Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 September 2005

Maternal Absence and Stability of Individual Differences in CSF 5-HIAA Concentrations in Rhesus Monkey Infants

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Early life events often lead to deficits in CNS serotonin function, which underlie a number of reoccurring psychopathological disorders. Studies using rhesus macaques have demonstrated that early maternal deprivation reduces CNS serotonin turnover, as measured by cisternal CSF 5-HIAA concentrations. In addition, individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA remain stable from the first year of life through adulthood. The purpose of this study was to assess 1) the impact of rearing environment on the early development (<6 months of age) of the serotonin system, and 2) at what stage of early development individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations stabilize. METHOD: The subjects were 256 infant rhesus macaques reared in three different conditions (mother-reared, peer-reared, and surrogate/peer-reared). Cisternal CSF was obtained at 14, 30, 60, 90, 120, and 150 days of age. RESULTS: No differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations were observed between peer only- and surrogate/peer-reared infants, and these groups combined exhibited lower 5-HIAA concentrations than mother-reared infants throughout early development. CSF 5-HIAA concentrations declined with increasing age regardless of rearing condition. Within each rearing condition, individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations remained stable from 14 to 150 days of age. CONCLUSIONS: Early maternal deprivation reduces CNS serotonin turnover, and individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations are trait-like and appear to stabilize in infancy.
Serotonin is thought to play a major role in a number of psychiatric diseases and various symptoms of other psychopathologies (1). To the extent that serotonin-mediated psychopathology is a life-long pattern of reoccurring episodes of disease with impaired function and distress (1), it is reasonable to postulate that stability of interindividual differences in central functioning would underlie the long-term risk for serotonin-mediated psychopathological disorders. CSF concentrations of the major serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) are thought to reflect CNS serotonin turnover rates (1). Whereas low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations have been linked to a variety of neuropsychiatric disorders (2), long-term individual stability of CNS serotonin functioning has rarely been investigated in humans, and longitudinal assessments to investigate the genesis of low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations have not been performed in humans, in part because of the difficulty of repeated CSF sampling in human subjects.
Nonhuman primates offer numerous advantages in modeling human psychopathology. Not only are they our closest relatives, but they have a relatively long period of development that, especially in anthropoids, involves considerable dependence on the mother. Furthermore, we can control rearing history in order to test hypotheses concerning the role of early experiences. Consequently, during the past decade, nonhuman primates have been used to assess stability of individual differences in CNS serotonin turnover. Several studies have demonstrated short- and long-term trait-like interindividual stability of CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in nonhuman primate species (310). Such individual differences are stable across different settings and situations, even in conditions that temporarily increase CSF 5-HIAA concentrations (36, 8). However, most of the studies have investigated either adult animals or infants who are near the age of weaning (3, 6, 11–13). Little is known about the earliest periods of development and at what point individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations stabilize. One study of rhesus macaques, which used a relatively small sample size, reported that neonatal and early infancy CSF 5-HIAA concentrations were not correlated with adult CSF 5-HIAA concentrations and that individual stability did not emerge until after the fourth month of life (7).
Early rearing history may play a crucial role in the development of the CNS serotonin system of nonhuman primates. Several studies have demonstrated that monkeys removed from their mother at birth and reared in a nursery with age-matched peers (termed peer-reared) exhibit chronically low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations that are evident by late infancy and continue into adolescence and adulthood (3, 4, 6, 11). However, this finding has not always been replicated (7, 14), perhaps in part because of methodological differences in the conditions of mother-rearing across studies. Specifically, studies that indicate no differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations between peer- and mother-reared animals have utilized relatively small sample sizes and limited the mother-reared rearing condition to mother-rearing in dyads (i.e., mothers and infants housed alone). Mother-only rearing, in the absence of peers, is a form of deprivation rearing, resulting in aberrations in social behavior, unstable dominance relationships, and increased stereotyped behaviors (15). Therefore, infants reared only with mothers, and thus deprived of peer interaction, may not represent an appropriate control group with which to compare the effects of early nursery rearing.
The primary purposes of the current study were 1) to determine the effects of rearing condition on rhesus monkey CSF 5-HIAA concentrations during early development and 2) to assess the stability of individual differences in those concentrations. In studying the effects of rearing condition, we utilized three conditions: mother-reared in social groups, peer only-reared, and surrogate/peer-reared. Although the effects of early peer only rearing on CSF 5-HIAA concentrations have been well documented, to date no comparable studies have been performed with surrogate/peer-reared infants. Surrogate/peer rearing is widely utilized in nonhuman primate nurseries, and as such, study of the physiological concomitants of this rearing condition is warranted. Furthermore, the surrogate/peer-rearing condition, by combining maternal deprivation with limited peer contact (surrogate/peer-reared infants are provided social contact with other animals only a few hours each day), allows us to further explore those aspects of early experience that may lead to deficits in the serotonin system. In other words, by comparing surrogate/peer-reared monkeys to peer only-reared monkeys—and ultimately to mother-reared monkeys—we can test whether deficits in the serotonin system may result from general social deprivation in addition to maternal absence.
To the degree that individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations are stable and represent a risk marker for a variety of psychopathologies, establishing how early in life individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations become stable may provide important clues to assess developmental trajectories and to develop intervention strategies. We therefore designed our study to begin during the neonatal period and to involve repeated sampling of a large number of infants across the first 5 months of life.

