Skip to main content
Full access
Review and Overview
Published Online: 1 May 2010

Genetic Sensitivity to the Environment: The Case of the Serotonin Transporter Gene and Its Implications for Studying Complex Diseases and Traits

Abstract

Abstract

Evidence of marked variability in response among people exposed to the same environmental risk implies that individual differences in genetic susceptibility might be at work. The study of such Gene-by-Environment (GxE) interactions has gained momentum. In this article, the authors review research about one of the most extensive areas of inquiry: variation in the promoter region of the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4; also known as 5-HTT) and its contribution to stress sensitivity. Research in this area has both advanced basic science and generated broader lessons for studying complex diseases and traits. The authors evaluate four lines of evidence about the 5-HTT stress-sensitivity hypothesis: 1) observational studies about the serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR), stress sensitivity, and depression in humans; 2) experimental neuroscience studies about the 5-HTTLPR and biological phenotypes relevant to the human stress response; 3) studies of 5-HTT variation and stress sensitivity in nonhuman primates; and 4) studies of stress sensitivity and genetically engineered 5-HTT mutations in rodents. The authors then dispel some misconceptions and offer recommendations for GxE research. The authors discuss how GxE interaction hypotheses can be tested with large and small samples, how GxE research can be carried out before as well as after replicated gene discovery, the uses of GxE research as a tool for gene discovery, the importance of construct validation in evaluating GxE research, and the contribution of GxE research to the public understanding of genetic science.
In 1996, it was reported that a repeat length polymorphism in the promoter region of the human serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4; also known as 5-HTT) regulates gene expression in vitro. Furthermore, individuals carrying one or two copies of the relatively low-expressing short (S) allele of the serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) exhibit elevated neuroticism, a personality trait involved in the propensity to depression (1). In 2002, it was reported that S-carriers exhibit elevated amygdala reactivity to threatening stimuli, as assessed by functional MRI (2). In 2003, it was reported that S-carriers exhibit elevated depressive symptoms, diagnosable depression, and suicidality after experiencing stressful life events and childhood maltreatment (3).
These three papers have influenced scientific and public discourse in three ways. First, the 5-HTTLPR has become the most investigated genetic variant in psychiatry, psychology, and neuroscience. Second, these three papers and those following have generated evidence for validity of the construct of genetically driven individual differences in stress sensitivity. Third, the 5-HTTLPR Gene x Environment (GxE) interaction has captured the public imagination and framed contemporary discussions about how genes and environments shape who we are. In this article, we review the cumulative evidence base documenting the role of 5-HTT in sensitivity to stress and vulnerability to psychopathology. Because the evidence base on 5-HTT and stress sensitivity is currently advanced relative to other GxE investigations, this hypothesis constitutes a case study with lessons that extend to GxE research in general. The article begins with a review of studies and ends with lessons.
This article takes an inclusive approach to the literature on 5-HTT and stress sensitivity, as opposed to an exclusive focus on papers attempting to approximate the methods of the initial report of a 5-HTTLPR GxE interaction (3). An inclusive review is essential once it is understood that the hypothesis of interest is that variation in 5-HTT influences reactivity to environmental stress exposure, and thereby brings about risk for depression. Accordingly, in many studies testing the 5-HTT stress-sensitivity hypothesis, the outcome is not depression per se. Rather, inferential advantages are gained by studying intermediate phenotypes on the causal pathway from stress to depression that are considered to index stress sensitivity (e.g., stress hormones, amygdala reactivity). Likewise, stress is not narrowly construed as a count of stressful life events. Other stressors are examined in the field and in the laboratory, whenever doing so augments scientific inference (e.g., hurricane exposure rules out gene-environment correlation because victims' genes could not evoke this life event; officially recorded child abuse rules out recall bias; experimental stress induction allows titration of stress dosage). Because the outcome is not restricted to human depression, important information comes from studies of 5-HTT and stress sensitivity in animals (e.g., genetically modified mice, rhesus macaques carrying an orthologous 5-HTTLPR variant).

Evidence for the 5-HTT Stress Sensitivity Hypothesis

It is evident from research conducted with multiple species and from research using both observational and experimental methods that variation in 5-HTT modifies organisms' stress responses to their environments (Figure 1). Complementary experimental and observational research designs are integral to testing not only the 5-HTT stress-sensitivity hypothesis, but all GxE hypotheses (4, 5). Experiments with humans, nonhuman primates, and rodents elucidate biological mechanisms behind the hypothesis and also validate findings from human observational studies by using designs with stronger internal validity (e.g., by random assignment to stress conditions). Observational studies use designs with stronger external validity (e.g., by studying real-world stressors), estimate the effect size of the 5-HTTLPR GxE interaction in the human population, and allow researchers to study clinical depression as the outcome.
Figure 1. Role of 5-HTT Variation in Stress Sensitivity as Underscored by the Coherence of Findings From Hypothesis-Driven Studies in Multiple Species Employing Multiple Methodologies

Human Observational Studies

The initial GxE effect (3) did not have an overwhelmingly impressive p value, but it was robust, having been 1) discovered in an epidemiologically sound longitudinal cohort study; 2) tested in a straightforward and transparent analysis; 3) reproduced across two stressors, child maltreatment and adult stressful life events; and 4) reproduced across four depression phenotypes. How has this hypothesis fared in observational studies since it was initially tested?
Table 1 and Table 2 list all human observational studies up to summer 2009 that tested the hypothesis that the 5-HTTLPR moderates the effect of stress on depression phenotypes. Three observations emerge from the tables. First, multiple studies have reported that S-carriage moderates the influence of stress on depression. Whether or not the initial finding can be replicated has been answered in the affirmative. Second, positive findings have emerged from a variety of observational research designs used to test the hypothesis, including phenotype case-only designs, case-control designs, cross-sectional designs, longitudinal designs, and exposure designs. This suggests the finding is "sturdy," in the sense that its signal can be detected despite noise from varying research settings, sample characteristics, and study designs (6). Third, there have also been quite a few negative findings. The degree to which negative findings call the original result into question depends on whether differences in study designs are systematically related to differences in study findings. If failures to replicate are characterized by systematically different subject populations or systematically weaker methodologies, their challenge to the original result is greatly diminished.
Table 1. Human Observational Studies Testing the Hypothesis That the 5-HTTLPR Moderates the Effect of Stress on Depression Phenotypes in Studies of Specific Stressors
Table 2. Human Observational Studies Testing the Hypothesis That the 5-HTTLPR Moderates the Effect of Stress on Depression Phenotypes in Studies of Stressful/Adverse Life Events
We considered factors that might covary with positive versus negative findings, including subjects' sex, age, and nationality, and features of phenotype measurement, but these did not covary systematically with findings. However, positive and negative findings did closely track variation in methodological features related to the quality of environmental exposure measurement. Concerns have been expressed about standards of stress assessment in tests of this hypothesis (7, 8). We call attention to three issues.
First, almost all nonreplications rely on brief self-report measures of stress, whereas studies using objective indicators or face-to-face interviews to assess stress exposure yield positive replications ("Stress Assessment" column in Tables 1 and 2). Face-to-face interviewers can clarify the meaning of a reported life event and enhance memory for life events by probing and by using techniques such as life event calendars, as did the initial study (3). In contrast, it is known that self-report event checklists gather idiosyncratic and inaccurate information (9, 10).
Second, studies of specific stressors consistently yield positive findings. Why are these studies so consistent? One possibility is that their focus on a specific, homogeneous, developmentally relevant, and clearly operationalized depression-inducing event decreased between-subject heterogeneity in the exposure and enhanced internal validity of the study design. Table 1 groups studies of two specific stressors that are established causes of depression: childhood maltreatment and medical illness. Nine studies report about depression that follows childhood experiences associated with maltreatment and victimization. Although exposure measurement is not uniform, the studies are united by focusing on threatening events in which physical, sexual, or relational harm were carried out or intended. Virtually all of these studies focus on children, adolescents, and young adults. All of them show that S-carriage moderates the association between child maltreatment and depression. Another nine studies report about depression following medical illness. Virtually all focus on middle-aged and elderly participants. Studies of patients suffering hip fractures, strokes, Parkinson's disease, heart disease, and chronic-disease load show that S-carriage moderates the association between medical illness and depression.
Third, whereas studies of specific stressors consistently generate positive findings, studies of stressful/adverse life events yield mixed results (Table 2). This inconsistency could result from the highly variable measurement of stressful life events (7, 11). The pool of studies exemplifies five difficulties in stress measurement: 1) Stress measures are sometimes noncomparable and fall prey to the fallacy that because measures have the same name they measure the same construct (12). For example, some studies count death of a spouse as a stressor, whereas others count being the child of a father in an unskilled job as a stressor. Some studies count stress events, others model event severity. Some stressors are chronic, others acute. Some studies define a "stressor" by its level of distress, others do not. Some studies examine events that happened to the proband, others examine events among the proband's friends and relatives. 2) Some studies assess stress through currently depressed individuals' self-reports, which are biased by mood-congruent memory revision and thus overcount events (13). Moreover, humans seek explanations, a phenomenon termed "effort after meaning," which leads respondents who have been depressed to misattribute their illness to a life event. Some studies assess events through long-term retrospective reports (sometimes over decades), which are flawed by forgetting and undercount life events, particularly among respondents who lack depression. In addition, respondents often overcount trivial and undercount severe events (9). These cognitive processes (mood-congruent memory revision, effort after meaning, and retrospective forgetting) working together can artifactually influence a study's association between life events and depression. Thus, a correlation between life events and depression does not indicate validity, contrary to claims (14). 3) Some studies test the connection between stress and depression contemporaneously, others across years or decades. 4) Some studies are unable to rule out reverse causation, in which depression precipitates stressful events; for example, one study measured depression over the respondents' lifetime, but ascertained life stress during only the past year (15). 5) Most studies do not consider variation in participants' depression history, despite evidence that stress is more relevant for initial than recurrent depression episodes.
In the first decade of research about the 5-HTTLPR GxE interaction, scientists have frequently taken advantage of existing data sets, quickly adding genotype data to studies that had previously measured depression and life events for other purposes. Not all of these studies' designs and measures are well-suited to testing the GxE hypothesis. Covariation between poor measurement quality and negative findings was observed early on (16) and has been confirmed with the increasing number of published GxE studies (17). Notably, many of the largest studies in Table 1 and Table 2 were obliged to collect brief retrospective self-reports of stress through telephone interviews or postal questionnaires in order to contain data collection costs. Thus, unfortunately, large sample size tends to coincide with poor measurement quality, and meta-analyses that give larger samples greater weight in estimating an effect across studies further compound this problem. There is hope that a new generation of cohort studies purpose-built for testing GxE interactions will improve replicability, but these must correct the problems of exposure measurement discussed in the previous paragraph, lest they merely repeat the problems on a far larger scale.
Most observational GxE research on 5-HTT in humans has focused on depression. However, additional evidence links the 5-HTTLPR to a broader range of stress-reactive phenotypes, including PTSD (18), posttrauma suicide attempt (19), aggressive reactions to a cold-pressor test (20), stress-linked alcohol consumption (21, 22) and substance use (23, 24), stress-related sleep disturbance (25), and even premature ejaculation (26). Research on quantitative endophenotypes shows that S-carriers with high levels of childhood maltreatment and adversity exhibit enhanced anxiety sensitivity (27) and a bias toward perceiving and expecting negative outcomes (28). Moreover, S-carrying children who are raised by unresponsive or nonsupportive mothers exhibit poor self-regulation of negative affect (2932), which predicts a variety of adult psychiatric disorders (33). Finally, research that monitors affective experiences on a daily basis shows that S-carriers experience anxious mood on days with more intense stressors (34) and larger increases in negative affect while trying to quit smoking (35). To claim that these diverse outcomes are heterotypic manifestations of a unifying genetic vulnerability to stress reflected in the 5-HTTLPR S allele requires a theory that specifies the unifying mechanism. The leading theory (1, 36) is that the 5-HTTLPR is a genetic substrate for a latent personality trait, termed negative affectivity or neuroticism. Negative affectivity prospectively predicts risk for all stress-related psychiatric disorders (37). In theory, 5-HTTLPR S-carriers are characterized by the stable trait of negative affectivity that is converted to psychopathology only under conditions of stress, just as glass is always characterized by the trait of brittleness but shatters only when a stone is thrown. Negative affectivity represents the potential for excitability of anxiety and fear neural circuits, and is characterized by an attentional bias toward negatively valenced information and a cognitive sensitivity to perceive threat (38). This trait is operationalized in all experimental tests of the 5-HTTLPR GxE hypothesis, reviewed next.

