Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective

Dysfunction of the hippocampus has long been suspected to be a key component of the pathophysiology of major depressive disorder. Despite evidence of hippocampal structural abnormalities in depressed patients, abnormal hippocampal functioning has not been demonstrated. The authors aimed to link spatial navigation deficits previously documented in depressed patients to abnormal hippocampal functioning using a virtual reality navigation task.

Method

Whole-head magnetoencephalography (MEG) recordings were collected while participants (19 patients diagnosed with major depressive disorder and 19 healthy subjects matched by gender and age) navigated a virtual Morris water maze to find a hidden platform; navigation to a visible platform served as a control condition. Behavioral measures were obtained to assess navigation performance. Theta oscillatory activity (4–8 Hz) was mapped across the brain on a voxel-wise basis using a spatial-filtering MEG source analysis technique.

Results

Depressed patients performed worse than healthy subjects in navigating to the hidden platform. Robust group differences in theta activity were observed in right medial temporal cortices during navigation, with patients exhibiting less engagement of the anterior hippocampus and parahippocampal cortices relative to comparison subjects. Left posterior hippocampal theta activity was positively correlated with individual performance within each group.

Conclusions

Consistent with previous findings, depressed patients showed impaired spatial navigation. Dysfunction of right anterior hippocampus and parahippocampal cortices may underlie this deficit and stem from structural abnormalities commonly found in depressed patients.
A pivotal role for the hippocampus in the pathophysiology of major depressive disorder is supported by abundant evidence (1, 2). Numerous studies, for instance, have reported hippocampal structural abnormalities in depressed patients; meta-analyses support the reliability of these observations (3, 4). Chronic stress and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis hyperactivity—commonly seen in depressed patients (5), and which cause hippocampal atrophy in animals and increased vulnerability of neurons to various insults such as glutamatergic excitotoxicity (6)—may cause these structural abnormalities. Given widespread projections of the hippocampus to prefrontal cortices and to the amygdala and striatum, hippocampal dysfunction might underlie the array of cognitive, affective, and reward-related changes associated with major depressive disorder.
Abnormal hippocampal functioning in depressed patients has been inferred from structural abnormalities, resting-state metabolic abnormalities (7), and neuropsychological tests (8) but has yet to be demonstrated in a hippocampus-dependent task. Recently, Gould and coworkers (9), using a virtual reality paradigm, found patients with major depressive disorder and patients with bipolar disorder to be impaired at spatial navigation. These results are noteworthy in that functional imaging research with virtual environments has shown that spatial navigation activates hippocampal and parahippocampal regions in the healthy human brain (10). Invasive electrophysiological studies in epileptic patients have also established that neuronal populations in the hippocampus and neocortex show rhythmic electrical activity during virtual navigation (11, 12). Rhythmic activity of about 4–8 cycles/second (Hz)—referred to as theta—is prominent in the animal hippocampus during exploration and may directly modulate synaptic plasticity for spatial learning (13, 14). Functional or structural brain measurements were not taken by Gould et al. (9), however, and thus a direct link between hippocampus functioning and spatial deficits in depressed patients remains to be demonstrated.
We used a virtual reality analogue of the Morris water maze task to establish this link. The goal of this task, like the actual version administered to rodents that is sensitive to hippocampal dysfunction (15, 16), is to escape from a pool of water by reaching a hidden, submerged platform. The fixed location of the hidden platform can be learned by using distal cues on the surrounding walls to guide navigation from various starting positions. Performance on the virtual water maze task is sensitive to the structural integrity of the hippocampus (17), and hippocampal and parahippocampal regions are active in healthy individuals (18, 19). Neuromagnetic data were collected noninvasively with whole-head magnetoencephalography (MEG). Theta oscillatory activity was mapped across the brain using a source analysis technique called synthetic aperture magnetometry (20). In a previous study using the same task, healthy participants showed robust anterior hippocampal theta activity during navigation to the hidden platform relative to performing aimless movements in the virtual pool; posterior hippocampal theta responses were positively correlated with navigation performance (18). On the basis of these findings, we hypothesized that together with impaired navigation to the hidden platform, depressed patients would exhibit reduced anterior hippocampal and parahippocampal theta activity relative to healthy subjects. We also hypothesized that posterior hippocampal theta would positively correlate with navigation performance at the individual level within each group.