Method

Subjects

The subjects were 256 rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) infants (144 males and 112 females). At birth, infants were assigned to one of three rearing conditions: mother reared (N=102; 58 males, 44 females), peer reared (N=73; 38 males, 35 females), or surrogate/peer reared (N=81; 48 males, 33 females).
Rearing condition assignments were made at random with the exception that an attempt was made to balance the number of males and females in each rearing condition in a given birth year. Mother-reared infants were housed in social groups approximating natural conditions, with 8–10 adult females (including the infant’s mother), the infant’s father and another adult male, and other similar-aged infants. These social groups were housed in indoor/outdoor enclosures measuring 2.44×3.05×2.21 m (indoor) and 2.44×3.0×2.44 m (outdoor). The floors of the enclosures were covered with wood chips. The lights in the indoor portion of the enclosure were maintained on a 12:12 cycle (7:00 a.m. to 7:00 p.m.). The animals in the social groups were fed high-protein monkey chow and received water ad libitum. Supplemental fruit was provided three times each week; sunflower seeds were presented daily.
Infants assigned to the peer only-reared and surrogate/peer-reared conditions were removed from their mothers between birth and 2 days of age and taken to the neonatal nursery, where they were reared according to standard procedures (16). During the first 37 days of life, the peer only-reared and surrogate/peer-reared infants were treated identically. From days 1–15 the infants were housed individually in plastic “incubator” cages measuring 51×38×43 cm. Each cage contained an inanimate, rocking “surrogate mother,” composed of a 16.5-cm circumference polypropylene cylinder 25 cm in height attached to an 11.5-cm wide circular metal base by a flexible metal spring. The surrogate mother was covered by an electrical heating pad, which in turn was covered with fleece fabric. A bottle with a nipple was attached to the surrogate mother for purposes of feeding. Loose pieces of fleece covered the floor of the cage. The internal temperature of the plastic “incubator” was maintained at approximately 27°C. The infants could see and hear, but not physically contact, other infants. At 15 days of age, infants were moved into individual wire mesh cages measuring 64×61×76 cm, with their fleece-covered surrogate and fleece blankets. Lights were on a 14:10 hour cycle (7:00 a.m. to 9:00 p.m.). The temperature of the room was maintained between 22°C and 26°C, and the humidity was kept at 50%–55%.
At 37 days of age all nursery infants were placed into the condition (peer only or surrogate/peer reared) randomly assigned at the time of their birth. This took place on day 37 because each subject was run through a novel cage experiment exactly 1 week after day 30, just prior to being placed into a rearing condition. Peer only-reared monkeys were placed in permanent social groups of four age-matched infants, housed together with constant access 24 hours a day in cages measuring 71×81×152 cm. Conversely, surrogate/peer-reared infants continued to be housed individually with their surrogates as described. Surrogate/peer-reared infants were provided with only limited peer contact, being placed in play groups of four individuals, along with their surrogates, in a 71×81×152 cm exercise cage for 2 hours/day, 5 days/week. Following the 2-hour play session, each surrogate/peer-reared infant was returned to its home cage for the remainder of the day. While surrogate/peer-reared infants were provided access to their surrogates at all times throughout the 5 months of the study, infants in the peer only-reared groups retained their surrogates only until the average age of the group members was 4 months.
All peer only-reared and surrogate/peer-reared infants were hand-fed a 50:50 mixture of Similac (Ross Laboratories, Columbus, Ohio) and Primalac (Bio-Serv, Frenchtown, N.J.) formula every 2 hours until 30 days of age. From day 30 until 4 months of age, formula was administered at 400 ml/day, standard high-protein monkey chow was gradually introduced to the diet, and the infants were provided ad libitum access to water. At 4 months the amount of formula provided was reduced to 300 ml/day, and at 5 months it was reduced again to 200 ml/day. At 6 months of age the infants were completely weaned from formula, and provided a diet of monkey chow, supplemental fruit, and water.