Experimental Neuroscience Studies

In 2002, a synergy emerged between research in human affective neuroscience and genetic research into the 5-HTTLPR. Specifically, noninvasive functional MRI (fMRI), which assays information processing within distinct neuronal circuits, revealed relatively exaggerated threat-related amygdala reactivity in carriers of the 5-HTTLPR S allele (2). This initial finding has since been replicated in independent samples of both healthy volunteers and psychiatric patients, using a multitude of threatening stimuli and neuroimaging modalities (3949). This effect on the magnitude of amygdala reactivity has recently been extended, with S-carriers also exhibiting a relatively faster response than L-allele homozygotes (50). Consistent with the heightened sensitivity to environmental threat documented in S-carriers, recent work suggests that the effects of the S allele on amygdala function may be unique to stimulus-provoked amygdala reactivity and not elevated baseline levels of activation (5153).
The bias in threat-related amygdala reactivity associated with the 5-HTTLPR S allele is positioned to drive the polymorphism's associations with altered mood and affective disorders, especially in interaction with exposure to environmental stressors and trauma. Evidence from animal and human studies demonstrates that the amygdala mediates both physiological (e.g., autonomic reactivity) and behavioral (e.g., reallocation of attentional resources) effects that allow an individual to respond to environmental and social challenges (54). Neuroimaging studies have reported positive correlations between indices of anxiety and amygdala reactivity to affective stimuli (especially threatening stimuli) (55). Such findings demonstrate that variability in the magnitude of threat-related amygdala reactivity predicts individual differences in sensitivity to environmental threat and stress.
Human neuroimaging research suggests that relatively increased amygdala reactivity associated with the 5-HTTLPR S allele is likely to reflect both the functional and structural architecture of a distributed network of brain regions. Research suggests that this network communicates information about the environment to the amygdala and relays signals between the amygdala and regulatory circuits in the medial prefrontal cortex. This putative mechanism is further underscored by the significant role serotonin signaling plays in the general development and function of this extended neural network (56). The S allele has been associated with altered functional coupling (as indexed by correlated fMRI signal strength) between the amygdala and regions of the medial prefrontal cortex (40, 57). These medial prefrontal regions integrate amygdala-mediated arousal and down regulate amygdala reactivity. Medial prefrontal regions are also involved in the extinction of conditioned fear responses, which are dependent on amygdala circuitry.
The pattern of 5-HTTLPR-associated differences in the functional dynamics of the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex is echoed in structural measures within this same network. Specifically, the S allele has been associated with relatively decreased gray matter volume in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (42, 57). The S allele has also been associated with alterations in the microstructure of the uncinate fasciculus, the white matter fiber bundle providing the majority of connections between the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (58). Individual differences in uncinate fasciculus microstructure correlate with trait anxiety (59). In addition, postmortem tissue analyses have associated the 5-HTTLPR S allele with relative enlargement of the pulvinar, which relays visual information to subcortical and higher cortical brain regions (60). Consistent with this, as well as with amygdala-mediated behavioral arousal, numerous studies have reported increased cortical activity in response to experimental provocation in S-carriers (6166).
In addition, a growing group of studies has begun to document effects of the 5-HTTLPR on intermediate behavioral and physiological processes that map onto these alterations in brain structure and function. The S allele is associated with increased acquisition of conditioned fear responses (67), increased auditory startle response (68, 69), and greater sympathetic reactivity when simply observing another person receiving shock (70). Moreover, the 5-HTTLPR S allele has been associated with increased HPA axis reactivity to aversive or threatening stimuli in a number of studies (7174). The S allele typically has no impact on baseline levels of HPA function in these studies, underscoring its documented effect on threat-related amygdala reactivity. In addition, the S allele has been linked with difficulty disengaging from, or preferential attention toward, threat-related stimuli (7579), a more negative information-processing bias (80), emotion-induced retrograde amnesia (81), sensitivity to financial loss (70, 82), and even social blushing (83). Although this literature is not without inconsistencies (e.g., some reported associations are sex-specific and others have not replicated), it does suggest that the effects of the 5-HTTLPR S allele on the brain's neural circuitry for responding to environmental threat and stress translate to biases in both behavioral and physiological processes which may, in turn, shape individual risk for depression upon exposure to acute trauma or chronic stressors (Figure 2). Multiple components of this ongoing research were highlighted in one report of increases in threat-related amygdala and medial prefrontal cortical activation as well as heart rate and startle amplitude in 5-HTTLPR S-carriers who also exhibited a self-reported sensitivity to perceived danger in the environment (28).
Figure 2. How the 5-HTTLPR Affects Neural Circuitry for Responding to Environmental Threat and Stress
aImplicated in humans and nonhuman primates.
bImplicated in humans, nonhuman primates, and rodents.

Nonhuman Primate Studies

Rhesus monkeys have an orthologue of the human 5-HTTLPR, making them an excellent model species for GxE studies. Like the human variant, the rhesus S allele is associated with decreased transcriptional efficiency in vitro (84). The modulating influence of the polymorphism on early life stress has been tested by separating infant rhesus monkeys from their mothers and rearing them with other infants (a long-established model of early life adversity in this species). During initial episodes of separation, monkeys carrying the rh5-HTTLPR S allele exhibit less "protest" and self-directed behaviors that are considered active coping responses to this stressor (85). Instead, separated S-allele monkeys display greater anxiety, agitation, stereotypies, and an exaggerated HPA axis response (85, 86).
The modulating influence of the rh5-HTTLPR on separation in infancy persists into later life, manifesting, for example, as higher ACTH responses to stress in S-carrier monkeys than LL homozygotes (87). It is important to underscore that these long-lasting phenotypic effects of the S allele only occur in monkeys exposed to maternal separation early life stress, echoing the GxE interaction observed in relation to human depression.
Another major parallel between the human and monkey data has been the finding that, as in humans, the stress-related S allele phenotype in monkeys is related to an intermediate neural phenotype characterized by abnormal corticolimbic structure and function. For example, the S allele in monkeys also has been mapped onto reduced gray matter volumes in the amygdala, medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex, and pulvinar (88). Moreover, monkeys with the S allele exhibit greater metabolic activity than LL homozygotes in the amygdala and its networked cortical regions, including orbitofrontal cortex, in response to the stress of relocation (89). Given the importance of the orbitofrontal cortex in social behavior, abnormalities in this region might also account for the finding that S-carriers engage in less eye gaze with high status conspecifics and are more risk-averse in their presence (90). An intriguing development is recent data from S-carrier monkeys (88) and 5-HTT mutant mice (91) demonstrating that reversal learning, a measure of cognitive flexibility subserved by the orbitofrontal cortex (92), is enhanced as a function of relative 5-HTT gene deficiency. This may reflect increased sensitivity to negative environmental stimuli, although further work will be needed to substantiate this. Notwithstanding, these data indicate that altered 5-HTT gene function may influence multiple higher behaviors, as would be predicted if it affects a core corticolimbic neural circuitry.

