Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective:

Based on evidence linking creativity and bipolar disorder, a model has been proposed whereby factors influencing liability to bipolar disorder confer certain traits with positive effects on reproductive fitness. The authors tested this model by examining key traits known to be associated with evolutionary fitness, namely, temperament and neurocognition, in individuals carrying liability for bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia probands and their co-twins were included as psychiatric controls.

Method:

Twin pairs discordant for bipolar disorder and schizophrenia and control pairs were identified through the Swedish Twin Registry. The authors administered a neuropsychological test battery and temperament questionnaires to samples of bipolar probands, bipolar co-twins, schizophrenia probands, schizophrenia co-twins, and controls. Multivariate mixed-model analyses of variance were conducted to compare groups on temperament and neurocognitive scores.

Results:

Bipolar co-twins showed elevated scores on a “positivity” temperament scale compared with controls and bipolar probands, while bipolar probands scored higher on a “negativity” scale compared with their co-twins and controls, who did not differ. Additionally, bipolar co-twins showed superior performance compared with controls on tests of verbal learning and fluency, while bipolar probands showed performance decrements across all neurocognitive domains. In contrast, schizophrenia co-twins showed attenuated impairments in positivity and overall neurocognitive functioning relative to their ill proband counterparts.

Conclusions:

These findings suggest that supra-normal levels of sociability and verbal functioning may be associated with liability for bipolar disorder. These effects were specific to liability for bipolar disorder and did not apply to schizophrenia. Such benefits may provide a partial explanation for the persistence of bipolar illness in the population.
Bipolar disorder affects approximately 1% of the population worldwide, with little change in prevalence across generations (1) despite substantial heritability (estimated to be between 59% and 85%) (2, 3). This persistence appears paradoxical given the expected evolutionary impact of illnesses that affect brain function. For example, in schizophrenia, based on evidence of reduced fecundity before and after illness onset (4), it is thought that persistence of the disorder at high prevalence is a function of relatively low selection pressure against any one liability-promoting mutation combined with the constant introduction of new liability-promoting mutations (5), an inheritance model known as polygenic selection-mutation balance. However, the fertility disadvantage in bipolar disorder is less clear, with observations of reduced fecundity in clinic-derived samples (6), minimal to no differences in population-based patient samples (7, 8), and increased fertility in nonaffected siblings of bipolar probands (7). One explanation, based on the anecdotal link between bipolar disorder and creativity (9), posits that certain traits associated with liability to this syndrome may confer reproductive advantages. If liability to bipolar disorder is associated with creativity and other traits thought to be advantageous in mating selection, then the fertility disadvantage would be expected to be minimal or absent. In this study, our aim was to evaluate fitness-related features in individuals carrying liability to bipolar illness in order to examine possible mechanisms for an associated reproductive advantage in contrast to the known disadvantage associated with schizophrenia.
Evaluation of fitness-related traits in the unaffected family members of bipolar probands provides a basis for examining this model empirically. On average, such individuals carry higher degrees of liability (from both predisposing genes and environmental risk exposures) for disorder than individuals in the general population (5). However, because unaffected family members do not have bipolar disorder themselves, measurement of liability-related traits is not complicated by secondary phenomena such as the effects of illness chronicity and exposure to psychotropic medications. Fitness-related traits of potential relevance include positive aspects of temperament (e.g., sociability and positive affect) and higher intellectual abilities. These traits have been theoretically and anecdotally related to bipolar disorder (913), are substantially heritable (14, 15), and are linked to increased reproductive success in the general population (16, 17). Such inquiry is aligned with growing interest in the study of positive virtues, character strengths, and associated well-being and mental health (18). Wisdom and knowledge (including creativity and social intelligence) are among virtues shown to be associated with success and life satisfaction (19).
If factors influencing liability to bipolar disorder confer greater fitness, the nonaffected first-degree relatives of bipolar probands would be expected to manifest higher levels of fitness-promoting phenotypes than members of the general population. If, on the other hand, there is no fitness-related advantage associated with bipolar illness, the nonaffected first-degree relatives would have either equivalent or lower levels of such traits compared with the general population, as has been observed in schizophrenia (5).
Surprisingly few studies have addressed this question. Two early studies demonstrated increased prevalence of affective disorders (especially bipolar disorder) among prominent writers (10) and individuals who achieved national recognition (11), as well as among their first-degree relatives, compared with controls. Furthermore, trend-level enhanced creativity, defined by vocational and avocational activities, was observed in unaffected relatives of manic-depressive individuals compared with affected probands (20). However, these studies suffered from several important limitations, including sampling flaws (e.g., selection of high-IQ eminent writers), the lack of modern, reliable clinical evaluations, and ambiguous measurement of constructs such as genius and creativity. More recent studies comparing first-degree relatives of bipolar patients with healthy samples on standardized tests of neurocognitive functioning have produced mixed results (15, 21). Because these studies used hospital admission-based ascertainment methods, it is unclear whether the samples of bipolar probands and relatives were representative of the overall populations of such individuals. Several studies examining total population registries suggest an association between enhanced cognitive functioning and risk for bipolar disorder (12, 13) and an overrepresentation in creative professions compared with controls (22).
We employed a population-based sampling strategy to compare twin pairs discordant for bipolar disorder and demographically similar control twins without a lifetime history of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia on well-validated measures of neurocognitive functioning and temperament. Specifically, we tested whether the non-ill co-twins of bipolar probands showed enhanced levels of prosocial temperament characteristics and neurocognitive functioning relative to controls, as predicted by the reproductive fitness model. Alternatively, levels similar to or lower than those of controls would lend support to a polygenic selection-mutation balance model. Negative temperament was also assessed to explore the specificity of findings in relation to prosocial temperament. Parallel analyses were conducted in twin pairs discordant for schizophrenia for model comparison.