Data Collection

Cisternal CSF samples were collected at 14, 30, 60, 90, 120, and 150 days of age. All of the CSF samples were collected in conjunction with blood samples that—on days 14, 30, 90, 120, and 150—were used to measure plasma cortisol and ACTH responses to a moderately challenging condition for a separate study of rearing condition effects on the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) system. On days 14 and 30, blood and CSF samples were collected following a 30-minute assessment of orientation, temperament, and motor maturity, which involved handling by a human experimenter (17). On days 90, 120, and 150, subjects were removed from their mother (mother-reared animals), peer group (peer only-reared animals), or home cage (surrogate/peer-reared animals) and isolated in an unfamiliar single cage (64×61×76 cm) for 30 minutes. At the end of the 30-minute period, blood and CSF samples were obtained. On day 60, however, both blood and CSF samples were collected after immediate capture and anesthetization. These samples were intended to provide baseline (nonchallenge) measures of plasma and CSF biochemical measures. All samples were collected between 11:30 a.m. and 2:30 p.m.
For CSF collection, animals were anesthetized using ketamine HCl (intramuscular: 10 mg/kg). One ml of CSF was obtained from the cisterna magna using a 22-gauge needle and 5-cc syringe within 15 minutes of anesthesia. CSF samples were immediately placed in liquid nitrogen and later transferred to a –70° freezer. Samples were assayed for 5-HIAA using high-performance liquid chromatography with electrochemical detection (18, 19). All intra- and interassay coefficients of variation were <10%.

Data Analysis

The data were first plotted to detect and eliminate possible outliers that could bias the statistical analyses. Preliminary analysis of the data using mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed no significant main effect of sex (F=0.002, df=1, 252, p<0.97) nor any significant interactions between sex and rearing condition (F=2.28, df=1, 252, p<0.14) or sex and age (F=1.85, df=5, 252, p<0.11) on CSF 5-HIAA concentrations. As a result, data for males and females were combined in subsequent analyses. Additionally, comparison of the two groups reared in the neonatal nursery (peer only-reared and surrogate/peer-reared) indicated no differences between these groups in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations (F=0.39, df=1, 152, p<0.54), and similar developmental changes for both groups. Consequently, the peer only-reared and surrogate/peer-reared animals were combined into a single category, nursery reared, for subsequent analyses.
In order to address the two main issues of this study, two types of analyses were conducted. First, we utilized locally weighted scatterplot smoothing to evaluate the effects of early rearing condition (mother reared versus nursery reared) on the development of the serotonin system during the first 5 months of life. Locally weighted scatterplot smoothing fits a nonlinear regression surface to data via local smoothing (20, 21). A “window,” or a portion of the surrounding data, is used to construct a line or curve at each data point. Locally weighted scatterplot smoothing fits were generated for mother-reared and nursery-reared subjects with the use of macros written for SAS (22) and modified by Dr. Dennis Young of Arizona State University. First, a range of “windows” (f values) was tested in order to identify the optimal f used to produce the final fits. The optimal f value of 0.2 was identified using the M plot method of Cleveland and Devlin (21). The final fits for mother-reared and nursery-reared subjects were then compared with the jackknife method proposed by Moses et al. (23), which addresses the problem of dependent data points that result from repeated measurements on each animal. By using locally weighted scatterplot smoothing, we were able to determine 1) whether CSF 5-HIAA concentrations change over time, 2) whether there was an effect of rearing condition on CSF 5-HIAA concentrations, averaged over time, and 3) whether the pattern of change over time was the same for each rearing condition.
Second, we utilized correlation analyses to assess the stability of individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations across the first 5 months of life and to determine at what stage in development these individual differences stabilized. Correlation analyses were conducted separately for the two rearing groups (mother reared and nursery reared). Pairwise correlations were calculated across all six time points, resulting in 15 different correlations for each rearing group.