Studies Involving Genetically Engineered 5-HTT Mutations in Rodents

Research using rodents allows for experimental control over genetic background and the environment to a degree that is neither practically nor ethically feasible in human or even nonhuman primate studies (93). Although there is functional gene variation in the murine 5-HTT (slc6a4) (94), there is no rodent orthologue of the 5-HTTLPR. As an alternative approach, mice and rats have been genetically engineered with loss-of-function mutations in the 5-HTT gene. Studying the consequences of these mutations for behavior and brain function has greatly complemented the work on the 5-HTTLPR in primates and provided some key insights into the mechanisms that mediate the influence of 5-HTT on negative affect and stress reactivity (56, 95).
Mice in which the 5-HTT has been functionally excised either by targeted mutation or chemical mutagenesis exhibit heightened anxiety-like behavior, impaired fear extinction, and exaggerated HPA-axis responses to acute stress. While it is far less common to engineer mutant rats than mice, a 5-HTT-null mutant rat has been generated and also shows increased anxiety-like behavior (96). Furthermore, providing an interesting counterpoint to these "knockout" mutants, mice with transgenic overexpression of the 5-HTT actually produce decreased anxiety-like behavior (97). The consistency of these findings across models, laboratories, and species is rarely seen in the field of rodent behavioral genetics and illustrates the strong penetrance of the mutation's effects.
The "depression-related" consequences of rodent 5-HTT knockout mice are, at first blush, less consistent than the anxiety-related consequences, in that they are seen in some of the standard rodent assays for this behavior but not others. This variability may, however, be a legitimate reflection of differences in the level of stress evoked under varying test conditions. In support of this hypothesis, following repeated exposure to stress (e.g., forced swimming, tail suspension), 5-HTT knockout mice develop a depression-related "despair-like" phenotype that is not seen with single exposure (97). The parallels with the primate data showing that the S-allele influence on depression is contingent upon repeated stress exposure are clear.
Much of our understanding of the functional role of the 5-HTT as a master modulator of the 5-HT system has been built upon work in rodents (98). As such, researchers have a ready platform and toolset from which to perform certain neural and molecular analyses in 5-HTT mutant rodents (e.g., in vivo measurement of brain 5-HT availability) that cannot be employed in humans. One of the key themes to emerge from this work is that the neural consequences of 5-HTT gene mutation extend well beyond the 5-HTT and its role as a regulator of 5-HT availability. 5-HTT null mutation leads to alterations throughout the 5-HT system that include changes in 5-HT receptor binding and 5-HT synthesis (56, 95). At the systems level, 5-HTT knockout mice exhibit an abnormally high density of excitatory dendritic spines on amygdala neurons and an increase in dendritic arborization of prefrontal cortex neurons (56). The implication here is that influence of 5-HTT variation may not be limited to effects on 5-HT availability or even on the 5-HT system. Recently, this implication was confirmed in a rhesus macaque model (88), in which the rh5-HTTLPR S allele affected behavior and brain morphology but not 5-HTT (99) or 5-HT1A concentrations in vivo. Similar complexities in the likely molecular consequences of the 5-HTTLPR have been documented in humans (100105). Collectively, mouse and monkey and human findings suggest that 5-HTTLPR's behavioral effects on stress-reactivity may be most consistently rooted in neural development.
An intriguing line of enquiry in this context has centered on the hypothesis that 5-HTT variation may in part modulate the capacity to cope with stress by shaping the early life development of corticolimbic circuitry (56). In fact, the importance of the 5-HT system in neurodevelopment has long been recognized, and the 5-HTT is known to be critical for the formation of cortical systems in particular (106, 107). Pharmacological inhibition of the 5-HTT during early life mimics the anxiety-like phenotype of 5-HTT knockout (108). Moreover, poor maternal care produces heightened anxiety-like behavior in mice with a partial (heterozygous) 5-HTT null mutation, which are phenotypically normal under conditions of good maternal care (109).
These findings raise the question of whether the effects of 5-HTT knockout are developmentally driven. It has been hypothesized that the 5-HTTLPR GxE interaction observed in relation to adult stressful life events should selectively affect people already "primed" by childhood adversity (8). This opens up some very interesting avenues for future animal studies. For example, would 5-HTT loss restricted to early life development be sufficient to increase anxiety and impair stress-coping? If so, is there a critical window and what is the corresponding ontogenic period in humans? Researchers could then elucidate the key neural and molecular changes underlying these effects. This could, in turn, "square the circle" by nominating mechanisms to target with novel therapeutic approaches in humans.

Lessons for GxE Research

In the previous section, we reviewed evidence about the 5-HTT stress-sensitivity hypothesis. Lessons learned in this research apply broadly to all GxE research. In this section, we draw on these lessons to dispel some misconceptions and offer some constructive recommendations.

GxE Hypotheses Can Be Tested With Large and Small Samples

Statistical power is critical for theory-free, exploratory scans for GxE interactions (110). This realization has prompted the creation of large case-control consortia and massive biobanks. A question that puzzles many readers is how to reconcile the obvious benefits of huge samples with evidence that GxE interactions have been reported in many small-sample studies of 5-HTT and stress sensitivity, particularly in studies comparing stress-exposed versus matched nonexposed groups (e.g., abused children) and in experimental studies of humans and animals. There are statistical reasons for this.
The problem has to do with the approach to testing interactions (111): If the product term (i.e., the interaction term in multiple regression) is calculated from two normally distributed symmetrical variables, it has restricted variance but is uncorrelated with the first-order predictors (Figure 3, top row). However, a product term of two categorical variables (e.g., minor allele frequency [MAF] of 25% and rate of exposure [Pexp] of 25%) is significantly correlated with the first-order predictors (Figure 3, middle row). Such is the case in practically all observational GxE studies of psychiatric phenotypes. As a result, the residual variance of the product term after factoring out first-order predictors—and the corresponding power to detect interactions—declines rapidly with minor allele frequencies and rates of exposure departing from 50%. The full power for testing interactions between categorical variables is only preserved in the optimal case where minor allele frequency and exposure rate equal 50% (bottom of Figure 3). An implication of this insight is that hypothesis-driven GxE studies that recruit participants on the basis of their genotype and their environmental exposure (e.g., experimental GxE studies with balanced cell sizes) are better powered to test for genetically moderated exposure effects than are observational field studies, which must make do with unequal-sized groups since these occur in nature.
Figure 3. How the Power to Detect GxE Interactions Depends on the Distributions of the Genotypes and Exposures in the Samplea
aThe two rows of graphs demonstrate a key difference between interactions involving normally distributed continuous variables (top row) and those involving asymmetrically distributed categorical ones (middle row). If the product term A*B (i.e., the term that represents interaction in a multiple regression) is calculated from two normally distributed symmetrical variables A and B, it has a restricted variance (leptokurtic distribution) but is uncorrelated with the first-order predictors (i.e., the correlations between A and A*B [rA, A*B] and between B and A*B [rB, A*B] are zero). However, the product term G*E that represents two categorical variables (G: genotype with a minor allele frequency [MAF] of 25%; and E: categorical exposure in the population [PEXP] of 25%) is strongly correlated with the first-order predictors (i.e., the correlations between G and G*E [rG, G*E] and between E and G*E [rE, G*E] are substantial). As a result, the residual variance of the product term (bottom of figure) after factoring out first-order predictors, and the power to detect interactions, declines rapidly as the rates of exposure and minor allele frequency depart from 50%. The full power for testing interactions between categorical variables is only preserved in the special case of minor allele frequency equal to 50% and exposure rate of 50% (the top segment in red). "Density" reflects the proportion of individuals falling within each narrow band of values of the variable on the x axis.

GxE Research Can Be Carried Out Before as Well as After Replicated Gene Discovery