Method

Participants

Depressed patients were recruited from inpatient and outpatient psychiatric units at the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) as part of randomized clinical medication trials for unipolar depression. The patient group consisted of 19 right-handed individuals with a diagnosis of major depressive disorder, currently depressed without psychotic features; diagnosis was confirmed with the Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders–Patient Version (21). Additional clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1. Patients with a DSM-IV (22) diagnosis of drug or alcohol dependence or abuse within the past 3 months, history of head injury, unstable medical illness, or uncorrected hypo- or hyperthyroidism were excluded. All subjects had been free of psychotropic drugs for at least 2 weeks (5 weeks for fluoxetine) and were medically healthy as determined by medical history, physical and neurological examinations, and laboratory tests. Most patients (89%) had in their lifetime received trials of medications, the most common being selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) (79%), venlafaxine (42%), duloxetine (37%), and bupropion (37%). During the current major depressive episode the most common trials were SSRIs (63%), duloxetine (26%), and bupropion (26%). No patient had received electroconvulsive therapy. The severity of their current depressive episodes was assessed by using the Montgomery-Åsberg Depression Rating Scale (23). Nine of the 19 patients had a comorbid anxiety disorder, none of which included posttraumatic stress disorder.
Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the Two Groups That Underwent Magnetoencephalography While Performing a Virtual Navigational Taska
Nineteen right-handed healthy comparison subjects were recruited from the local community. Healthy subjects underwent screening at NIMH that included a medical history, physical examination performed by a physician, and a Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (24). Exclusion criteria were current medical illness, axis I DSM-IV psychiatric disorder, or first-degree relative with a history of psychiatric disorders; family history was assessed per self-report during the interview with the clinician. No subject could be currently taking psychotropic medications per self-report. The study was approved by the Combined Neuroscience Institutional Review Board of NIH. After complete description of the study to participants, written informed consent was obtained.

Virtual Morris Water Maze

Participants completed 40 trials in which they moved with a fiber-optic joystick around a virtual pool using commercial software (NeuroInvestigations, Inc., Lethbridge, Alb., Canada). Participants began each trial at the pool's edge in one of four locations (pseudorandomly ordered). For half of the trials, participants were instructed to quickly navigate to a hidden platform in a fixed location (hidden condition). If the hidden platform was not found within 20 seconds, it became visible and participants were instructed to navigate to it to finish the trial. This condition requires the use of distal cues on the surrounding walls to learn the fixed location of the hidden platform. For the other half of the trials, participants were instructed to navigate to a visible platform in a variable location (visible condition). As the control condition, navigating to the visible platform does not require use of distal cues. The platform had a variable location across visible trials to limit spatial learning during this condition. Different pool environments were used for the two conditions with the only difference being the distal cues (e.g., door and shelves in one environment versus abstract patterns in the other); each virtual environment was assigned to contain the hidden or visible platform in equal proportions between groups. There were 2-second breaks between trials. The order of conditions was counterbalanced across participants.
Latency to reach the platform from first movement, latency to first movement, path length, and heading error or angular deviation from a direct path to the platform were recorded for each hidden and visible platform trial. Heading error is a point estimate calculated when the path length exceeds one fourth of the pool's diameter. Each of these measures was collapsed into five blocks of four trials. All participants received exposure to the task in a previous session before the MEG session to ensure ample practice. Platform locations were different between sessions.

Data Acquisition

Neuromagnetic data were recorded with a CTF-OMEGA 275-channel whole-head magnetometer (VSM MedTech, Coquitlam, B.C., Canada) in a magnetically shielded room (Vacuumschmelze GmbH, Hanau, Germany) using synthetic third gradient balancing for active noise cancellation. Magnetic flux density was sampled at 1200 Hz with a bandwidth of 0–300 Hz. Electrical coils attached at three fiducial sites—nasion and right and left preauricular sites—were energized during each MEG recording to track head movements in real time and for offline registration to anatomical MRIs. Radiological markers (IZI Medical Products, Baltimore, Md.) were attached to the same fiducial sites during the MRI scan. High-resolution T1-weighted anatomical MRIs were obtained in a separate session using a 1.5 or 3 Tesla whole-body scanner (GE Signa, Waukesha, Wisc.).