Results

Locally Weighted Scatterplot Smoothing Analysis

The locally weighted scatterplot smoothing fits for CSF 5-HIAA concentrations in mother-reared and nursery-reared infants during the first 5 months of life are presented in Figure 1 and Figure 2, respectively. CSF 5-HIAA concentrations declined throughout this period of early development in both rearing groups. Furthermore, nursery-reared infants consistently displayed lower CSF 5-HIAA concentrations than mother-reared infants (jackknife comparison of locally weighted scatterplot smoothing fits, Z=7.664, p<0.0001). The locally weighted scatterplot smoothing results do suggest that the overall difference between rearing groups is not necessarily consistent across the full length of early development, and that the mother-reared animals become more similar to the nursery-reared animals toward the end of 5 months. However, further analysis of the data indicate significant differences between these two groups at each of the time points (day 14: t=6.02, df=254, p<0.0001; day 30: t=4.30, df=254, p<0.0001; day 60: t=3.62, df=254, p=0.0004; day 90: t=2.97, df=254, p<0.004; day 120: t=2.41, df=254, p<0.02; day 150: t=2.40, df=254, p<0.02).

Correlation Analyses

Correlation analyses showed consistent individual stability across each of the measurements in both mother-reared (Table 1) and nursery-reared (Table 2) monkeys. In each rearing group, all 15 of the CSF 5-HIAA correlations were positive and statistically significant (p<0.0001), with similar average correlation coefficients seen for mother-reared infants (r=0.51) and nursery-reared infants (r=0.50).

Discussion

Rearing Condition and Early Development

Our results show that rearing condition can affect the early development of the serotonin system in rhesus monkey infants. As early as day 14 of life, nursery-reared infants exhibited lower CSF 5-HIAA concentrations than the mother-reared infants, and they continued to show lower concentrations over the next 5 months. Although some previous studies have failed to find differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations between mother-reared and nursery-reared monkeys (7, 14), the present results indicate that in a large sample of monkeys, nursery-reared infants exhibit lower CSF 5-HIAA concentrations than mother-reared infants when the mother-reared infants were reared in conditions that more closely approximate the species-typical social environment.
Our results also indicate that the quantity of peer interactions in nursery-reared infants did not affect CSF 5-HIAA concentrations. Whether the infants were reared with limited peer contact (surrogate/peer reared) or constant access to other age-mates (peer only reared), their CSF 5-HIAA concentrations were lower than in the mother-reared infants. This, coupled with the lack of significant differences in CSF 5-HIAA between surrogate/peer-reared and peer only-reared monkeys, suggests that maternal deprivation has more of an influence on the developing serotonin system than general social deprivation.
In the nursery environment, the absence of a mother results in a number of important factors being absent or diminished. In the earliest stages of life, these include vestibular stimulation/postural changes provided by mothers’ locomotion, tactile stimulation from handling and grooming, and nutritional provisions from mothers’ breast milk. In a separate study, we found that human handling of nursery-reared infants before day 14 did not result in elevated CSF 5-HIAA concentrations, suggesting that vestibular and tactile stimulation by individuals other than the mother is not necessarily an effective substitute for “mother.” Likewise, commercially available infant formula is not necessarily an effective substitute for a mother’s breast milk, since it is lower in total fatty acid and α-lactalbumin concentrations compared with rhesus monkey and human breast milk (2427). Long-chain essential fatty acids have been linked to healthy development of the brain and central nervous system in human and nonhuman primate infants (26, 28–34) and may also predict levels of serotonin and dopamine metabolites (35). The protein α-lactalbumin is rich in tryptophan, a major precursor of CNS 5-HT, and therefore is directly linked to the development of the serotonin system (27). As nursery-reared infants grow older, there is also a lack of appropriate social feedback. Mothers provide discipline, serve as social referents in situations of uncertainty, and act as models for the development of appropriate social behaviors. It is unlikely that nursery-reared infants receive the same benefits from their interaction with same-aged peers.
Physical differences between the nursery- and mother-rearing environment, such as light cycles, cage size, and physical and social complexity of the environment, may also play a role in early developmental differences between mother-reared and nursery-reared monkeys. However, the fact that rearing condition differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations were found as early as day 14 suggests a limited role, if any. During the first 2 weeks of life, infants are very rarely found off their mothers’ ventrums, and as such their experience of the wider environment is limited. This suggests that the differences in serotonin function observed in nursery-reared and mother-reared infants at such an early age are more likely being driven by factors related specifically to the mother and the microenvironment she provides. These mother-specific factors likely contribute to the continuing differences observed between mother-reared and nursery-reared monkeys over the next months of life.
The developmental decline in CSF 5-HIAA levels that we observed during the first 5 months in both nursery-reared and mother-reared infants is consistent with other studies of nonhuman primates as well as with studies of human children of comparable developmental ages (3, 6, 7, 36–40). However, while these studies report declines in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations from infancy into late adolescence or young adulthood, the current study shows that concentrations actually begin to decline as early as day 30. To our knowledge, this is the first comprehensive report of a developmental decline in the early infancy period of either humans or nonhuman primates.

