wilmington, Del, Jastak Associates, 1984
26.
Heaton RK: The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1981
27.
Golden CJ: Stroop Color and Word Test: A Manual for Clinical and Experimental Uses. Wood Dale, Ill, Stoelting Co, 1978
28.
Reitan RM, Wolfson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation, 2nd ed. Tucson, Ariz, Neuropsychology Press, 1993
29.
Benton AL, Hamsher K: Multilingual Aphasia Examination Manual. Iowa City, University of Iowa, Department of Neurology, 1976
30.
Weintraub S, Mesulam MM: Mental state assessment of young and elderly adults in behavioral neurology, in Principles of Behavioral Neurology. Edited by Mesulam MM. Philadelphia, Davis, 1985, pp 77–123
31.
Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA: The California Verbal Learning Test Manual. New York, Psychological Corp, 1987
32.
Wechsler D: The Wechsler Memory Scale—3rd ed. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp (Harcourt), 1997
33.
Matthews CG, Klove H: Instruction Manual for the Adult Neuropsychology Test Battery. Madison, Wisc, University of Wisconsin Medical School, 1964
34.
Censits DM, Ragland JD, Gur RC, Gur RE: Neuropsychological evidence supporting a neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Schizophr Res 1997; 24:289–298
35.
Nelson EB, Sax KW, Strakowski SM: Attentional performance in patients with psychotic and nonpsychotic major depression and schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:137–139
36.
Gold S, Arndt S, Nopoulos P, O’Leary DS, Andreasen NC: Longitudinal study of cognitive function in first-episode and recent-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1342–1348
37.
Hoff AL, Sakuma M, Wieneke M, Horon R, Kushner M, DeLisi LE: Longitudinal neuropsychological follow-up study of patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1336–1341
38.
Sweeney JA, Haas GL, Keilp JG, Long M: Evaluation of the stability of neuropsychological functioning after acute episodes of schizophrenia: one-year follow-up study. Psychiatry Res 1991; 38:63–76
39.
Hill SK, Schuepbach D, Herbener ES, Keshavan MS, Sweeney JA: Pretreatment and longitudinal studies of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia with never-medicated first-episode patients. Schizophr Res 2004; 68:49–63
40.
Green MF: What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:321–330
41.
Velligan DI, Bow-Thomas CC, Mahurin RK, Miller AL, Halgunseth BA: Do specific neurocognitive deficits predict specific domains of community function in schizophrenia? J Nerv Ment Dis 2000; 188:518–524
42.
Sweeney JA, Strojwas MH, Mann JJ, Thase ME: Prefrontal and cerebellar abnormalities in major depression: evidence from oculomotor studies. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 43:584–594
43.
Cassens G, Wolfe L, Zola M: The neuropsychology of depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1990; 2:202–213
44.
Merriam EP, Thase ME, Haas GL, Keshavan MS, Sweeney JA: Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in depression determined by Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:780–782

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 996 - 1003
PubMed: 15169687

History

Published online: 1 June 2004
Published in print: June 2004

Authors

Details

S. Kristian Hill, Ph.D.
Matcheri S. Keshavan, M.D.
Michael E. Thase, M.D.
John A. Sweeney, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share