Synthetic Aperture Magnetometry

Synthetic aperture magnetometry is a minimum-variance beamformer algorithm that uses the signal covariance from the sensor array for constructing optimum spatial filters at each location in the brain; an optimum spatial filter suppresses all source activity except for that which originates from the location of interest (25). Regional oscillatory changes estimated by this method are highly consistent with fMRI results (26), at cortical and subcortical loci (27), and in healthy individuals and depressed patients (28). Source imaging procedures were identical to those used by Cornwell et al. (18). Signal covariance was calculated between all sensors with data filtered between 4 and 8 Hz for 20 unaveraged 1-second epochs during navigation for each condition. Source space was sampled in 5-mm steps with current sources estimated at each location based on a multisphere model derived from the MRI of each participant. Dual-state images were calculated by dividing integrated power over 1-second windows during navigation by integrated power over 1-second pretrial windows (pseudo-F ratios), yielding estimates of relative increases (or decreases) in theta activity during navigation in each condition. With a constant pretrial window, relative theta power was estimated across 17 1-second windows during navigation with 75% overlap (0.00–1.00 second, 0.25–1.25, …, 4.00–5.00), encompassing the first 5 seconds of each trial before the platform had been reached.
Using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages software (29), individual source image volumes for each condition and time window were manually warped to a Talairach brain template to allow for group analysis in a standardized source space and within-volume normalized using a z-score transformation. The latter procedure was performed to limit the influence of global power that may vary across participants, or systematically between groups, on local power estimates in a particular brain region.

Performance Analyses

Navigation performance was assessed as follows: 1) latency to reach the platform from the first recorded movement, 2) path length to the platform, and 3) heading error to the platform. Latency to first movement was also analyzed as a control measure. Gender and age were added as nuisance factors in our analyses given evidence of their associations with spatial navigation performance (30, 31). We performed 2×2×2×5 mixed factorial ANCOVAs, with age as a continuous covariate, group and gender as between-group factors, and condition (hidden versus visible platform) and block (1 to 5) as repeated-measures variables. Significant interactions between group and condition were followed up with simple effect analyses for each condition. For cases in which differences emerged between patients and healthy subjects, post hoc analyses were run to determine whether there was evidence of differences within the patient group between those with and those without comorbid anxiety disorders. Two patients' path length and heading error data were lost to computer malfunction.

Functional Imaging Analyses

Group SAM analyses of 4–8 Hz theta power consisted of 2 (patients versus comparison subjects) × 2 (hidden versus visible) mixed factorial ANOVAs. We confined our subsequent simple effect analyses to functional regions of interest showing significant 2-way interactions at a nominal p value of <0.05. Within these regions, we compared theta activity between groups for the hidden condition and the visible condition using Student's t tests and estimated false discovery rates (32). A single correction for multiple comparisons was performed that incorporated all voxels within these regions and all time windows, such that we kept the overall false discovery rate of the source imaging results below 5%. As with the behavioral data, post hoc analyses were run that compared depressed patients with and those without comorbid anxiety disorders specifically within clusters in medial temporal cortices showing differential activation between groups.
We determined what regional activity correlated at the individual level with navigation performance on hidden trials. Spearman correlations were calculated at each voxel for each time window for each group separately. A nonparametric approach was deemed most appropriate given the small samples. We identified only those regions with at least two contiguous voxels showing negative correlations, commonly in both groups but irrespective of the time window, surviving a statistical threshold of p<0.005. Given previous results in left posterior hippocampal and parahippocampal cortices (18), we considered negative correlations only insofar as they reflect higher regional theta activity being associated with better navigation performance (e.g., shorter mean latencies).

