Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective:

Pharmacological treatments for agitation and aggression in patients with Alzheimer’s disease have shown limited efficacy. The authors assessed the heterogeneity of response to citalopram in the Citalopram for Agitation in Alzheimer Disease (CitAD) study to identify individuals who may be helped or harmed.

Method:

In this double-blind parallel-group multicenter trial of 186 patients with Alzheimer’s disease and clinically significant agitation, participants were randomly assigned to receive citalopram or placebo for 9 weeks, with the dosage titrated to 30 mg/day over the first 3 weeks. Five planned potential predictors of treatment outcome were assessed, along with six additional predictors. The authors then used a two-stage multivariate method to select the most likely predictors; grouped participants into 10 subgroups by their index scores; and estimated the citalopram treatment effect for each.

Results:

Five covariates were likely predictors, and treatment effect was heterogeneous across the subgroups. Patients for whom citalopram was more effective were more likely to be outpatients, have the least cognitive impairment, have moderate agitation, and be within the middle age range (76–82 years). Patients for whom placebo was more effective were more likely to be in long-term care, have more severe cognitive impairment, have more severe agitation, and be treated with lorazepam.

Conclusions:

Considering several covariates together allowed the identification of responders. Those with moderate agitation and with lower levels of cognitive impairment were more likely to benefit from citalopram, and those with more severe agitation and greater cognitive impairment were at greater risk for adverse responses. Considering the dosages used and the association of citalopram with cardiac QT prolongation, use of this agent to treat agitation may be limited to a subgroup of people with dementia.
Aggression and agitation are common in people with dementia or Alzheimer’s disease. Agitation is generally multidetermined, may wax and wane, is often associated with psychosocial stress, disruption, intercurrent medical conditions, and drug effects, and often can be managed by addressing the underlying conditions, without psychotropic medications (1, 2). Aggression is associated with similar factors, often fluctuates, and may be a response to perceived threat, but it requires intervention when it causes patients to be a threat to their own or others’ well-being. In clinical practice, a range of psychotropic drugs are prescribed for agitation or aggression, most often antipsychotics, and anxiolytics and antidepressants to a lesser extent (3). These medications have substantial limitations in terms of efficacy and safety (4).
Trials of antidepressants have been conducted for treating depression in people with dementia (5, 6) and, more recently, agitation (7). Citalopram in particular showed equivalence to antipsychotics for inpatients and outpatients, but with differential adverse events (8, 9). A recent multicenter randomized placebo-controlled trial (7) showed efficacy for citalopram in patients with Alzheimer’s disease with agitation, based on clinical global and agitation scale outcomes. As part of the statistical analysis plan of that clinical trial, here we report subgroup analyses that use a univariate method and a two-stage multivariate method (10) to assess heterogeneity of response and possible predictors of differential responses between citalopram and placebo treatments.