Stability of Individual Differences

Despite developmental declines in absolute 5-HIAA concentrations, individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA exhibited trait-like stability across repeated measurements, as exemplified by high positive correlation coefficients across repeated measurements for both mother-reared and nursery-reared infants. These results are consistent with previous findings concerning juvenile and adult nonhuman primates (310). Such stability of individual differences may be present in humans as well, although results have been conflicting (36, 41).
As stated earlier, CSF samples were collected under both moderate challenge conditions (days 14, 30, 90, 120, and 150) and a “baseline” condition (day 60). The moderate challenge conditions are known activators of the HPA axis in all rearing groups (42). Because the monoamines are stress-sensitive, it is possible that the 5-HIAA measures taken under the moderate challenge conditions reflected increased CNS serotonin activity. However, we were unable to test this possibility. First, due to the necessity of anesthetizing animals for sample collection, it was impossible to collect both baseline and postchallenge samples on the same day, which would have allowed us to directly compare these values. Second, due to the developmental decline in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations, a direct comparison of the day 60 baseline samples and the postchallenge samples obtained on the other days would have been inappropriate. We do note that across both baseline (day 60) and challenge conditions (days 14, 30, 90, 120, and 150), the developmental decline is still evident, and individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations remained relatively stable, despite the differences in sampling technique.