Results

Group Differences in Navigation Performance

There were significant group-by-condition interactions on mean latency to reach the platform (F=6.89, df=1, 33, p<0.05) and path length (F=6.06, df=1, 31, p<0.05) (Figure 1) but not heading error nor latency to first movement. For the hidden platform condition, patients were slower than comparison subjects to reach the platform (F=10.77, df=1, 33, p<0.005) and they also took longer paths relative to comparison subjects (F=13.28, df=1, 31, p<0.005). For the visible platform condition, there was no difference between patients and healthy subjects in mean latency, but patients did take longer paths to the platform relative to comparison subjects (F=15.76, df=1, 31, p<0.001). For mean heading error, there was a significant group difference (F=8.86, df=1, 31, p<0.01), with patients performing worse than comparison subjects in both conditions. There was also a significant group difference in latency to first movement (F=4.70, df=1, 33, p<0.05), with patients initiating movements faster than comparison subjects in both conditions. Post hoc comparisons between patients with and without comorbid anxiety disorders in the hidden platform condition revealed no differences in latency or path length. Regarding gender differences, men showed shorter path lengths and smaller heading errors than women (F=16.52, df=1, 31, p<0.001 and F=7.69, df=1, 31, p<0.01, respectively). There was a difference between men and women in latency to reach the platform that approached significance (F=3.44, df=1, 33, p<0.10) but not in latency to first movement.
Figure 1. Virtual Spatial Navigation Task Performance of Depressed Patients and Healthy Subjects by Condition
*p<0.005.

Group Differences in Navigation-Related Theta Activity

Table 2 presents regions that showed significant group differences in theta activity for the hidden and visible platform conditions. Healthy subjects exhibited greater theta activity than depressed patients in medial temporal cortices in five time windows during the hidden condition, all in the right anterior region (Figure 2). Peak activity differences varied between the hippocampus at 1.25–2.25 seconds and 3.25–4.25 seconds relative to trial onset, and anterior parahippocampal cortices at 0.75–1.75, 2.75–3.75, and 3.00–4.00 seconds. In all cases, group differences reflected a combination of greater theta activity in healthy subjects and lesser activity in patients relative to pretrial baseline activity. There were no instances in which patients exhibited greater theta activity than comparison subjects in medial temporal cortices during the hidden condition.
Figure 2. Peak Differences in Regional Theta (4–8 Hz) Activity in Right Medial Temporal Cortices Between Healthy Subjects (N=19) and Depressed Patients (N=19) During a Virtual Spatial Navigation Task
aCoronal and sagittal views of differential activations are overlaid on an averaged anatomical MRI, presented in radiological orientation (left=right, right=left). Bar graphs show mean theta power by group and condition at the local maximum for each time window depicted in the images (*: significant between-group difference, false discovery rate <5%).
Table 2. Regional Theta Activity Showing Significant Group Differences (Healthy Subjects Minus Depressed Patients) During Navigation on Hidden and Visible Platform Trials
Other regions showing greater theta activity in healthy subjects relative to patients included multiple loci in lateral prefrontal cortices. Conversely, greater theta activity in the cerebellum and posterior cortical regions was observed in patients relative to comparison subjects. Post hoc analyses found no significant differences in theta activity between patients with and without comorbid anxiety disorders within regions showing differences between depressed and healthy subjects when corrected for multiple comparisons. Finally, few regions exhibited group differences in activity for the visible condition. Healthy subjects exhibited greater activity in dorsal prefrontal cortical regions and, in one instance, left parahippocampal cortices relative to patients. Patients exhibited greater theta activity than comparison subjects in the cerebellum and posterior parietal cortices.

Regional Theta and Navigation Performance Correlations

Correlation analyses demonstrated that left posterior hippocampal/parahippocampal theta activity was negatively correlated with mean latency to reach the hidden platform (i.e., greater theta was associated with better performance) in both patients and comparison subjects (Figure 3). Time windows in which these correlations were observed differed between groups, with patients showing this correlation at 3.00–4.00 seconds and healthy subjects at 1.25–2.25 seconds relative to trial onset. Other regions with at least two contiguous voxels showing negative correlations of a similar magnitude commonly across groups in any time window included the following: left precuneus (Brodmann's area [BA] 7; local maxima [x,y,z]= –4, –49, 42), right precentral gyrus (BA 4; x,y,z=21, –23, 59), right precuneus (BA 7; x,y,z=3, –37, 45) and right middle frontal gyrus (BA 10; x,y,z=36, 43, 22).
Figure 3. Correlations Between Regional Theta Activity and Navigation Performance in Healthy Subjects and Depressed Patients
aCoronal images are in radiological orientation (left=right, right=left). Scatterplots, with least squares lines, depict inverse relationships between regional theta activity in left posterior hippocampus at 1.25–2.25 seconds in healthy subjects during navigation (rs= –0.66, p<0.005) and at 3.00–4.00 seconds in depressed patients (rs= –0.81, p<0.005) and mean latency to reach the hidden platform across 20 trials (i.e., greater theta activity associated with better performance).