Some researchers claim that GxE studies should only be carried out if there exists a genotype-to-phenotype main effect, but this claim is statistically unwarranted (112). Such a strategy also precludes identification of environmentally dependent genetic effects that are small in absolute size or are contingent on relatively uncommon environmental factors (Figure 4). Moreover, genotype-phenotype association studies may not replicate if GxE interactions are operating and research samples differ on environmental risk exposure. Waiting for genomewide association studies (GWAS) to throw up candidate genes may be ill-advised because GxE interactions may conceal good candidates from GWAS. Inconsistent genotype-phenotype associations have inspired successful searches for GxE interactions in different fields of medicine, from asthma (113) to cardiovascular disease (114). Inconsistent associations between the 5-HTTLPR S allele and depression (115117) prompted us to consider a GxE interaction in our initial studies of the 5-HTTLPR and depression.
Figure 4. How the Frequency of an Environmental Exposure in a Sample Influences the Ability to Detect Genetic Effects and GxE Interactionsa
aPanel A shows the influence of environmental exposure frequency on the ability to identify genetic effects, in two genotypes of equal prevalence. Genotype A shows no phenotypic response to the environmental exposure. Genotype B shows a response to the environmental exposure. What would happen if the association between genotype and phenotype were studied without knowledge of the environmental exposure and its frequency (shown from 10% to 90%)? A sample having many exposed subjects will report a genetic effect on the phenotype, whereas a sample having few exposed subjects will not, and if exposure is not ascertained, the source of nonreplication will remain a mystery. Panel B shows the influence of the rate of environmental exposure on statistical power to detect GxE interactions and main effects of genes. Each point is based on 10,000 simulations of samples of 1000 drawn from a population with equal distributions of two genotypes, with a continuous outcome generated as a moderately strong GxE (i.e., the difference in the environment-phenotype correlation between genetic strata =0.3), and no main effect. In samples with exposure frequency close to 0, there is no detectable interaction or main effect. For exposure frequency below 50%, there is greater power to detect a GxE interaction (blue line) than to detect a main effect of genes (red line). With rates of exposure exceeding 50%, the power of detecting a direct effect of genes (red line) increases above that of detecting an interaction, even though interaction is the data-generating mechanism. The probability of detecting a spurious main effect of genes (or environments) remains at the 2.5% chance level across the range of exposure frequency if the interaction term is retained in the equation.
The Psychiatric GWAS Consortium (118) recommends conducting GxE studies only after convincing genotype-phenotype associations have been identified by 1) finding the disease susceptibility gene by conducting a GWAS, then 2) identifying the functional consequences of the putative causal variant, and only then 3) testing interactions between the variant and environmental factors. This strategy is presumed to offer a foolproof approach to detecting replicable GxE interactions. However, research in obesity illustrates this strategy may not work. FTO was found to be a susceptibility gene through GWAS (119), and FTO's functional consequences were identified (120123). GxE research then documented that an active lifestyle mitigates obesity risk from FTO (124127). However, this GxE interaction has not universally replicated (128, 129), in part because of cross-study differences in the quality of physical activity measurement. The moral is that a robust genotype-phenotype association cannot guarantee a robust GxE finding, because the study of GxE interactions requires more appropriate and high-quality exposure measurement.

GxE Research Is a Helpful Tool for Gene Discovery

Although most GxE research uses candidate genes, environmental exposures can also be used to discover novel loci. Indeed, one possible reason for the paucity of susceptibility genes in psychiatry is that gene-discovery studies have been searching for genetic effects on disease rather than for genetic effects on vulnerability to environmental causes of disease (130). Whereas in genetic association studies, a candidate gene is a gene suspected of being involved in a trait or disease—either because its protein product is relevant or because it has been uncovered in the course of association or linkage analysis of the phenotype—in GxE research a candidate gene is one plausibly related to the organisms' reactivity to the environmental risk or pathogen (131). The idea that genes may moderate the effect of environmental risk has direct implications for hypothesis-driven selection of novel candidate genes. For example, genes associated with the physiological response to psychological stress, particularly in the HPA axis, are natural candidates for GxE research on stress and depression (132). Genes regulated by hypoxia are candidates for GxE research on obstetric complications and schizophrenia (133). Genes involved in biosynthesis of fatty acids are candidates for GxE research on nutrition and brain development (134). Genes involved in lead absorption are relevant for research on attention deficits and hyperactivity (135). Genes involved in ototoxicity are relevant for research on learning difficulties (136).
Research on "candidate environmental risks" can be combined with theory-free genetics to discover novel loci in two ways. One way is to turn GWAS into Gene-Environment-Wide Interaction Studies (137). Theoretically, the ability to measure GxE interactions should sharpen measurement of gene-disease associations in subsets of the population and even potentially increase statistical power to detect such associations (137). This will become increasingly possible as researchers seek to integrate genome-wide information with information about environmental exposures gathered in the context of epidemiological studies. But sample sizes will become prohibitive when testing gene-environment-wide interactions because 1) more tests are involved, 2) tests for interactions have less power compared to tests for main effects, and 3) environmental exposures introduce additional measurement error. If genetic epidemiologists embrace purely agnostic, theory-free approaches and data-mining tools in studying GxE interactions, the "fishing expedition" may net little. The new generation of purpose-built Gene-Environment-Wide Interaction studies may be an improvement over opportunistic studies published in these early years of GxE research, but even these will fall short unless they attend to the measurement of environmental exposures. An alternative is to pursue study designs that use confirmed environmental effects on disease. Such "exposed-only designs" will test genome-wide associations comparing equally exposed individuals who do versus do not develop a disease in order to discover novel susceptibility loci. Examples of this design can be seen in research on infectious disease, whose starting point is pathogen exposure (138). The environmental risks (i.e., pathogens) for many psychiatric conditions are well established, if not always well measured. As such, the strong prior probabilities for environmental risks can be harnessed in psychiatry to design genome-wide studies focused on identifying genetic differences in responses to well-defined environmental risks. This approach to gene discovery will involve entirely different designs and sampling frames than currently used in case-control studies and biobanks.
A second way in which environmental exposures can be used to discover novel loci is to study gene expression (mRNA levels) as a quantitative phenotype, although attention needs to be paid to tissue informativeness (139). Gene expression profiling offers a powerful tool to identifying genomic responses to the environment by investigating responses to specific, well-operationalized, and reliably measured pathogens and stressors, including exposures to social adversities (140). By assessing genotype effects on gene expression levels (141), polymorphisms in environmentally responsive genes may be identified and then used to study why some people become ill when challenged by the environment and others do not. Incorporating environmental genomics into psychiatry may facilitate identifying susceptibility factors in environmentally induced psychiatric conditions.

Construct Validation Is a Useful Way to Evaluate GxE Research

There are two distinct cultures vying to evaluate the worth of the 5-HTTLPR GxE findings: a purely statistical (theory-free) approach that relies wholly on meta-analysis (142, 143) versus a construct-validity (theory-guided) approach that looks for a nomological network of convergent evidence (this article). The statistical approach is essential for confirming direct genotype-phenotype association discoveries. This approach is driven by the imperative to avoid false positives when evaluating associations sifted from huge amounts of data in theory-free, genome-wide testing with nil prior probability of gene-disease association (144). Naturally, the statistical approach prizes exact replication. In the statistical approach, replication attempts' elements should match the original report's elements, including sample, phenotype, polymorphism, genetic model, and direction of effect. Larger samples are given greater weight in statistical evaluation, because with all other study elements held equal, power is decisive (145).
It is our contention that the purely statistical approach is not sufficient, or necessary, for evaluating research into GxE hypotheses involving candidate genes. In such GxE research, the prior probability of association is far from nil, thus mitigating the risk of false positives. For example, the 5-HTTLPR stress-sensitivity hypothesis was informed by knowledge about the serotonin system's role in depression and the transporter gene's function (1), by inconsistent associations between the 5-HTTLPR and depression suggesting environmental moderation might be operating (146), by evidence that stress causes depression (147), and by initial reports that 5-HTT variation influenced stress reactivity (2, 148, 149). In GxE research, replication attempts' elements need not match those of the original report. GxE research involves not only polymorphism and phenotype, but another element: the environment. Whereas genetic measurements are standard and unchanging across time and across studies and phenotypic measurements can also be standardized to a high degree, environmental exposure measurements vary markedly across studies (150). Two kinds of heterogeneity should be distinguished: heterogeneity in the types of stress exposure versus heterogeneity in the quality of exposure measurements. Regarding exposure types, stressful experiences come in many forms (Table 1 and Table 2) and studies of the 5-HTTLPR GxE have rightly gone beyond the original report to incorporate them. This environmental measurement heterogeneity has implications for matching the genetic model across studies, because the "correct" genetic model could vary depending on severity of the environmental exposure or other factors such as developmental stage and course of illness (e.g., first-onset versus recurrent depression). By insisting that all results must conform to one genetic model, the meta-analysis approach conceals potentially informative patterns, if they exist. Regarding measurement quality, in GxE research it is folly to give greater weight to larger samples, because many large samples are afflicted by poor exposure measurement. Overall, heterogeneity in both the type of stress exposures and in the quality of exposure measurements renders the studies in Table 1 and Table 2 inappropriate for drawing one simple conclusion about statistical replication (145).
Meta-analysis can be a useful tool for interpreting multiple tests of a GxE hypothesis, when best practice is followed. Meta-analyses should table the universe of publications testing the GxE interaction, explaining in a transparent way why each was analyzed or omitted. The subsample analyzed should represent the distribution of positive and negative results in the literature. Metaregression should be undertaken to evaluate methodological sources of variation among findings. Methodological evaluation should be guided by long-established cautions. For example, large samples often suffer poor measurement quality, and large exposure-to-outcome correlations often signal measurement bias, not validity. It should be appreciated that when the sample of studies is small, a statistical test for heterogeneity is underpowered and its nonsignificance does not contraindicate metaregression. If methodological heterogeneity is ruled out, metaregression should investigate substantive sources of variation among findings (e.g., sex, age, exposure severity), and if these are uncovered, variation in genetic model should be considered in relation to the substantive findings. Meta-analyses of the 5-HTTLPR GxE hypothesis have been reported (142, 143), but did not follow best practice (17, 151155).
In any case, whether GxE studies can meet prerequisite standards for statistical meta-analysis is immaterial, since replicating a theory-free association is not the goal. The goal is to evaluate the construct validity of a theory-guided hypothesis (156). In contrast to the statistical approach, the construct validation approach prizes design heterogeneity (although it requires high-quality samples and measures) (157). Construct validation seeks "sturdy" findings (6), defined as results that emerge repeatedly despite variation in sample characteristics, phenotype measurement, and environmental exposure, and that are validated across human epidemiology, experimental neuroscience, and animal models. We have attempted to show that this is the case with evidence for the 5-HTT stress-sensitivity hypothesis.