Serotonin, CNS Development, and Psychopathology

CSF 5-HIAA is thought to measure CNS serotonin functioning by measuring aggregate CNS serotonin turnover (1). While specific neuronal responses cannot be pinpointed using this method, a number of studies have found that CSF 5-HIAA concentrations are correlated with serotonin turnover in the frontal cortex (1, 43–45). Relative to the entire lifetime of the rhesus monkey, major developmental processes are taking place in the cortical areas of the brain, including the frontal cortex (46), during the first 6 months of life. Psychological processes such as emotional self-regulation and impulse control first emerge during this same period, in parallel with CNS development. From an evolutionary perspective, it is clear that for normative development of the CNS system to occur, environmental input is of critical importance, especially during such sensitive periods (47). As such, evolution has selected for mothers to provide emotional security and to punish inappropriate social impulses, perhaps providing the input needed to properly link the serotonin system to the frontal cortex region, which is of critical importance in both emotional regulation and impulse control. While speculative, it is possible that with the absence of a mother to provide appropriate input, offspring may be at risk for serotonin-mediated affective disorders and impulse control deficits.
Our results have important ramifications for understanding the repetitive nature of psychopathology and relapse. Temperament and personality are widely recognized as contributing to a predictable style of social interaction as well as to axis II psychopathology. To the degree that psychopathology is influenced by the serotonin system, our findings suggest that the risk for psychopathology and serotonin-mediated axis II personality disorders may be exacerbated by experiences early in life that impair the function of the central serotonin system.
In conclusion, this study demonstrates that individual differences in CSF 5-HIAA concentrations are stable and trait-like across the neonatal period in rhesus macaques. These results suggest that low CSF 5-HIAA concentrations are an enduring, stable biological marker that may have roots in early infancy. Furthermore, our findings show that early nursery rearing reduces CNS serotonin turnover, possibly increasing the risk for serotonin-mediated psychopathologies.
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
Figure 1. CSF 5-HIAA Concentrations Plotted Against Age in Mother-Reared Rhesus Monkey Infants (N=102)
Figure 2. CSF 5-HIAA Concentrations Plotted Against Age in Nursery-Reared Rhesus Monkey Infants (N=154)

Footnote

Presented in part at the 18th annual meeting of the American Society of Primatologists, Scottsdale, Ariz., June 21–24, 1995. Received Dec. 5, 2003; revision received May 19, 2004; accepted Sept. 27, 2004. From the Laboratory of Comparative Ethology and the Laboratory of Clinical Studies, NIH Animal Center, Poolesville, Md. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Higley, Research Psychologist, NIH Animal Center, Building 112, P.O. Box 529, Poolesville, MD 20837-0529; [email protected] (e-mail). Dr. Linnoila died in February 1998. The authors wish to thank Alan Dodson, Ted King, Stephen Lindell, Erick Singley, and Kristin Zajicek for their assistance in data collection, assays, and manuscript preparation. Dr. Kenneth Kirk, Laboratory of Bio-Organic Chemistry, National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, Bethesda, Md., provided the high performance liquid chromatography internal standards. Dr. Dennis Young at Arizona State University provided SAS macros used in the data analysis. Animal research protocols were approved, in accordance with and as required by the Animal Welfare Act, by the Animal Care and Use Committees of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (ASP 96-014) and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (ASP LCS-075).