Discussion

We investigated neural correlates of spatial navigation deficits in depressed patients (9) using a virtual analogue of the Morris water maze task. Whole-head MEG recordings were collected as depressed patients and healthy subjects navigated the water maze. For both mean latency and path length to the hidden platform (Figure 1), patients showed impaired navigation performance relative to comparison subjects, replicating results obtained from depressed patients performing a different virtual navigation task (9). Source analyses revealed group differences in regional theta (4–8 Hz) activity during navigation, including reduced oscillatory activity in right anterior hippocampus and parahippocampal cortices in patients relative to healthy subjects. These findings provide evidence of abnormal hippocampal and parahippocampal cortical functioning in major depressive disorder, complementing evidence for structural abnormalities in this population.
Our data point to spatial navigation deficits in depressed patients being the result of dysfunction of right anterior medial temporal structures (Figure 2). A recent meta-analysis of morphometric studies found evidence of bilateral volumetric reductions of the hippocampus in depressed patients (4), suggesting the absence of lateralized changes. However, both functional imaging (7) and postmortem (33) evidence point to abnormalities in the right hippocampus only as seen in the present results. Theta activity in this right anterior region, however, did not correlate with navigation performance at the individual level within groups. Instead, left posterior hippocampal theta activity was positively correlated with performance within both groups (Figure 3), replicating previous findings (18). Posterior regions are widely believed to be especially critical to spatial navigation in animals (34) and humans (35), and the within-group correlations observed here are consistent with this view. Anterior regions have also been implicated in spatial navigation and seem to play a role limited to early encoding of novel environments, showing decreased activity as navigation performance improves (18, 35). Anterior hippocampal and parahippocampal dysfunction in patients indicates they fail to initially encode the spatial layout in a way that optimizes navigation to the hidden platform.
Reduced activity of lateral prefrontal cortices in patients (Table 2), which may be indicative of spatial working memory dysfunction (36), supports evidence of generalized deficits in visuospatial processing and working memory in depression (37, 38). Moreover, patients performed worse than healthy subjects on visible platform trials in addition to hidden platform trials, at least in terms of path length to the visible platform, implying that inferior performance in depressed patients might be partly explained by basic sensorimotor difficulties. Potential sensorimotor coordination deficits, however, cannot account for greater group differences in navigating to the hidden platform compared with group differences in navigating to the visible platform as the sensorimotor demands were the same in both conditions. Psychomotor retardation and diminished motivation associated with depression might be other factors to consider; however, their impact seems negligible as patients were just as fast as healthy subjects in reaching the visible platform, and faster in initiating movement in both conditions, which would not be expected if patients exhibited significant psychomotor retardation or lacked motivation to perform the task. In total, inferior performance of depressed patients in navigating to the hidden platform can be attributed primarily to spatial learning deficits.
The significance of our findings linking hippocampal and parahippocampal dysfunction to major depressive disorder must be tempered by the following considerations. First, 47% (9/19) of our patients had a current anxiety disorder. Because anxious depression is associated with worse outcome and more severe impairment than nonanxious depression (39), concerns arise about whether our evidence of hippocampal and parahippocampal dysfunction applies only to these more severely ill patients or to the presence of clinical anxiety symptoms. Within the patient group, there was no evidence that the degree of behavioral impairment or level of hippocampal and parahippocampal activity differed between those with and those without an anxiety disorder. It is unlikely, therefore, that differences between the patient and healthy groups were driven predominantly by patients with anxious depression. Anxiety may, nevertheless, be a mediating factor for which future research may need to consider with a larger sample of anxious and nonanxious major depressive disorder patients (40). Finally, because our patients were currently depressed with moderate-to-severe symptoms, we are unable to determine whether hippocampal and parahippocampal dysfunction is state-dependent or also present in those at risk for depression.
With mounting evidence of hippocampal structural abnormalities in major depressive disorder, we observed clear reductions of right anterior hippocampal and parahippocampal oscillatory activity in depressed individuals during a virtual reality task in which they showed impaired spatial navigation. These observations suggest that together with volumetric reductions of the hippocampus in depressed patients reported elsewhere, the right anterior hippocampus and parahippocampal cortices function abnormally in depressed patients. Whether a contributing factor or consequence, abnormal functioning of these structures is likely to have pervasive effects on cognition and affect in those who suffer from depression. Virtual navigation tasks, combined with neuroimaging, provide a tool to expand on the significance of hippocampal dysfunction for the pathophysiology of major depressive disorder.