Method

The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the institutional review board of the University of California, Los Angeles, and the regional ethical review board at Karolinska Institute in Stockholm. All participants signed approved informed consent forms prior to participation.

Participants

The Swedish Twin Registry and Swedish National Patient Registry were linked to identify same-sex twin pairs born in Sweden between 1940 and 1985 containing at least one member with a hospital discharge diagnosis of schizophrenia or bipolar I disorder (see reference 3 for detailed recruitment procedures). This yielded a cohort of 562 potential probands (257 male and 305 female) (see Figure S1 in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article). Discordant monozygotic and dizygotic pairs were recruited randomly from this population, along with demographically balanced samples of control pairs of each zygosity. Twin pairs recruited as discordant in which the co-twin was determined to have an eligible diagnosis, and control pairs in which either co-twin was found to meet criteria for an eligible diagnosis, remained eligible and were reclassified accordingly.
The final sample consisted of 258 individuals. Participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics, along with information on zygosity, are presented in Table 1. There were no differences between groups in age, sex, handedness, or years of education. There were no differences between co-twins and controls in lifetime medication status or lifetime axis I diagnoses except history of substance use disorders, for which schizophrenia co-twins had a greater incidence than bipolar co-twins and controls (χ2=7.61, p=0.02). As expected, significant group differences were observed on symptom ratings, such that bipolar and schizophrenia probands had higher scores relative to co-twins and controls, who did not differ from each other.
TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of Twin Pairs Discordant for Bipolar Disorder and Schizophrenia and Control Pairsa
CharacteristicControls (N=63; Monozygotic, N=33; Dizygotic, N=30)Schizophrenia Probands (N=55; Monozygotic, N=21; Dizygotic, N=34)Schizophrenia Co-Twins (N=31; Monozygotic, N=7; Dizygotic, N=24)Bipolar Disorder Probands (N=64; Monozygotic, N=33; Dizygotic, N=31)Bipolar Disorder Co-Twins (N=45; Monozygotic, N=17; Dizygotic, N=28)
 MeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
Age (years)47.011.549.710.251.69.948.810.749.19.8
Education (years)13.03.012.73.014.02.512.92.613.02.7
Hamilton Depression Rating Scale score2.13.18.26.23.645.27.13.14.5
Young Mania Rating Scale score0.92.02.12.80.91.72.03.20.91.9
Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms score0.6§1.918.920.30.5§1.73.16.30.7§2.0
Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms score3.7§7.937.824.15.5§10.49.210.32.6§5.9
 N%N%N%N%N%
Female3352.42749.11651.63960.92760.0
Left-handed1015.9610.910.360.940.9
Medication statusb          
 Antipsychotic00.03258.213.22031.324.4
 Mood stabilizer00.035.5001726.600.0
 Other or none63100.02036.43096.82742.24395.6
Axis I comorbidities, lifetime          
 Anxiety disorder69.51527.3412.92437.5920.0
 Depressive disorder1320.6916.41341.900.01226.7
 Substance use disorder23.21730.9619.41218.836.7
 Other23.235.513.257.848.9
a
Symbols indicate significant differences between groups at p≤0.01.
b
Significant difference between groups, p<0.01.

Procedures

Clinical evaluation.

Diagnostic status was determined using clinical interview and medical register diagnostic history dating back to 1973. Each participant was interviewed by a psychiatrist using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID) (23) and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (24). The determination of a lifetime history of psychosis in bipolar probands was based on information gathered from the psychosis module of the SCID, with psychosis defined as the occurrence of hallucinations and/or delusions during at least one affective episode. Thirty percent of bipolar probands (19 of 64) had a positive lifetime history of psychosis. All participants were rated using the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D) (25), the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS) (26), the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) (27), and the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS) (28).

Participation criteria.