Public Understanding of Genetic Science

One of GxE research's important contributions is often overlooked by scientists: teaching the falsehood of genetic (and environmental) determinism (158). For over a century the public has been fed a diet of determinism, beginning with early 20th-century eugenics policies to correct all human flaws by culling the breeding stock. Mid-century opinions swung back toward naive environmental determinism, exemplified by B.F. Skinner's 1948 Walden Two. In the late 20th century, public opinion was compelled toward genetic determinism again when high heritability estimates were taken to imply that nongenetic factors have little importance for mental health and behavior. Discoveries of single mutations causing rare disorders strengthened the public's belief that knowing one's genetic makeup is tantamount to knowing one's future. Deterministic beliefs, environmental or genetic, are dangerous. Determinism encourages policies that violate human rights (at worst) and waste resources on ill-conceived mental health improvement programs (at best). Media coverage of this century's new findings of gene-environment interaction (and environmental effects on gene expression) is persuading the public to embrace a more realistic, nuanced understanding of the causes of behavior, in which some genes' effects depend on lifestyle choices that are often under human control. That understanding will be the best defense against misuse of genetic information. Interdependence between life stress and the 5-HTTLPR leads this shift in understanding, because stress and depression touch almost everyone.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Eric Lenze, Richie Poulton, and David Reiss for helpful discussions of the research literature.

Supplementary Material

File (ajp_167_05_509_01.pdf)