References

1.
Perera TD, Park S, Nemirovskaya Y: Cognitive role of neurogenesis in depression and antidepressant treatment. Neuroscientist 2008; 14:326–338
2.
Sahay A, Hen R: Adult hippocampal neurogenesis in depression. Nat Neurosci 2007; 10:1110–1115
3.
Campbell S, Marriott M, Nahmias C, MacQueen GM: Lower hippocampal volume in patients suffering from depression: a meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:598–607
4.
Videbech P, Ravnkilde B: Hippocampal volume and depression: a meta-analysis of MRI studies. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1957–1966
5.
McEwen BS: Glucocorticoids, depression, and mood disorders: structural remodeling in the brain. Metabolism 2005; 54(suppl 1):20–23
6.
Sapolsky RM: Glucocorticoids and hippocampal atrophy in neuropsychiatric disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:925–935
7.
Videbech P, Ravnkilde B, Pedersen AR, Egander A, Landbo B, Rasmussen NA, Andersen F, Stodkilde-Jorgensen H, Gjedde A, Rosenberg R: The Danish PET/depression project: PET findings in patients with major depression. Psychol Med 2001; 31:1147–1158
8.
Sweeney JA, Kmiec JA, Kupfer DJ: Neuropsychologic impairments in bipolar and unipolar mood disorders on the CANTAB neurocognitive battery. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:674–684
9.
Gould NF, Holmes MK, Fantie BD, Luckenbaugh DA, Pine DS, Gould TD, Burgess N, Manji HK, Zarate CA: Performance on a virtual reality spatial memory navigation task in depressed patients. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:516–519
10.
Maguire EA, Burgess N, O'Keefe J: Human spatial navigation: cognitive maps, sexual dimorphism, and neural substrates. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1999; 9:171–177
11.
Ekstrom AD, Caplan JB, Ho E, Shattuck K, Fried I, Kahana MJ: Human hippocampal theta activity during virtual navigation. Hippocampus 2005; 15:881–889
12.
Kahana MJ, Sekuler R, Caplan JB, Kirschen M, Madsen JR: Human theta oscillations exhibit task dependence during virtual maze navigation. Nature 1999; 399:781–784
13.
Buzsaki G: Theta rhythm of navigation: link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus 2005; 15:827–840
14.
O'Keefe J, Recce ML: Phase relationship between hippocampal place units and the EEG theta rhythm. Hippocampus 1993; 3:317–330
15.
Morris RGM: Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learn Motivat 1981; 12:239–260
16.
Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O'Keefe J: Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature 1982; 297:681–683
17.
Astur RS, Taylor LB, Mamelak AN, Philpott L, Sutherland RJ: Humans with hippocampus damage display severe spatial memory impairments in a virtual Morris water task. Behav Brain Res 2002; 132:77–84
18.
Cornwell BR, Johnson LL, Holroyd T, Carver FW, Grillon C: Human hippocampal and parahippocampal theta during goal-directed spatial navigation predicts performance on a virtual Morris water maze. J Neurosci 2008; 28:5983–5990
19.
Shipman SL, Astur RS: Factors affecting the hippocampal BOLD response during spatial memory. Behav Brain Res 2008; 187:433–441
20.
Robinson SE, Vrba J: Functional neuroimaging by Synthetic Aperture Magnetometry (SAM), in Recent Advances in Biomagnetism. Edited by, Yoshimoto T, Kotani M, Kuriki S, Karibe H, Nakasato N. Sendai, Japan, Tohoku University Press, 1999, pp 302–305
21.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams AR: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 2001
22.
American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed (DSM-IV). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association, 1994
23.
Montgomery SA, Asberg M: A new depression scale designed to be sensitive to change. Br J Psychiatry 1979; 134:382–389
24.
Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Press, 1994
25.
Hillebrand A, Singh KD, Holliday IE, Furlong PL, Barnes GR: A new approach to neuroimaging with magnetoencephalography. Hum Brain Mapp 2005; 25:199–211
26.
Singh KD, Barnes GR, Hillebrand A, Forde EM, Williams AL: Task-related changes in cortical synchronization are spatially coincident with the hemodynamic response. Neuroimage 2002; 16:103–114
27.
Cornwell BR, Carver FW, Coppola R, Johnson L, Alvarez R, Grillon C: Evoked amygdala responses to negative faces revealed by adaptive MEG beamformers. Brain Res 2008; 1244:103–112
28.
Salvadore G, Cornwell BR, Colon-Rosario V, Coppola R, Grillon C, Zarate CA, Manji HK: Increased anterior cingulate cortical activity in response to fearful faces: a neurophysiological biomarker that predicts rapid antidepressant response to ketamine. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 65:289–295
29.
Cox RW: AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 1996; 29:162–173
30.
Astur RS, Ortiz ML, Sutherland RJ: A characterization of performance by men and women in a virtual Morris water task: a large and reliable sex difference. Behav Brain Res 1998; 93:185–190
31.
Driscoll I, Hamilton DA, Petropoulos H, Yeo RA, Brooks WM, Baumgartner RN, Sutherland RJ: The aging hippocampus: cognitive, biochemical and structural findings. Cereb Cortex 2003; 13:1344–1351
32.
Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T: Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 2002; 15:870–878
33.
Dunham JS, Deakin JF, Miyajima F, Payton A, Toro CT: Expression of hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor and its receptors in Stanley consortium brains. J Psychiatr Res 2009; 43:1175–1184
34.
Moser E, Moser MB, Andersen P: Spatial learning impairment parallels the magnitude of dorsal hippocampal lesions, but is hardly present following ventral lesions. J Neurosci 1993; 13:3916–3925
35.
Maguire EA, Woollett K, Spiers HJ: London taxi drivers and bus drivers: a structural MRI and neuropsychological analysis. Hippocampus 2006; 16:1091–1101
36.
Wager TD, Smith EE: Neuroimaging studies of working memory: a meta-analysis. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2003; 3:255–274
37.
Hinkelmann K, Moritz S, Botzenhardt J, Riedesel K, Wiedenmann K, Kellner M, Otte C: Cognitive impairment in major depression: association with salivary cortisol. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 66:879–885
38.
Taylor-Tavares JV, Clark L, Cannon DM, Erickson K, Drevets WC, Sahakian BJ: Distinct profiles of neurocognitive function in unmedicated unipolar depression and bipolar II depression. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:917–924
39.
Fava M, Rush AJ, Alpert JE, Balasubramani GK, Wisniewski SR, Carmin CN, Biggs MM, Zisook S, Leuchter A, Howland R, Warden D, Trivedi MH: Difference in treatment outcome in outpatients with anxious versus nonanxious depression: a STAR*D report. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:342–351
40.
Mueller SC, Temple V, Cornwell B, Grillon C, Pine DS, Ernst M: Impaired spatial navigation in pediatric anxiety. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50:1227–1234