Eligibility for inclusion as a proband was a consensus diagnosis of bipolar I disorder or schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder. Exclusion criteria for all participants included a neurological disorder, history of significant head injury with loss of consciousness, mental retardation, substance use disorder within the previous 6 months, and inability to read or comprehend spoken and written Swedish. All proband participants were clinically stable (i.e., were outpatients living independently or residents of board and care homes) and were receiving medication and/or were in a period of remission. Healthy control twin pairs were screened to exclude individuals with a personal or family history of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia.

Zygosity.

DNA analysis determined zygosity following the procedure described by Hannelius et al. (29) using a highly multiplexed panel of 47 single-nucleotide polymorphisms, including a sex-specific marker, to calculate percent of allele sharing between twins. Likelihood of zygosity for the genotyped twin samples was then calculated assuming a 1% prior genotyping error rate, which is expected to result in a false positive rate <0.01.

Temperament Measures

All participants completed a battery of well-established, psychometrically sound self-report questionnaires: the Barratt Impulsivity Scale, Version 11 (30), a measure of attentional, motor, and nonplanning trait impulsiveness; the Zuckerman Sensation-Seeking Scale, Form II (31), a measure of sensitivity to internal sensations; the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (32), based on diagnostic criteria for schizotypal personality disorder; and the Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and San Diego–autoquestionnaire version, short-form (33), based on temperamental variations in psychiatric patients and healthy volunteers.

Temperament scale creation.

Items from the instruments described above were used to derive two contrasting temperament scales indexing positive and negative temperament traits (see Table S1 in the online data supplement) in a procedure that combined theoretical and empirical approaches for optimal scale construction. First, exploratory principal-components analysis of the 133 original test items was conducted to extract the major unifying factors that categorized scale items. Results indicated four predominant factors, one characterized by elements of prosocial temperament and three comprising more typically “negative” temperament characteristics. A composite scale of negative temperament was formed, as negativity scales were highly conceptually related and correlated. Coefficient alpha was maximized by subtracting items with low correlations with scale totals. The final 24-item “positivity” scale is characterized by social ease, confidence, and assertiveness. It demonstrated acceptable internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha=0.72). The final 28-item “negativity” scale is marked by depressiveness, anxiety, suspiciousness, and social withdrawal. It showed good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha=0.88). The two scales were moderately negatively correlated (r=−0.20, p<0.01).

Neuropsychological Assessment

A standard neuropsychological test battery was administered to all participants in a fixed order by a neuropsychologist. The battery included the following well-validated tests: the vocabulary and block design subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (34); the digit span, spatial span, and visual reproduction subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (35); the phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tests (36); the Trail Making Test, parts A and B (37); the California Verbal Learning Test–Revised (38); the divided attention task (39); the verbal working memory task (40); and the grooved pegboard test (41). All of the measures were translated into Swedish and back-translated into English; the Swedish-language versions have been validated and used extensively in previous studies (e.g., 42).

Neurocognitive domains.

An initial principal-components analysis of the 13 neuropsychological tests identified three predominant factors broadly characterized by 1) g factor; 2) high verbal performance (including measures of verbal fluency and verbal memory) and low visuospatial performance; and 3) attention/working memory and psychomotor processing speed. Construction of neurocognitive domains was based on this pattern of results and followed previous efforts to categorize neuropsychological tests into domains (e.g., 43, 44). Individual scores were z-transformed relative to controls and averaged across relevant tasks to form five domains (estimated IQ: vocabulary, block design; nonverbal learning and memory: spatial span, visual reproduction-delayed; verbal learning and fluency: phonemic and semantic verbal fluency, California Verbal Learning Test [learning slope, long-delay free recall]; attention and working memory: digit span, Trail Making Test part B, divided attention task, verbal working memory task; psychomotor processing speed: Trail Making Test part A, grooved pegboard test).

Statistical Analysis

Mixed-model analyses of variance with repeated measures were conducted to compare temperament and cognitive scores of bipolar and schizophrenia probands, their unaffected co-twins, and controls. Separate analyses were conducted for temperament scales and neurocognitive domains. Data were analyzed using PROC MIXED in SAS, version 9.2 (SAS Institute, Cary, N.C.). A multivariate approach was used to account for correlation among repeated effects (i.e., temperament scales, neurocognitive factors). To account for the clustered nature of the twin data, subjects were treated as individuals nested within pairs, and the data were modeled with an unstructured variance-covariance matrix form, allowing for unique variance within each group and covariance within each twin pair. A multivariate approach was used to guard against type I errors, and simple effects were examined only when multivariate effects were significant. Age and sex were entered into the models as covariates. Interaction terms for age and sex were not significant and hence were dropped from the models, and data were collapsed across zygosity for primary analyses.
Tests of linear trends on genetic loading for bipolar disorder were conducted contrasting monozygotic co-twins, dizygotic co-twins, and controls. All analyses measured significance at the 0.05 level (two-tailed).