References

1.
Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, Benjamin J, Muller CR, Hamer DH, Murphy DL: Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 1996; 274:1527–1531
2.
Hariri AR, Mattay VS, Tessitore A, Kolachana B, Fera F, Goldman D, Egan MF, Weinberger DR: Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science 2002; 297:400–404
3.
Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McClay J, Mill J, Martin J, Braithwaite A, Poulton R: Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science 2003; 301:386–389
4.
Caspi A, Moffitt TE: Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2006; 7:583–590
5.
van Os J, Rutten BP: Gene-environment-wide interaction studies in psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:964–966
6.
Robins LN: Sturdy childhood predictors of adult antisocial behavior: replications from longitudinal studies. Psychol Med 1978; 8:611–622
7.
Monroe SM, Reid MW: Gene-environment interactions in depression research: genetic polymorphisms and life-stress polyprocedures. Psychol Sci 2008; 19:947–956
8.
Brown GW, Harris TO: Depression and the serotonin transporter 5-HTTLPR polymorphism: a review and a hypothesis concerning gene-environment interaction. J Affect Disord 2008; 111:1–12
9.
Monroe SM: Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2008; 4:33–52
10.
Dohrenwend BP: Inventorying stressful life events as risk factors for psychopathology: toward resolution of the problem of intracategory variability. Psychol Bull 2006; 132:477–495
11.
Shanahan MJ, Bauldry S: Improving environmental markers in gene-environment research: insights from life course sociology, in The Dynamic Genome and Mental Health: The Role of Genes and Environments in Development. Edited by, Kendler K, Jaffee S, Romer D. New York, Oxford University Press (in press)
12.
Block J: A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description. Psychol Bull 1995; 117:187
13.
Joormann J, Hertel PT, LeMoult J, Gotlib IH: Training forgetting of negative material in depression. J Abnorm Psychol 2009; 118:34–43
14.
Merikangas KR, Risch N: Gene-environment interactions and depression (Letter). JAMA 2009; 302:1861–1862
15.
Gillespie NA, Whitfield JB, Williams B, Heath AC, Martin NG: The relationship between stressful life events, the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and major depression. Psychol Med 2005; 35:101–111
16.
Uher R, McGuffin P: The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental adversity in the aetiology of mental illness: review and methodological analysis. Mol Psychiatry 2008; 13:131–146
17.
Uher R, McGuffin P: The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental adversity in the aetiology of mental illness: 2009 update. Mol Psychiatry 2010; 15:18–22
18.
Xie P, Kranzler H, Poling J, Stein M, Anton R, Brady K, Weiss R, Farrer L, Gelernter J: The interactive effect of stressful life events and serotonin transporter 5-HTTLPR genotype on PTSD diagnosis in two independent populations. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:1201–1209
19.
Roy A, Hu XZ, Janal MN, Goldman D: Interaction between childhood trauma and serotonin transporter gene variation in suicide. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007; 32:2046–2052
20.
Verona E, Joiner TE, Johnson F, Bender TW: Gender specific gene-environment interactions on laboratory-assessed aggression. Biol Psychol 2006; 71:33–41
21.
Covault J, Tennen H, Armeli S, Conner TS, Herman AI, Cillessen AH, Kranzler HR: Interactive effects of the serotonin transporter 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and stressful life events on college student drinking and drug use. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:609–616
22.
Barr CS, Newman TK, Lindell S, Shannon C, Champoux M, Lesch KP, Suomi SJ, Goldman D, Higley JD: Interaction between serotonin transporter gene variation and rearing condition in alcohol preference and consumption in female primates. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:1146–1152
23.
Brody GH, Beach SR, Philibert RA, Chen YF, Lei MK, Murry VM, Brown AC: Parenting moderates a genetic vulnerability factor in longitudinal increases in youths' substance use. J Consult Clin Psychol 2009; 77:1–11
24.
Brody GH, Beach SR, Philibert RA, Chen YF, Murry VM: Prevention effects moderate the association of 5-HTTLPR and youth risk behavior initiation: gene & environment hypotheses tested via a randomized prevention design. Child Dev 2009; 80:645–661
25.
Brummett BH, Krystal AD, Ashley-Koch A, Kuhn CM, Zuchner S, Siegler IC, Barefoot JC, Ballard EL, Gwyther LP, Williams RB: Sleep quality varies as a function of 5-HTTLPR genotype and stress. Psychosom Med 2007; 69:621–624
26.
Safarinejad MR: Polymorphisms of the serotonin transporter gene and their relation to premature ejaculation in individuals from Iran. J Urol 2009; 181:2656–2661
27.
Stein MB, Schork NJ, Gelernter J: Gene-by-environment (serotonin transporter and childhood maltreatment) interaction for anxiety sensitivity, an intermediate phenotype for anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 33:312–319
28.
Williams LM, Gatt JM, Schofield PR, Olivieri G, Peduto A, Gordon E: "Negativity bias" in risk for depression and anxiety: brain-body fear circuitry correlates, 5-HTT-LPR and early life stress. Neuroimage 2009; 47:804–814
29.
Fox NA, Nichols KE, Henderson HA, Rubin K, Schmidt L, Hamer D, Ernst M, Pine DS: Evidence for a gene-environment interaction in predicting behavioral inhibition in middle childhood. Psychol Sci 2005; 16:921–926
30.
Barry RA, Kochanska G, Philibert RA: G × E interaction in the organization of attachment: mothers' responsiveness as a moderator of children's genotypes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49:1313–1320
31.
Kochanska G, Philibert RA, Barry RA: Interplay of genes and early mother and child relationship in the development of self-regulation from toddler to preschool age. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50:1331–1338
32.
Pauli-Pott U, Friedl S, Hinney A, Hebebrand J: Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR), environmental conditions, and developing negative emotionality and fear in early childhood. J Neural Transm 2009; 116:503–512
33.
Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Newman DL, Silva PA: Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders: longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:1033–1039
34.
Gunthert KC, Conner TS, Armeli S, Tennen H, Covault J, Kranzler HR: Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety reactivity in daily life: A daily process approach to gene-environment interaction. Psychosom Med 2007; 69:762–768
35.
Gilbert DG, Zuo Y, Rabinovich NE, Riise H, Needham R, Huggenvik JI: Neurotransmission-related genetic polymorphisms, negative affectivity traits, and gender predict tobacco abstinence symptoms across 44 days with and without nicotine patch. J Abnorm Psychol 2009; 118:322–334
36.
Jacobs N, Kenis G, Peeters F, Derom C, Vlietinck R, van Os J: Stress-related negative affectivity and genetically altered serotonin transporter function: evidence of synergism in shaping risk of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:989–996
37.
Krueger RF, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Silva PA, McGee R: Personality traits are differentially linked to mental disorders: a multitrait-multidiagnosis study of an adolescent birth cohort. J Abnorm Psychol 1996; 105:299–312
38.
Watson D, Clark LA: Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience aversive emotional states. Psychol Bull 1984; 96:465–490
39.
Furmark T, Tillfors M, Garpenstrand H, Marteinsdottir I, Långström B, Oreland L, Fredrikson M: Serotonin transporter polymorphism related to amygdala excitability and symptom severity in patients with social phobia. Neuroscience Letters 2004; 362:189–192
40.
Heinz A, Braus DF, Smolka MN, Wrase J, Puls I, Hermann D, Klein S, Grusser SM, Flor H, Schumann G, Mann K, Buchel C: Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic variation of the serotonin transporter. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8:20–21
41.
Bertolino A, Arciero G, Rubino V, Latorre V, De Candia M, Mazzola V, Blasi G, Caforio G, Hariri A, Kolachana B, Nardini M, Weinberger DR, Scarabino T: Variation of human amygdala response during threatening stimuli as a function of 5-HTTLPR genotype and personality style. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:1517–1525
42.
Canli T, Omura K, Haas BW, Fallgatter A, Constable RT, Lesch KP: Beyond affect: a role for genetic variation of the serotonin transporter in neural activation during a cognitive attention task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005; 102:12224–12229
43.
Hariri AR, Drabant EM, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Mattay VS, Egan MF, Weinberger DR: A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:146–152
44.
Brown SM, Hariri AR: Neuroimaging studies of serotonin gene polymorphisms: exploring the interplay of genes, brain, and behavior. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2006; 6:44–52
45.
Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, Kugel H, Baune BT, Hohoff C, Kersting A, Arolt V, Heindel W, Deckert J, Suslow T: Serotonergic genes modulate amygdala activity in major depression. Genes Brain Behav 2007; 6:672–676
46.
Domschke K, Braun M, Ohrmann P, Suslow T, Kugel H, Bauer J, Hohoff C, Kersting A, Engelien A, Arolt V, Heindel W, Deckert J: Association of the functional -1019C/G 5-HT1A polymorphism with prefrontal cortex and amygdala activation measured with 3 T fMRI in panic disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2006; 9:349–355
47.
Heinz A, Smolka MN, Braus DF, Wrase J, Beck A, Flor H, Mann K, Schumann G, Büchel C, Hariri AR, Weinberger DR: Serotonin transporter genotype (5-HTTLPR): effects of neutral and undefined conditions on amygdala activation. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:1011–1014
48.
Dannlowski U, Ohrmann P, Bauer J, Deckert J, Hohoff C, Kugel H, Arolt V, Heindel W, Kersting A, Baune BT, Suslow T: 5-HTTLPR biases amygdala activity in response to masked facial expressions in major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007; 33:418–424
49.
Smolka MN, Buhler M, Schumann G, Klein S, Hu XZ, Moayer M, Zimmer A, Wrase J, Flor H, Mann K, Braus DF, Goldman D, Heinz A: Gene-gene effects on central processing of aversive stimuli. Mol Psychiatry 2007; 12:307–317
50.
Furman D, Hamilton P, Joormann J, Gotlib I: Altered timing of amygdala activation during sad mood elaboration as a function of 5-HTTLPR. Biol Psychiatry (in press)
51.
Viviani R, Sim E, Lo H, Beschoner P, Osterfeld N, Maier C, Seeringer A, Godoy A, Rosa A, Comas D, Kirchheiner J: Baseline brain perfusion and the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism. Biol Psychiatry (in press)
52.
Canli T, Qiu M, Omura K, Congdon E, Haas BW, Amin Z, Herrmann MJ, Constable RT, Lesch KP: Neural correlates of epigenesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006; 103:16033–16038
53.
Rao H, Gillihan SJ, Wang J, Korczykowski M, Sankoorikal GM, Kaercher KA, Brodkin ES, Detre JA, Farah MJ: Genetic variation in serotonin transporter alters resting brain function in healthy individuals. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:600–606
54.
Whalen PJ, Phelps EA(eds): The Human Amygdala. New York, The Guilford Press, 2009
55.
Hariri AR: The neurobiology of individual differences in complex behavioral traits. Annu Rev Neurosci 2009; 32:225–247
56.
Hariri AR, Holmes A: Genetics of emotional regulation: the role of the serotonin transporter in neural function. Trends Cogn Sci 2006; 10:182–191
57.
Pezawas L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Drabant EM, Verchinski BA, Munoz KE, Kolachana BS, Egan MF, Mattay VS, Hariri AR, Weinberger DR: 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat Neurosci 2005; 8:828–834
58.
Pacheco J, Beevers CG, Benavides C, McGeary J, Stice E, Schnyer DM: Frontal-limbic white matter pathway associations with the serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism. J Neurosci 2009; 29:6229–6233
59.
Kim MJ, Whalen PJ: The structural integrity of an amygdala-prefrontal pathway predicts trait anxiety. J Neurosci 2009; 29:11614–11618