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 836 - 844
PubMed: 20439387

History

Received: 4 May 2009
Accepted: 7 December 2009
Published online: 1 July 2010
Published in print: July 2010

Authors

Details

Brian R. Cornwell, Ph.D.
Giacomo Salvadore, M.D.
Veronica Colon-Rosario, B.A.
Frederick W. Carver, Ph.D.
Richard Coppola, Ph.D.
Husseini K. Manji, M.D.
Carlos A. Zarate, Jr., M.D.
Christian Grillon, Ph.D.

Notes

Presented in part at the 63rd Annual Meeting and Convention of the Society of Biological Psychiatry, Washington, D.C., May 1–3, 2008. Received May 4, 2009; revisions received Aug. 31 and Dec. 1, 2009; accepted Dec. 7, 2009. From the Mood and Anxiety Disorders Program and the Magnetoencephalography Core Facility, National Institute of Mental Health. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Cornwell, Mood and Anxiety Disorders Program, NIMH, 15K North Dr., MSC 2670, Bethesda, MD 20892; [email protected] (e-mail).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests at the time the study was conducted and the manuscript prepared and submitted. A patent application for the use of ketamine in depression has been submitted listing Drs. Manji and Zarate among the inventors. Drs. Manji and Zarate have assigned their rights on the patent to the U.S. government. Dr. Manji is now Vice President, CNS and Pain, Johnson and Johnson Pharmaceutical Research and Development.

Funding Information

Supported by the NIMH Intramural Research Program, National Institutes of Health, and a National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression award to Dr. Zarate.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share