Results

Tests of Sample Representativeness

The studied probands were comparable to the remainder of the twin proband population in sex distribution, age at first hospital admission, and number of hospital admissions. However, studied probands were younger on average than the remainder of the twin proband population (proband participants: mean=49.24 years, SD=10.44; proband nonparticipants: mean=53.65 years, SD=10.22; t=3.33, df=241, p<0.01). To control for this potential bias, we included age as a covariate and matched groups on age. There were no interactions of age with any other effects, and a median split on age produced equivalent results in both the younger and older samples, suggesting that the observed age differences cannot account for the pattern of results described below.

Temperament Analyses

There was a significant interaction between group and temperament scale (F=16.15, df=4, 252, p<0.01), with groups differing significantly on the positivity scale (F=6.32, df=4, 104, p<0.01) and the negativity scale (F=12.71, df=4, 104, p<0.01). Results are summarized in Figure 1. Bipolar co-twins scored significantly higher on the positivity scale than bipolar probands (t=2.08, df=104, p<0.05) and controls (t=−2.89, df=104, p<0.01). Bipolar probands were indistinguishable from controls on this scale. In contrast, bipolar co-twins scored significantly lower than bipolar probands on the negativity scale (t=−3.48, df=104, p<0.01). Their scores were not significantly lower than those of controls. Scores of bipolar probands were elevated on this measure compared with both other groups (co-twins: t=−3.48, df=104, p<0.01; controls: t=−2.85, df=104, p<0.01). These findings were equivalent when clinical ratings of mania symptoms (total score on the YMRS) were added to the model. There was also a significant main effect of age on scale scores (F=14.85, df=1, 252, p<0.01). A 1-year increase in age was associated with a decrease of 0.08 in overall mean scale score (t=−3.85, df=252, p<0.01). This relationship did not vary depending on group or scale.
FIGURE 1. Temperament Profiles on Positivity and Negativity Scales for Probands With Bipolar Disorder or Schizophrenia, Non-Affected Co-Twins, and Control Subjectsa
a Error bars indicate standard deviation.
In contrast, schizophrenia co-twins exhibited a similar, although attenuated, pattern of results compared with their proband counterparts. Linear trend analysis revealed evidence of a stepwise pattern in which schizophrenia probands showed significantly lower positivity scores than controls, with co-twins in-between (p<0.05) (see Figure 1). On the negativity scale, schizophrenia probands had higher scores than their co-twins (t=5.26, df=104, p<0.01) and controls (t=4.86, df=104, p<0.01), and co-twins were indistinguishable from controls.

Neurocognitive Analyses

There was a significant interaction between group and neurocognitive domain (F=4.11, df=16, 1011, p<0.01). There was also a significant main effect of age on neuropsychological performance (F=23.34, df=1, 1011, p<0.01); this relationship did not vary by group or neurocognitive factor. Specifically, a 1-year increase in age was associated with a decrease of 0.02 in overall z-transformed mean score (t=−4.83, df=1011, p<0.01).
Tests of simple effects demonstrated significant group differences on all domains (estimated IQ: F=9.02, df=4, 104, p<0.01; nonverbal learning and memory: F=10.95, df=4, 104, p<0.01; verbal learning and fluency: F=14.21, df=4, 104, p<0.01; attention and working memory: F=7.14, df=4, 104, p<0.01; psychomotor processing speed: F=10.64, df=4, 104, p<0.01). Bipolar probands showed deficits across all neurocognitive factors relative to their unaffected co-twins (estimated IQ: t=3.47, df=104, p<0.01; nonverbal learning and memory: t=4.22, df=104, p<0.01; verbal learning and fluency: t=4.81, df=104, p<0.01; attention and working memory: t=2.88, df=104, p<0.01; psychomotor processing speed: t=3.19, df=104, p<0.01) and controls (estimated IQ: t=3.16, p<0.01; nonverbal learning and memory: t=4.22, df=104, p<0.01; verbal learning and fluency: t=2.43, df=104, p=0.02; attention and working memory: t=3.13, df=104, p<0.01; psychomotor processing speed: t=3.79, df=104, p<0.01). These deficits were most pronounced in nonverbal learning and memory and psychomotor processing speed. Bipolar co-twins scored significantly higher on the verbal learning and fluency domain relative to controls (t=−2.01, df=104, p<0.05) (Figure 2). For all other factors, bipolar co-twins were indistinguishable from controls. These findings were equivalent when YMRS total score was included in the model, indicating that effects were not attributable to clinical ratings of mania symptoms.
FIGURE 2. Neurocognitive Scores for Probands With Bipolar Disorder or Schizophrenia, Non-Affected Co-Twins, and Control Subjectsa
a Error bars indicate standard deviation.
In contrast, schizophrenia co-twins exhibited a pattern of attenuated neurocognitive impairments relative to their schizophrenia twins. Schizophrenia probands showed deficits across all domains compared with their unaffected co-twins (estimated IQ: t=−4.05, df=104, p<0.01; nonverbal learning and memory: t=−3.93, df=104, p<0.01; verbal learning and fluency: t=−4.09, df=104, p<0.01; attention and working memory: t=−3.10, df=104, p<0.01; psychomotor processing speed: t=−3.59, df=104, p<0.01) and controls (estimated IQ: t=−3.89, df=104, p<0.01; nonverbal learning and memory: t=−3.78, df=104, p<0.01; verbal learning and fluency: t=−4.84, df=104, p<0.01; attention and working memory: t=−4.22, df=104, p<0.01; psychomotor processing speed: t=−5.48, df=104, p<0.01). Linear trend analysis revealed a significant effect of liability for schizophrenia for each neurocognitive domain (p values <0.01) whereby schizophrenia probands performed worse than their unaffected co-twins, who in turn performed worse than controls.
Neurocognitive results were equivalent when education level (in years) was included in the model as a covariate.