References

1.
Stanley M, Traskman-Bendz L, Dorovini-Zis K: Correlations between aminergic metabolites simultaneously obtained from human CSF and brain. Life Sci 1985; 37:1279–1286
2.
Murphy DL: Neuropsychiatric disorders and the multiple human brain serotonin receptor subtypes and subsystems. Neuropsychopharmacology 1990; 3:457–471
3.
Higley JD, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: A longitudinal assessment of CSF monoamine metabolite and plasma cortisol concentrations in young rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry 1992; 32:127–145
4.
Higley JD, Thompson WT, Champoux M, Goldman D, Hasert MF, Kraemer GW, Scanlan JM, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: Paternal and maternal genetic and environmental contributions to cerebrospinal fluid monoamine metabolites in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:615–623
5.
Higley JD, King ST, Hasert MF, Champoux M, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: Stability of interindividual differences in serotonin function and its relationship to aggressive wounding and competent social behavior in rhesus macaque females. Neuropsychopharmacology 1996; 14:67–76
6.
Higley JD, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: A nonhuman primate model of type II excessive alcohol consumption? part 1: low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentrations and diminished social competence correlate with excessive alcohol consumption. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1996; 20:629–642
7.
Kraemer GW, Ebert MH, Schmidt DE, McKinney WT: A longitudinal study of the effect of different social rearing conditions on cerebrospinal fluid norepinephrine and biogenic amine metabolites in rhesus monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology 1989; 2:175–189
8.
Mehlman PT, Higley JD, Faucher I, Lilly AA, Taub DM, Vickers J, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: Correlation of CSF 5-HIAA concentration with sociality and the timing of emigration in free-ranging primates. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:907–913
9.
Raleigh MJ, Brammer GL, McGuire MT, Pollack DB, Yuwiler A: Individual differences in basal cisternal cerebrospinal fluid 5-HIAA and HVA in monkeys: the effects of gender, age, physical characteristics, and matrilineal influences. Neuropsychopharmacology 1992; 7:295–304
10.
Raleigh MJ, McGuire MT: Serotonin, aggression, and violence in vervet monkeys, in The Neurotransmitter Revolution. Edited by Masters RD, McGuire MT. Carbondale, Southern Illinois University Press, 1994, pp 129–145
11.
Higley JD, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: A nonhuman primate model of type II alcoholism? part 2: diminished social competence and excessive aggression correlates with low cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid concentrations. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1996; 20:643–650
12.
Kraemer GW, Ebert MH, Lake CR, McKinney WT: Cerebrospinal fluid measures of neurotransmitter changes associated with pharmacological alteration of the despair response to social separation in rhesus monkeys. Psychiatry Res 1984; 11:303–315
13.
Kraemer GW, Ebert MH, Schmidt DE, McKinney WT: Strangers in a strange land: a psychobiological study of infant monkeys before and after separation from real or inanimate mothers. Child Dev 1991; 62:548–566
14.
Clarke AS, Hedeker DR, Ebert MH, Schmidt DE, McKinney WT, Kraemer GW: Rearing experience and biogenic amine activity in infant rhesus monkeys. Biol Psychiatry 1996; 40:338–352
15.
Kondo K, Minami T, Itoigawa N: Behavioral differences between feral group-reared and mother-reared young Japanese monkeys, in Primate Behavior and Sociobiology. Edited by Chiarelli AB, Corruccini RS. New York, Springer-Verlag, 1981, pp 64–71
16.
Ruppenthal GC: Survey of protocols for nursery rearing infant macaques, in Nursery Care of Nonhuman Primates. Edited by Ruppenthal GC. New York, Plenum, 1979, pp 165–185
17.
Schneider M, Suomi SJ: Laboratory assessment of temperament and postrotary nystagmus responses in rhesus monkey infants (Macaca mulatta). Phys Occup Ther Pediatr 1992; 12:37–52
18.
Scheinin M, Chang WH, Kirk KL, Linnoila M: Simultaneous determination of 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol, 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, and homovanillic acid in cerebrospinal fluid with high-performance liquid chromatography using electrochemical detection. Anal Biochem 1983; 131:246–253
19.
Seppala T, Scheinin M, Capone A, Linnoila M: Liquid chromatographic assay for CSF catecholamines using electrochemical detection. Acta Pharmacol Toxicol 1984; 55:81–87
20.
Cleveland WS: Robust locally weighted regression and smoothing scatterplots. J Am Stat Assoc 1979; 74:829–836
21.
Cleveland WS, Devlin SJ: Locally weighted regression: an approach to regression analysis by local fitting. J Am Stat Assoc 1988; 83:596–610
22.
Friendly M: SAS System for Statistical Graphics. Cary, NC, SAS Institute, 1991
23.