60.
Young KA, Holcomb LA, Bonkale WL, Hicks PB, Yazdani U, German DC: 5HTTLPR polymorphism and enlargement of the pulvinar: unlocking the backdoor to the limbic system. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:813–818
61.
Hensch T, Wargelius HL, Herold U, Lesch KP, Oreland L, Brocke B: Further evidence for an association of 5-HTTLPR with intensity dependence of auditory-evoked potentials. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006; 31:2047–2054
62.
Battaglia M, Ogliari A, Zanoni A, Citterio A, Pozzoli U, Giorda R, Maffei C, Marino C: Influence of the serotonin transporter promoter gene and shyness on children's cerebral responses to facial expressions. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:85–94
63.
Fallgatter AJ, Herrmann MJ, Roemmler J, Ehlis AC, Wagener A, Heidrich A, Ortega G, Zeng Y, Lesch KP: Allelic variation of serotonin transporter function modulates the brain electrical response for error processing. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29:1506–1511
64.
Gallinat J, Senkowski D, Wernicke C, Juckel G, Becker I, Sander T, Smolka M, Hegerl U, Rommelspacher H, Winterer G, Herrmann WM: Allelic variants of the functional promoter polymorphism of the human serotonin transporter gene is associated with auditory cortical stimulus processing. Neuropsychopharmacology 2003; 28:530–532
65.
Strobel A, Debener S, Schmidt D, Hünnerkopf R, Lesch KP, Brocke B: Allelic variation in serotonin transporter function associated with the intensity dependence of the auditory evoked potential. Am J Med Genet 2003; 118B:41–47
66.
Fallgatter AJ, Jatzke S, Bartsch AJ, Hamelbeck B, Lesch KP: Serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism influences topography of inhibitory motor control. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 1999; 2:115–120
67.
Lonsdorf TB, Weike AI, Nikamo P, Schalling M, Hamm AO, Öhman A: Genetic gating of human fear learning and extinction: possible implications for gene-environment interaction in anxiety disorder. Psychol Sci 2009; 20:198–206
68.
Armbruster D, Moser DA, Strobel A, Hensch T, Kirschbaum C, Lesch KP, Brocke B: Serotonin transporter gene variation and stressful life events impact processing of fear and anxiety. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009; 12:393–401
69.
Brocke B, Armbruster D, Muller J, Hensch T, Jacob CP, Lesch KP, Kirschbaum C, Strobel A: Serotonin transporter gene variation impacts innate fear processing: acoustic startle response and emotional startle. Mol Psychiatry 2006; 11:1106–1112
70.
Crisan LG, Pana S, Vulturar R, Heilman RM, Szekely R, Druga B, Dragos N, Miu AC: Genetic contributions of the serotonin transporter to social learning of fear and economic decision making. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2009; 4:399-408
71.
Way B, Taylor S: The serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) is associated with cortisol response to psychosocial stress. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67:487–492
72.
Gotlib IH, Joormann J, Minor KL, Hallmayer J: HPA axis reactivity: a mechanism underlying the associations among 5-HTTLPR, stress, and depression. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 63:847–851
73.
Alexander N, Kuepper Y, Schmitz A, Osinsky R, Kozyra E, Hennig J: Gene-environment interactions predict cortisol responses after acute stress: implications for the etiology of depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34:1294–1303
74.
Mueller A, Brocke B, Fries E, Lesch KP, Kirschbaum C: The role of the serotonin transporter polymorphism for the endocrine stress response in newborns. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35:289–296
75.
Beevers CG, Gibb BE, McGeary JE, Miller IW: Serotonin transporter genetic variation and biased attention for emotional word stimuli among psychiatric inpatients. J Abnorm Psychol 2007; 116:208–212
76.
Osinsky R, Reuter M, Kapper Y, Schmitz A, Kozyra E, Alexander N, Hennig J: Variation in the serotonin transporter gene modulates selective attention to threat. Emotion 2008; 8:584–588
77.
Beevers CG, Wells TT, Ellis AJ, McGeary JE: Association of the serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism with biased attention for emotional stimuli. J Abnorm Psychol 2009; 118:670–681
78.
Gibb BE, Benas JS, Grassia M, McGeary J: Children's attentional biases and 5-HTTLPR genotype: potential mechanisms linking mother and child depression. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2009; 38:415–426
79.
Fox E, Ridgewell A, Ashwin C: Looking on the bright side: biased attention and the human serotonin transporter gene. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2009; 276:1747–1751
80.
Hayden EP, Dougherty LR, Maloney B, Olino TM, Sheikh H, Durbin CE, Nurnberger JI, Lahiri DK, Klein DN: Early-emerging cognitive vulnerability to depression and the serotonin transporter promoter region polymorphism. J Affect Disord 2008; 107:227–230
81.
Strange BA, Kroes MC, Roiser JP, Tan GC, Dolan RJ: Emotion-induced retrograde amnesia is determined by a 5-HTT genetic polymorphism. J Neurosci 2008; 28:7036–7039
82.
Roiser JP, de Martino B, Tan GCY, Kumaran D, Seymour B, Wood NW, Dolan RJ: A genetically mediated bias in decision making driven by failure of amygdala control. J Neuroscience 2009; 29:5985–5991
83.
Domschke K, Stevens S, Beck B, Baffa A, Hohoff C, Deckert J, Gerlach A: Blushing propensity in social anxiety disorder: influence of serotonin transporter gene variation. J Neural Transm 2009; 116:663–666
84.
Barr CS, Newman TK, Becker ML, Parker CC, Champoux M, Lesch KP, Goldman D, Suomi SJ, Higley JD: The utility of the non-human primate model for studying gene by environment interactions in behavioral research. Genes Brain Behav 2003; 2:336–340
85.
Spinelli S, Schwandt M, Lindell S, Newman TK, Heilig M, Suomi SJ, Higley JD, Goldman D, Barr CS: Association between the recombinant human serotonin transporter linked promoter region polymorphism and behavior in rhesus macaques during a separation paradigm. Dev Psychopathol 2007; 19:977–987
86.
Barr CS, Newman TK, Shannon C, Parker C, Dvoskin RL, Becker ML, Schwandt M, Champoux M, Lesch KP, Goldman D, Suomi SJ, Higley JD: Rearing condition and rh5-HTTLPR interact to influence limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis response to stress in infant macaques. Biol Psychiatry 2004; 55:733–738
87.
Barr CS, Newman TK, Schwandt M, Shannon C, Dvoskin RL, Lindell S, Taubman J, Thompson B, Champoux M, Lesch KP, Goldman D, Suomi SJ, Higley JD: Sexual dichotomy of an interaction between early adversity and the serotonin transporter gene promoter variant in rhesus macaques. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2004; 101:12358–12363
88.
Jedema HP, Gianaros PJ, Greer PJ, Kerr DD, Liu S, Higley JD, Suomi SJ, Olsen AS, Porter JN, Lopresti BJ, Hariri AR, Bradberry CW: Cognitive impact of genetic variation of the serotonin transporter in primates is associated with differences in brain morphology rather than serotonin neurotransmission. Mol Psychiatry 2009 (in press)
89.
Kalin NH, Shelton SE, Fox AS, Rogers J, Oakes TR, Davidson RJ: The serotonin transporter genotype is associated with intermediate brain phenotypes that depend on the context of eliciting stressor. Mol Psychiatry 2008; 13:1021–1027
90.
Watson K, Ghodasra J, Platt M: Serotonin transporter genotype modulates social reward and punishment in rhesus macaques. PLoS One 2009; 4:e4156
91.
Brigman J, Mathur P, Harvey-White J, Izquierdo A, Saksida L, Bussey T, Fox S, Deneris E, Murphy D, Holmes A: Pharmacological or genetic inactivation of the serotonin transporter improves reversal learning in mice. Cereb Cortex (in press)
92.
Schoenbaum G, Shaham Y: The role of orbitofrontal cortex in drug addiction: a review of preclinical studies. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 63:256–262
93.
Cryan J, Holmes A: The ascent of mouse: advances in modelling human depression and anxiety. Nat Rev Drug Discov 2005; 4:775–790
94.
Carneiro A, Airey D, Thompson B, Zhu C, Lu L, Chesler E, Erikson K, Blakely R: Functional coding variation in recombinant inbred mouse lines reveals multiple serotonin transporter-associated phenotypes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009; 106:2047–2052
95.
Murphy DL, Lesch KP: Targeting the murine serotonin transporter: insights into human neurobiology. Nat Rev Neurosci 2008; 9:85–96
96.
Homberg J, Olivier J, Smits B, Mul J, Mudde J, Verheul M, Nieuwenhuizen O, Cools A, Ronken E, Cremers T, Schoffelmeer A, Ellenbroek B, Cuppen E: Characterization of the serotonin transporter knockout rat: a selective change in the functioning of the serotonergic system. Neuroscience 2007; 146:1662–1672
97.
Jennings K, Loder M, Sheward W, Pei Q, Deacon R, Benson M, Olveerman H, Hastie N, Harmar A, Shen S, Sharp T: Increased expression of the 5-HT transporter confers a low-anxiety phenotype linked to decreased 5-HT transmission. J Neurosci 2006; 26:8955–8964
98.
Torres G, Amara S: Glutamate and monoamine transporter: new visions of form and function. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2007; 17:8955–8964
99.
Christian BT, Fox AS, Oler JA, Vandehey NT, Murali D, Rogers J, Oakes TR, Shelton SE, Davidson RJ, Kalin NH: Serotonin transporter binding and genotype in the nonhuman primate brain using [C-11]DASB PET. NeuroImage 2009; 47:1230–1236
100.
Heinz A, Jones DW, Mazzanti C, Goldman D, Ragan P, Hommer D, Linnoila M, Weinberger DR: A relationship between serotonin transporter genotype and in vivo protein expression and alcohol neurotoxicity. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 47:643–649
101.
Shioe K, Ichimiya T, Suhara T, Takano A, Sudo Y, Yasuno F, Hirano M, Shinohara M, Kagami M, Okubo Y, Nankai M, Kanba S: No association between genotype of the promoter region of serotonin transporter gene and serotonin transporter binding in human brain measured by PET. Synapse 2003; 48:184–188
102.
David SP, Murthy NV, Rabiner EA, Munafo MR, Johnstone EC, Jacob R, Walton RT, Grasby PM: A functional genetic variation of the serotonin (5-HT) transporter affects 5-HT1A receptor binding in humans. J Neurosci 2005; 25:2586–2590
103.
Lee M, Bailer UF, Frank GK, Henry SE, Meltzer CC, Price JC, Mathis CA, Putnam KT, Ferrell RE, Hariri AR, Kaye WH: Relationship of a 5-HT transporter functional polymorphism to 5-HT1A receptor binding in healthy women. Mol Psychiatry 2005; 10:715–716
104.
Reimold M, Smolka MN, Schumann G, Zimmer A, Wrase J, Mann K, Hu XZ, Goldman D, Reischl G, Solbach C, Machulla HJ, Bares R, Heinz A: Midbrain serotonin transporter binding potential measured with [11C]DASB is affected by serotonin transporter genotype. J Neural Transm 2007; 114:635–639
105.
Parsey RV, Hastings RS, Oquendo MA, Hu X, Goldman D, Huang Y-y, Simpson N, Arcement J, Huang Y: Effect of a triallelic functional polymorphism of the serotonin-transporter-linked promoter region on expression of serotonin transporter in the human brain. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:48–51
106.
Esaki T, Cook M, Shimoji K, Murphy DL, Sokoloff L, Holmes A: Developmental disruption of serotonin transporter function impairs cerebral responses to whisker stimulation in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005; 102:5582–5587
107.
Gaspar P, Cases O, Maroteaux L: The developmental role of serotonin: news from mouse molecular genetics. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003; 4:1002–1012
108.
Ansorge M, Zhou M, Lira A, Hen R, Gingrich J: Early-life blockade of 5-HT transporter alters emotional behavior in adult mice. Science 2004; 306:879–881
109.
Carola V, Frazzetto G, Pascucci T, Audero E, Puglisi-Allegra S, Cabib S, Lesch KP, Gross C: Identifying molecular substrates in a mouse model of the serotonin transporter × environment risk factor for anxiety and depression. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 63:840–846
110.
Burton PR, Hansell AL, Fortier I, Manolio TA, Khoury MJ, Little J, Elliott P: Size matters: just how big is BIG?: quantifying realistic sample size requirements for human genome epidemiology. Int J Epidemiol 2009; 38:263–273
111.