Exploratory Analyses

To further characterize positive temperament and verbal ability, heritability estimates were calculated using Falconer’s formula, which roughly approximates the proportion of total phenotypic variation attributable to genetic effects. The analysis comprised a total of 30 control twin pairs, 16 of them monozygotic and 14 of them same-sex dizygotic; three singletons were excluded. Proband twin pairs were excluded from analyses to minimize the impact of illness state on scores and covariance structures. The heritability of the temperament positivity scale was estimated at 0.44 (monozygotic: r=0.61; dizygotic: r=0.39). Estimated heritability of the verbal learning and fluency domain was 0.70 (monozygotic: r=0.69; dizygotic: r=0.34). This pattern of correlations suggests substantial additive genetic effects for both domains as well as a common environmental effect for the positivity scale, although our ability to further parse the relative genetic and environmental contributions is limited by the sample size.
Linear trend analysis comparing controls with dizygotic and monozygotic bipolar co-twins revealed a significant linear effect for the temperament positivity scale (F=5.59, df=1, 29, p<0.03) and the verbal learning and fluency domain (F=4.32, df=1, 29, p<0.05). Although post hoc tests revealed significant differences between controls and dizygotic bipolar co-twins (positivity: t=−2.86, df=29, p<0.01; verbal: t=−2.92, df=29, p<0.01), monozygotic and dizygotic co-twins did not differ from each other on either temperament positivity or verbal learning and fluency, possibly because of lack of power due to the small cell sizes of co-twins split by zygosity. Furthermore, there were no differences in neurocognitive or temperament measures between co-twins of bipolar probands with and without a lifetime history of psychosis. However, these analyses were underpowered because of the small sample of co-twins of bipolar probands with a history of psychosis. The majority of bipolar probands with a history of psychosis were concordant twins, leaving only one-third of nonaffected co-twins of bipolar probands with such a history.