Moses LE, Gale LC, Altmann J: Methods for analysis of unbalanced, longitudinal growth data. Am J Primatol 1992; 28:49–59
24.
Larque E, Demmelmair H, Koletzko B: Perinatal supply and metabolism of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids. Ann NY Acad Sci 2002; 967:299–310
25.
Hibbeln JR, Linnoila M, Umhau JC, Rawlings R, George DT, Salem N: Essential fatty acids predict metabolites of serotonin and dopamine in cerebrospinal fluid among healthy control subjects, and early- and late-onset alcoholics. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:235–242
26.
Champoux M, Hibbeln JR, Shannon C, Majchrzak S, Suomi SJ, Salem N, Higley JD: Fatty acid formula supplementation and neuromotor development in rhesus monkey neonates. Pediatr Res 2002; 51:273–281
27.
Heine WE: The significance of tryptophan in infant nutrition. Adv Exp Med Biol 1999; 467:705–710
28.
Neuringer M, Connor WE, Van Petten C, Barstad L: Dietary omega-3 fatty acid deficiency and visual loss in infant rhesus monkeys. J Clin Invest 1984; 73:272–276
29.
Neuringer M, Connor WE, Lin DS, Barstad L, Luck S: Biochemical and functional effects of prenatal and postnatal omega 3 fatty acid deficiency on retina and brain in rhesus monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1986; 83:4021–4025
30.
Nettleton J: Are n-3 fatty acids essential nutrients for fetal and infant development? J Am Diet Assoc 1993; 93:58–64
31.
Uauy R, De Andraca I: Human milk and breast feeding for optimal mental development. J Nutr 1995; 125:2278S-2280S
32.
Koletzko B, Rodriguez-Palmero M: Polyunsaturated fatty acids in human milk and their role in early infant development. J Mammary Gland Biol Neoplasia 1999; 4:269–284
33.
Xiang M, Alfvén G, Blennow M, Trygg M, Zetterström R: Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in human milk and brain growth during early infancy. Acta Paediatr 2000; 89:142–147
34.
Lönnerdal B, Keen CL, Glazier CE, Anderson J: A longitudinal study of rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) milk composition: trace elements, minerals, protein, carbohydrate, and fat. Pediatr Res 1984; 18:911–914
35.
Kunz C, Lönnerdal B: Protein composition of rhesus monkey milk: comparison to human milk. Comp Biochem Physiol 1993; 104:793–797
36.
Riddle MA, Anderson GM, McIntosh S, Harcherik DF, Shaywitz BA, Cohen DJ: Cerebrospinal fluid monoamine precursor and metabolite levels in children treated for leukemia: age and sex effects and individual variability. Biol Psychiatry 1986; 21:69–83
37.
Shelton SE, Kalin NH, Gluck JP, Keresztury MF, Schneider VA, Lewis MH: Effects of age on cisternal cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of monoamine metabolites in nonhuman primates. Neurochem Int 1988; 13:353–357
38.
Andersson H, Roos BE: 5-Hydroxyindoleacetic acid in cerebrospinal fluid of hydrocephalic children. Acta Paediatr Scand 1969; 58:601–608
39.
Cohen DJ, Caparulo BK, Shaywitz BA, Bowers MBJ: Dopamine and serotonin metabolism in neuropsychiatrically disturbed children: CSF homovanillic acid and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1977; 34:545–550
40.
Langlais PJ, Walsh FX, Bird ED, Levy HL: Cerebrospinal fluid neurotransmitter metabolites in neurologically normal infants and children. Pediatrics 1985; 75:580–586
41.
Linnoila M, Ninan PT, Scheinin M, Waters RN, Chang WH, Bartko J, van Kammen DP: Reliability of norepinephrine and major monoamine metabolite measurements in CSF of schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:1290–1294
42.
Shannon C, Champoux M, Suomi SJ: Rearing condition and plasma cortisol in rhesus monkey infants. Am J Primatol 1998; 46:311–321
43.
Knott P, Haroutunian V, Bierer L, Perl D, Handler M, DeLeon M, Yang R, Davis K: Correlations post-mortem between ventricular CSF and cortical tissue concentrations of MHPG, 5-HIAA, and HVA in Alzheimer’s disease (abstract). Biol Psychiatry 1989; 25:112
44.
Wester P, Bergstrom U, Eriksson A, Gezelius C, Hardy J, Winblad B: Ventricular cerebrospinal fluid monoamine transmitter and metabolite concentrations reflect human brain neurochemistry in autopsy cases. J Neurochemistry 1990; 54:1148–1156
45.
Doudet D, Hommer D, Higley JD, Andreason PJ, Moneman R, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M: Cerebral glucose metabolism, CSF 5-HIAA levels, and aggressive behavior in rhesus monkeys. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1782–1787
46.
Gibson KR: Myelination and behavioral development: a comparative perspective on questions of neoteny, altriciality and intelligence, in Brain Maturation and Cognitive Development. Edited by Gibson KR, Peterson AC. New York, DeGruyter, 1991, pp 29–63
47.
Greenough WT: Experience and brain development. Child Dev 1987; 58:539–559

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1658 - 1664
PubMed: 16135625

History

Published online: 1 September 2005
Published in print: September 2005

Authors

Details

Courtney Shannon, B.S.
Melanie L. Schwandt, Ph.D.
Maribeth Champoux, Ph.D.
Stephen J. Suomi, Ph.D.
Markku Linnoila, Ph.D.
James D. Higley, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share