McClelland GH, Judd CM: Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychol Bull 1993; 114:376
112.
Rutter M, Thapar A, Pickles A: Gene-environment interactions: biologically valid pathway or artifact? Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:1287–1289
113.
Martinez FD: Gene-environment interactions in asthma. Proc Am Thorac Soc 2007; 4:26–31
114.
Ordovas J, Tai ES: Why study gene-enviroment interactions? Curr Opin Lipidol 2008; 19:158–167
115.
Sen S, Burmeister M, Ghosh D: Meta-analysis of the association between a serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits. Am J Med Genet 2004; 127B:85–89
116.
Lasky-Su JA, Faraone SV, Glatt SJ, Tsuang MT: Meta-analysis of the association between two polymorphisms in the serotonin transporter gene and affective disorders. Am J Med Genet 2005; 133B:110–115
117.
Hoefgen B, Schulze TG, Ohlraun S, von Widdern O, Höfels S, Gross M, Heidmann V, Kovalenko S, Eckermann A, Kölsch H, Metten M, Zobel A, Becker T, Nöthen MM, Propping P, Heun R, Maier W, Rietschel M: The power of sample size and homogenous sampling: association between the 5-HTTLPR serotonin transporter polymorphism and major depressive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57:247–251
118.
Committee PGCS: A framework for interpreting genome-wide association studies of psychiatric disorders. Mol Psychiatry 2008; 14:10–17
119.
Frayling TM, Timpson NJ, Weedon MN, Zeggini E, Freathy RM, Lindgren CM, Perry JR, Elliott KS, Lango H, Rayner NW, Shields B, Harries LW, Barrett JC, Ellard S, Groves CJ, Knight B, Patch AM, Ness AR, Ebrahim S, Lawlor DA, Ring SM, Ben-Shlomo Y, Jarvelin MR, Sovio U, Bennett AJ, Melzer D, Ferrucci L, Loos RJ, Barroso I, Wareham NJ, Karpe F, Owen KR, Cardon LR, Walker M, Hitman GA, Palmer CN, Doney AS, Morris AD, Smith GD, Hattersley AT, McCarthy MI: A common variant in the FTO gene is associated with body mass index and predisposes to childhood and adult obesity. Science 2007; 316:889–894
120.
Cecil JE, Tavendale R, Watt P, Hetherington MM, Palmer CN: An obesity-associated FTO gene variant and increased energy intake in children. N Engl J Med 2008; 359:2558–2566
121.
Gerken T, Girard CA, Tung YC, Webby CJ, Saudek V, Hewitson KS, Yeo GS, McDonough MA, Cunliffe S, McNeill LA, Galvanovskis J, Rorsman P, Robins P, Prieur X, Coll AP, Ma M, Jovanovic Z, Farooqi IS, Sedgwick B, Barroso I, Lindahl T, Ponting CP, Ashcroft FM, O'Rahilly S, Schofield CJ: The obesity-associated FTO gene encodes a 2-oxoglutarate-dependent nucleic acid demethylase. Science 2007; 318:1469–1472
122.
Fredriksson R, Hagglund M, Olszewski PK, Stephansson O, Jacobsson JA, Olszewska AM, Levine AS, Lindblom J, Schioth HB: The obesity gene, FTO, is of ancient origin, up-regulated during food deprivation and expressed in neurons of feeding-related nuclei of the brain. Endocrinology 2008; 149:2062–2071
123.
Wardle J, Carnell S, Haworth CM, Farooqi IS, O'Rahilly S, Plomin R: Obesity associated genetic variation in FTO is associated with diminished satiety. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2008; 93:3640–3643
124.
Rampersaud E, Mitchell BD, Pollin TI, Fu M, Shen H, O'Connell JR, Ducharme JL, Hines S, Sack P, Naglieri R, Shuldiner AR, Snitker S: Physical activity and the association of common FTO gene variants with body mass index and obesity. Arch Intern Med 2008; 168:1791–1797
125.
Andreasen CH, Stender-Petersen KL, Mogensen MS, Torekov SS, Wegner L, Andersen G, Nielsen AL, Albrechtsen A, Borch-Johnsen K, Rasmussen SS, Clausen JO, Sandbak A, Lauritzen T, Hansen L, Jargensen T, Pedersen O, Hansen T: Low physical activity accentuates the effect of the FTO rs9939609 polymorphism on body fat accumulation. Diabetes 2008; 57:95–101
126.
Vimaleswaran KS, Li S, Zhao JH, Luan JA, Bingham SA, Khaw KT, Ekelund U, Wareham NJ, Loos RJ: Physical activity attenuates the body mass index-increasing influence of genetic variation in the FTO gene. Am J Clin Nutr 2009; 90:425–428
127.
Ruiz JR, Labayen I, Ortega FB, Legry V, Moreno LA, Dallongeville J, Martínez-Gómez D, Bokor S, Manios Y, Ciarapica D, Gottrand F, Molnar SH, Sjöström M, Meirhaeghe A: Physical activity attenuates the effect of the FTO rs9939609 polymorphism on total and central body fat in adolescents: the HELENA Study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med (in press)
128.
Hakanen M, Raitakari OT, Lehtimaki T, Peltonen N, Pahkala K, Sillanmaki L, Lagstrom H, Viikari J, Simell O, Ronnemaa T: FTO genotype is associated with body mass index after the age of seven years but not with energy intake or leisure-time physical activity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2009; 94:1281–1287
129.
Jonsson A, Renström F, Lyssenko V, Brito E, Isomaa B, Berglund G, Nilsson P, Groop L, Franks P: Assessing the effect of interaction between an FTO variant (rs9939609) and physical activity on obesity in 15,925 Swedish and 2,511 Finnish adults. Diabetologia 2009; 52:1334–1338
130.
Rutter M(Ed): Genetic Effects on Environmental Vulnerability to Disease. Chichester, England, John Wiley & Sons, 2008
131.
Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M: Strategy for investigating interactions between measured genes and measured environments. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:473–481
132.
Bradley RG, Binder EB, Epstein MP, Tang Y, Nair HP, Liu W, Gillespie CF, Berg T, Evces M, Newport DJ, Stowe ZN, Heim CM, Nemeroff CB, Schwartz A, Cubells JF, Ressler KJ: Influence of child abuse on adult depression: moderation by the corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor gene. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65:190–200
133.
Nicodemus KK, Marenco S, Batten AJ, Vakkalanka R, Egan MF, Straub RE, Weinberger DR: Serious obstetric complications interact with hypoxia-regulated/vascular-expression genes to influence schizophrenia risk. Mol Psychiatry 2008; 13:873–877
134.
Caspi A, Williams B, Kim-Cohen J, Craig IW, Milne BJ, Poulton R, Schalkwyk LC, Taylor A, Werts H, Moffitt TE: Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007; 104:18860–18865
135.
Hopkins MR, Ettinger AS, Hernández-Avila M, Schwartz J, Téllez-Rojo MM, Lamadrid-Figueroa H, Bellinger D, Hu H, Wright RO: Variants in iron metabolism genes predict higher blood lead levels in young children. Environ Health Perspect 2008; 116:1261–1266
136.
Ross CJ, Katzov-Eckert H, Dube MP, Brooks B, Rassekh SR, Barhdadi A, Feroz-Zada Y, Visscher H, Brown AM, Rieder MJ, Rogers PC, Phillips MS, Carleton BC, Hayden MR: Genetic variants in TPMT and COMT are associated with hearing loss in children receiving cisplatin chemotherapy. Nat Genet 2009; 41:1345–1349
137.
Khoury MJ, Wacholder S: Invited commentary: from genome-wide association studies to gene-environment-wide interaction studies: challenges and opportunities. Am J Epidemiol 2009; 169:227–230
138.
Kotb M, Norrby-Teglund A, McGeer A, El-Sherbini H, Dorak ST, Khurshid A, Green K, Peeples J, Wade J, Thomson G, Schwartz B, Low DE: An immunogenetic and molecular basis for differences in outcomes of invasive group A streptococcal infections. Nat Med 2002; 8:1398–1404
139.
Dimas AS, Deutsch S, Stranger BE, Montgomery SB, Borel C, Attar-Cohen H, Ingle C, Beazley C, Arcelus MG, Sekowska M, Gagnebin M, Nisbett J, Deloukas P, Dermitzakis ET, Antonarakis SE: Common regulatory variation impacts gene expression in a cell type-dependent manner. Science 2009; 325:1246–1250
140.
Miller GE, Chen E, Fok AK, Walker H, Lim A, Nicholls EF, Cole S, Kobor MS: Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2009; 106:14716–12721
141.
Cheung VG, Spielman RS: Genetics of human gene expression: mapping DNA variants that influence gene expression. Nat Rev Genet 2009; 10:595–604
142.
Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang KY, Eaves L, Hoh J, Griem A, Kovacs M, Ott J, Merikangas KR: Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: a meta-analysis. JAMA 2009; 301:2462–2471
143.
Munafò MR, Durrant C, Lewis G, Flint J: Gene × environment interactions at the serotonin transporter locus. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 65:211–219
144.
Moonesinghe R, Khoury MJ, Liu T, Ioannidis JP: Required sample size and nonreplicability thresholds for heterogeneous genetic associations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008; 105:617–622
145.
Chanock S, Manolio T, Boehnke M, Boerwinkle E, Hunter D, Thomas G, Hirschhorn J, Abecasis G, Altshuler D, Bailey-Wilson J, Brooks L, Cardon L, Daly M, Donnelly P, Fraumeni JJ, Freimer N, Gerhard D, Gunter C, Guttmacher A, Guyer M, Harris E, Hoh J, Hoover R, Kong C, Merikangas K, Morton C, Palmer L, Phimister E, Rice J, Roberts J, Rotimi C, Tucker M, Vogan K, Wacholder S, Wijsman E, Winn D, Collins F: Replicating genotype-phenotype associations. Nature 2007; 447:655–660
146.
Lesch KP: Neuroticism and serotonin: a developmental genetic perspective, in Behavioral Genetics in the Postgenomic Era. Edited by, Plomin R, DeFries J, Craig I, McGuffin P. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2003
147.
Monroe SM, Simons AD: Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychol Bull 1991; 110:406–425
148.
Bennett AJ, Lesch KP, Heils A, Long JC, Lorenz JG, Shoaf SE, Champoux M, Suomi SJ, Linnoila MV, Higley JD: Early experience and serotonin transporter gene variation interact to influence primates CNS function. Mol Psychiatry 2002; 7:118–122
149.
Murphy DL, Li Q, Engel S, Wichems C, Andrews A, Lesch KP, Uhl G: Genetic perspectives on the serotonin transporter. Brain Res Bull 2001; 56:487–494
150.
Ioannidis JP, Loy EY, Poulton R, Chia KS: Researching genetic versus nongenetic determinants of disease: a comparison and proposed unification. Science Translational Med 2009; 1:1–6
151.
Kaufman J, Gelernter J, Kaffman A, Caspi A, Moffitt TE: Arguable assumptions, debatable conclusions (letter). Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67:e19–e20
152.
Koenen KC, Galea S: Gene-environment interactions and depression (letter). JAMA 2009; 302:1859
153.
Lotrich FE, Lenze E: Gene-environment interactions and depression (letter). JAMA 2009; 302:1860
154.
Rutter M: Commentary in 'Cutting Edge' series of 'Depression and Anxiety' gene-environment interplay. Depress Anxiety 2010; 27:1–4
155.
Schwahn C, Grabe HJ: Gene-environment interactions and depression (letter). JAMA 2009; 302:1860
156.
Strauss ME, Smith GT: Construct validity: advances in theory and methodology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2009; 5:1–25
157.
Vandenbroucke JP: Observational research, randomised trials, and two views of medical science. PLoS Med 2008; 5:e67
158.
Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M: Measured gene-environment interactions in psychopathology. Perspect Psychol Sci 2006; 1:5–27

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 509 - 527
PubMed: 20231323

History

Received: 12 October 2008
Accepted: 5 January 2010
Published online: 1 May 2010
Published in print: May 2010

Authors

Details

Rudolf Uher, Ph.D., M.R.C.Psych.
Terrie E. Moffitt, Ph.D.

Notes

Received Oct. 12, 2009; revisions received Dec. 18 and Dec. 29, 2009; accepted Jan. 5, 2010. From the Departments of Psychology & Neuroscience, Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, and Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University; the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London; and the Section on Behavioral Science and Genetics, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, NIH. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Caspi, Duke University, 2020 West Main Street, Suite 201, Box 104410, Durham, NC 27708; [email protected] (e-mail).

Competing Interests

Drs. Caspi and Moffitt, through the Wisconsin Alumni Research Foundation, have applied for a patent entitled "Method for Assessing a Behavioral Disposition" (U.S. Patent Office serial number 10/889,450). The remaining authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

Supported by grants from the U.K. Medical Research Council (G0100527, G0601483), National Institute on Aging (AG032282), NIMH (MH077874, MH072837), NICHD (HD061298), and Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Avshalom Caspi is a Royal Society-Wolfson Merit Award holder.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share