Discussion

The persistence of severe mental illness is a paradox that has long puzzled evolutionary psychologists and psychiatric geneticists. We sought in this study to evaluate the presence of key fitness-related traits (temperament and neurocognitive abilities) in nonaffected twins of bipolar disorder probands to evaluate a model of reproductive fitness in contrast to a selection-mutation balancing model of mental disorder liability. The former model posits that susceptibility alleles confer adaptive benefits under certain circumstances or below some threshold for disorder. In support of a reproductive fitness model, we found that certain temperament and neurocognitive benefits appear to be associated with liability for bipolar disorder. Specifically, bipolar co-twins had elevated scores on a “positivity” temperament scale, indicating traits of social ease, confidence, and assertiveness, as well as superior neurocognitive performance on tests of verbal learning and fluency relative to healthy subjects. This study provides evidence that increases in liability for bipolar disorder that remain below some clinical threshold appear to be associated with fitness-promoting features such as sociability and verbal facility. Such findings are consistent with evidence of minimal to no evolutionary impact of bipolar disorder (7, 8) in contrast to the severe fertility disadvantages observed in schizophrenia (4).
Personality and temperament traits occurring at the extreme ends of normalized dimensions have been associated with risk for psychiatric disorders and, by extension, reduced reproductive fitness (e.g., “difficult” temperament [45], neuroticism, and mood lability [46]). Our findings of elevated pessimism, suspiciousness, and asociality among both proband samples support a close association between negative personality characteristics and illness state. Conversely, the personality traits we examined here, encompassing aspects of prosociality and positive affect, have relevance to mate selection and reproductive success. In general, individuals who tend to be more verbal, outgoing and sociable, optimistic, and psychologically resilient may be viewed as more desirable partners (47, 48).
In terms of neurocognition, previous work demonstrated that poor verbal learning and memory was associated with functional impairment in euthymic bipolar subjects (49), suggesting that deficits in this particular domain may be related to impaired role functioning and thereby decreased fitness. On the other hand, in the general population, higher intelligence is associated with lower mortality rates (50) and increased reproductive fitness (51, 52). Whether such benefits can be attributed to specific domains of intelligence, such as verbal ability, is not known. Our findings suggest that the characteristic personality and neurocognitive profiles evident in bipolar co-twins due to the presence of susceptibility factors may indeed confer increased reproductive fitness while remaining below threshold for disorder. The observed increases in fitness-promoting features in nonaffected bipolar relatives dovetail with evidence of increased fertility in healthy sisters of bipolar probands (7), suggesting enhanced fitness in nonaffected individuals carrying bipolar liability factors.
The performance benefits observed in bipolar co-twins appear unique to specific personality and neurocognitive domains, rather than representative of a ubiquitous advantage. Bipolar co-twins did not show reduced levels of negative temperament relative to controls, and the two scales were only moderately correlated (r=−0.20). Similarly, scores of bipolar co-twins were equivalent to those of controls on other neurocognitive dimensions, with selected strengths in the verbal domain. These findings stand in contrast to previous neuropsychological studies demonstrating support for candidate neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder (15, 21), with nonaffected first-degree relatives showing impairments relative to healthy controls in the domains of executive functioning, verbal memory, and sustained attention. However, those studies used clinic-derived samples, which are heavily weighted toward more severe cases. The key advantage of a population-based sampling approach, as employed here, is confidence that the studied probands are representative of the broader population of probands—and, by extension, the studied nonaffected co-twins of bipolar probands are likely to be representative. Moreover, one previous study demonstrated intact verbal learning and memory in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar I disorder despite impairments in executive functioning relative to controls (53). Furthermore, a recent cohort study of over 1 million men suggested that verbal aspects of intelligence may serve as a risk factor for pure forms of bipolar disorder (i.e., without psychiatric comorbidities [54]). Our findings here suggest that the benefits associated with liability for bipolar disorder may be unique to the domain of verbal skills.
The findings for bipolar co-twins are contrasted with those for schizophrenia co-twins, who did not exhibit such benefits but rather showed deficits intermediate between controls and probands. Schizophrenia co-twins showed lower levels of “positivity” and more generalized reductions in neurocognitive functioning relative to controls, although not as low or impaired as schizophrenia probands. Co-twins of schizophrenia probands had deficits on the same traits on which co-twins of bipolar patients showed advantaged or equivalent performance relative to controls, suggesting segregation at the endophenotypic level between these two syndromes, at least with respect to temperament and neurocognitive functioning. The neurocognitive domains of nonverbal learning and memory, attention and working memory, and psychomotor processing speed appear to be particularly robust endophenotypic markers of schizophrenia, consistent with previous studies (5). No neurocognitive impairment was specific to bipolar disorder, as both proband groups tended to show performance decrements, indicating that illness manifestation is associated with impairments in both syndromes. Taken together, the findings in schizophrenia implicate a polygenic selection-mutation balance, whereby genetic persistence of illness appears to hinge on the maintenance of many small-effect genetic variants in the population, which may be common, rare, inherited, or de novo, but against which there is relatively low selection pressure. This highlights the possibility of an inherent difference between bipolar disorder and schizophrenia and suggests that a single evolutionary model to explain the presence of all forms of mental illness in the population is unlikely.
A possible limitation of this study is the slightly younger proband samples relative to the remainder of the proband population. That we recruited fewer older participants than is representative of the general population suggests that our results may have an ascertainment bias toward younger proband twin pairs. It is possible that an age difference emerged because older probands were more likely to decline study participation because of age-related health issues or travel difficulties, or because they could not be reached because of illness or other factors. However, we controlled for this potential bias by matching groups on age, and there were no interactions between age and group on neurocognitive or temperament scores. Secondary analyses bifurcating groups on age showed equivalent effects. Therefore, the characterized age differences cannot account for the observed pattern in the present findings. Additionally, it is possible that nonaffected relatives of more severe cases (in terms of genetic loading, history of psychosis, shared genetic liability with schizophrenia, or clinical severity) would not exhibit fitness-promoting features but rather performance decrements, consistent with initial findings of reduced fecundity from clinic-derived bipolar samples (6). Future studies should ensure adequate power to investigate heterogeneity in bipolar disorder.
To our knowledge, this is the first systematic study to indicate a possible familial compensatory advantage to bipolar illness in discordant twin pairs. These findings contribute to mounting evidence linking creativity and enhanced cognitive functioning and risk for bipolar disorder (913, 20, 22) and may inform models of mechanisms underpinning this association. That liability for bipolar disorder may confer temperament and neurocognitive benefits could provide one explanation for the genetic persistence of this illness.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2014.13121683.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Weissman MM, Bland RC, Canino GJ, Faravelli C, Greenwald S, Hwu HG, Joyce PR, Karam EG, Lee CK, Lellouch J, Lépine JP, Newman SC, Rubio-Stipec M, Wells JE, Wickramaratne PJ, Wittchen H, Yeh EK: Cross-national epidemiology of major depression and bipolar disorder. JAMA 1996; 276:293–299
2.
McGuffin P, Rijsdijk F, Andrew M, Sham P, Katz R, Cardno A: The heritability of bipolar affective disorder and the genetic relationship to unipolar depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:497–502
3.
Lichtenstein P, Yip BH, Björk C, Pawitan Y, Cannon TD, Sullivan PF, Hultman CM: Common genetic determinants of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in Swedish families: a population-based study. Lancet 2009; 373:234–239
4.
Bundy H, Stahl D, MacCabe JH: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fertility of patients with schizophrenia and their unaffected relatives. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2011; 123:98–106
5.
Cannon TD: The inheritance of intermediate phenotypes for schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiatry 2005; 18:135–140
6.
Baron M, Risch N, Mendlewicz J: Differential fertility in bipolar affective illness. J Affect Disord 1982; 4:103–112
7.
Power RA, Kyaga S, Uher R, MacCabe JH, Långström N, Landen M, McGuffin P, Lewis CM, Lichtenstein P, Svensson AC: Fecundity of patients with schizophrenia, autism, bipolar disorder, depression, anorexia nervosa, or substance abuse vs their unaffected siblings. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:22–30
8.
MacCabe JH, Koupil I, Leon DA: Lifetime reproductive output over two generations in patients with psychosis and their unaffected siblings: the Uppsala 1915–1929 Birth Cohort Multigenerational Study. Psychol Med 2009; 39:1667–1676
9.
Murray G, Johnson SL: The clinical significance of creativity in bipolar disorder. Clin Psychol Rev 2010; 30:721–732
10.
Andreasen NC: Creativity and mental illness: prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree relatives. Am J Psychiatry 1987; 144:1288–1292
11.
McNeil TF: Prebirth and postbirth influence on the relationship between creative ability and recorded mental illness. J Pers 1971; 39:391–406
12.
MacCabe JH, Lambe MP, Cnattingius S, Sham PC, David AS, Reichenberg A, Murray RM, Hultman CM: Excellent school performance at age 16 and risk of adult bipolar disorder: national cohort study. Br J Psychiatry 2010; 196:109–115
13.
Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, Poulton R, Caspi A: Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:50–57
14.
Evans L, Akiskal HS, Keck PE Jr, McElroy SL, Sadovnick AD, Remick RA, Kelsoe JR: Familiality of temperament in bipolar disorder: support for a genetic spectrum. J Affect Disord 2005; 85:153–168
15.
Glahn DC, Almasy L, Barguil M, Hare E, Peralta JM, Kent JW Jr, Dassori A, Contreras J, Pacheco A, Lanzagorta N, Nicolini H, Raventós H, Escamilla MA: Neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder identified in multiplex multigenerational families. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:168–177
16.
Eaves LJ, Martin NG, Heath AC, Hewitt JK, Neale MC: Personality and reproductive fitness. Behav Genet 1990; 20:563–568
17.
Miller GF: Sexual selection for indicators of intelligence, in The Nature of Intelligence. Edited by Bock G, Goode J, Webb K. New York, John Wiley & Sons, 2000, pp 260–275
18.
Seligman MEP, Csikszentmihalyi M: Positive psychology: an introduction. Am Psychol 2000; 55:5–14
19.
Seligman MEP, Steen TA, Park N, Peterson C: Positive psychology progress: empirical validation of interventions. Am Psychol 2005; 60:410–421
20.
Richards R, Kinney DK, Lunde I, Benet M, Merzel APC: Creativity in manic-depressives, cyclothymes, their normal relatives, and control subjects. J Abnorm Psychol 1988; 97:281–288
21.
Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C: Cognitive endophenotypes of bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives. J Affect Disord 2009; 113:1–20
22.
Kyaga S, Lichtenstein P, Boman M, Hultman C, Långström N, Landén M: Creativity and mental disorder: family study of 300,000 people with severe mental disorder. Br J Psychiatry 2011; 199:373–379
23.
Spitzer RL, Williams JBW, Gibbon M, First MB: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1994
24.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW, Benjamin L: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II), version 2.0. New York, New York State Psychiatric Institute, Biometrics Research, 1994
25.
Hamilton M: A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23:56–62
26.
Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA: A rating scale for mania: reliability, validity, and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry 1978; 133:429–435
27.
Andreasen NC: The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1984
28.
Andreasen NC: The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1984
29.
Hannelius U, Gherman L, Mäkelä VV, Lindstedt A, Zucchelli M, Lagerberg C, Tybring G, Kere J, Lindgren CM: Large-scale zygosity testing using single nucleotide polymorphisms. Twin Res Hum Genet 2007; 10:604–625
30.
Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES: Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Clin Psychol 1995; 51:768–774
31.
Kolin EA, Price L, Zoob I: Development of a sensation-seeking scale. J Consult Psychol 1964; 28:477–482
32.
Raine A: The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull 1991; 17:555–564
33.
Akiskal HS, Mendlowicz MV, Jean-Louis G, Rapaport MH, Kelsoe JR, Gillin JC, Smith TL: TEMPS-A: validation of a short version of a self-rated instrument designed to measure variations in temperament. J Affect Disord 2005; 85:45–52
34.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-III) Manual. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1997
35.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised (WMS-III) Manual. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1997
36.
Benton A, Hamsher K: Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, University of Iowa, 1976
37.
Reitan R, Wolfson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation. Tucson, Ariz, Neuropsychology Press, 1985
38.
Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA: California Verbal Learning Test. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1987
39.
Vilkki J, Virtanen S, Surma-Aho O, Servo A: Dual task performance after focal cerebral lesions and closed head injuries. Neuropsychologia 1996; 34:1051–1056
40.
Daneman M, Carpenter PA: Individual differences in working memory and reading. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 1980; 19:450–466
41.
Tiffin J: Purdue Pegboard Examiner’s Manual. Rosemont, Ill, London House, 1968
42.
Osterberg K, Ørbaek P, Karlson B: Neuropsychological test performance of Swedish multiple chemical sensitivity patients: an exploratory study. Appl Neuropsychol 2002; 9:139–147
43.
Carter CS, Barch DM: Cognitive neuroscience-based approaches to measuring and improving treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia: the CNTRICS initiative. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:1131–1137
44.
Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF, Heaton RK: Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2004; 72:29–39
45.
Maziade M, Caron C, Côté R, Boutin P, Thivierge J: Extreme temperament and diagnosis: a study in a psychiatric sample of consecutive children. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990; 47:477–484
46.
Bienvenu OJ, Samuels JF, Costa PT, Reti IM, Eaton WW, Nestadt G: Anxiety and depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality: a higher- and lower-order personality trait investigation in a community sample. Depress Anxiety 2004; 20:92–97
47.
Smith BR, Blumstein DT: Fitness consequences of personality: a meta-analysis. Behav Ecol 2008; 19:448–455
48.
Michalski RL, Shackelford TK: Evolutionary personality psychology: reconciling human nature and individual differences. Pers Individ Dif 2010; 48:509–516
49.
Altshuler LL, Bearden CE, Green MF, van Gorp W, Mintz J: A relationship between neurocognitive impairment and functional impairment in bipolar disorder: a pilot study. Psychiatry Res 2008; 157:289–293
50.
Batty GD, Deary IJ, Gottfredson LS: Premorbid (early life) IQ and later mortality risk: systematic review. Ann Epidemiol 2007; 17:278–288
51.
Arden R, Gottfredson LS, Miller G: Does a fitness factor contribute to the association between intelligence and health outcomes? Evidence from medical abnormality counts among 3654 US veterans. Intelligence 2009; 37:581–591
52.
Arden R, Gottfredson LS, Miller G, Pierce A: Intelligence and semen quality are positively correlated. Intelligence 2009; 37:277–282
53.
Clark L, Sarna A, Goodwin GM: Impairment of executive function but not memory in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar I disorder and in euthymic patients with unipolar depression. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1980–1982
54.
Gale CR, Batty GD, McIntosh AM, Porteous DJ, Deary IJ, Rasmussen F: Is bipolar disorder more common in highly intelligent people? A cohort study of a million men. Mol Psychiatry 2013; 18:190–194

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1191 - 1198
PubMed: 25124743

History

Received: 20 December 2013
Revision received: 2 April 2014
Accepted: 6 May 2014
Published online: 1 November 2014
Published in print: November 01, 2014

Authors

Details

Rachel G. Higier, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm; and the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.
Amy M. Jimenez, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm; and the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.
Christina M. Hultman, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm; and the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.
Jacqueline Borg, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm; and the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.
Cristina Roman, B.A.
From the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm; and the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.
Isabelle Kizling, M.Sc.
From the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm; and the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.
Henrik Larsson, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm; and the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.
Tyrone D. Cannon, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles; the Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institute, Stockholm; and the Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn.

Notes

Presented at the Third Biennial Schizophrenia International Research Society Conference in Florence, Italy, April 14–18, 2012.
Address correspondence to Dr. Cannon ([email protected]).

Author Contributions

The first two authors contributed equally to this work.

Funding Information

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.Supported by NIH grant ROIMH052857 (to Dr. Cannon), with additional support from National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowships (to Dr. Higier and Dr. Jimenez).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share