Method

The primary objective of the Citalopram for Agitation in Alzheimer Disease (CitAD) trial was to assess citalopram’s efficacy for agitation or aggression (see the primary report [7] and the detailed methods report [11]). Participants had probable Alzheimer’s disease, scores between 5 and 28 on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) (12), and clinically significant agitation, for which a physician determined that medication would be appropriate, based on scale ratings of either “very frequently” (once or more per day) or “occurring frequently with moderate or marked severity” (several times per week and difficult to redirect or control) on the agitation/aggression domain of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (13). They could not have had a major depressive episode or psychosis requiring antidepressant or antipsychotic treatment. Cholinesterase inhibitors and memantine at stable dosages were allowed. Patients received a psychosocial intervention and were randomly assigned to receive either citalopram (N=94) or placebo (N=92) for 9 weeks. Dosages were titrated to as high as 30 mg/day over 3 weeks, based on response and tolerability. The co-primary outcome measures were the modified Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Clinical Global Impression of Change (CGIC) (14), modified to assess agitation, and the agitation subscale of the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale (15). The 18-point agitation subscale of the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale consists of the agitation, hostility/uncooperativeness, and disinhibition items from the parent scale, each rated on a 0–6 scale as “not present,” “very mild,” “mild,” “moderate,” “moderately severe,” “severe,” or “extremely severe.” Other outcome measures were scores from the short form of the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (16), the agitation/aggression subscale of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory, the 12-item Neuropsychiatric Inventory, the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Activities of Daily Living scale (17), and the MMSE, and the use of lorazepam as a rescue medication.
Patients in the citalopram group (78% received 30 mg/day and 15% received 20 mg/day) were significantly improved compared with those in the placebo group on both primary outcome measures. The estimated treatment difference at week 9 on the agitation subscale of the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale (citalopram minus placebo) was −0.93 points (95% CI=−1.80, −0.06; p=0.04). Forty percent of the citalopram group were rated as moderately or markedly improved on the CGIC, compared with 26% of the placebo group, which constitutes an absolute risk difference of 14% and an estimated treatment effect (i.e., the odds ratio of being at or better than a given CGIC category) of 2.13 (95% CI=1.23, 3.69; p=0.01). The citalopram group also showed significant improvement on the short-form Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory, the 12-item Neuropsychiatric Inventory, and caregiver distress scores, but not on the Neuropsychiatric Inventory agitation subscale, the Activities of Daily Living scale, or in use of lorazepam (7). Worsening of cognition (as measured by the MMSE) and QT interval prolongation were seen in the citalopram group (7, 18).
The planned subgroup analyses reported here included the five prespecified baseline predictors: residency status (long-term care or outpatient), presence of psychosis (i.e., delusions or hallucinations as rated on the Neuropsychiatric Inventory), and severity of functional impairment (based on the Activities of Daily Living scale), cognitive impairment (based on the MMSE), and agitation (based on the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale). We also selected six additional potential predictors of outcome: age, gender, and use of memantine, lorazepam, trazodone, or cholinesterase inhibitors within 3 weeks of baseline. Each predictor was collapsed into two or three categories. The MMSE was divided into mild (scores ≥21), moderate (scores from 11 to 20), and severe (scores ≤10) cognitive impairment, based on the literature (19). Other continuous variables were categorized as tertiles.
Subgroup analyses were conducted using univariate and multivariate interaction methods. For the univariate interaction method, the treatment effect across categories of a predictor was compared using logistic regressions for CGIC response and for Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale response outcomes (i.e., odds ratio of a 50% reduction from baseline, citalopram compared with placebo). The remaining continuous outcome measures (the Activities of Daily Living scale, the MMSE, and the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory) were modeled using linear mixed-effects regression models that included a patient-specific random intercept, a visit indicator, a treatment indicator, treatment-by-visit interactions, and baseline outcome. Likelihood ratio tests were used to assess the significance of the treatment-by-covariate interactions in all models.
For the multivariate interaction method, a typical regression that includes multiple interactions relies heavily on model assumptions; we therefore used an exploratory post hoc two-stage approach (10). Of the 11 baseline covariates, those for which there was more than a threefold change in odds ratio between any two covariate levels were included in two-stage working models.
In the first stage, an index score was calculated for each participant based on the parametric working models using the baseline predictors. Specifically, for each participant, we used his or her baseline covariate values in the models to obtain a predicted response probability for citalopram and a predicted response probability for placebo. The index score was then calculated as the difference between the predicted response probabilities from the citalopram and placebo models and represents the predicted treatment effect for that participant based on the working models (see the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article). Participants with the same index score can be thought of as a subset with that combination of covariate values. The value of the index score, however, is not necessarily an accurate estimate of the true treatment effect for that subset if the working models are incorrect.
The second stage of the analysis was conducted to allow for the possibility that the working models might be incorrect. In this stage, participants were sorted by index score and grouped into deciles; the treatment effect for each group was estimated nonparametrically as the difference between the empirical response-probability for citalopram minus placebo for that group. Confidence intervals for the subgroup treatment effect estimates were calculated by bootstrapping with a correction for multiple comparisons (see the data supplement).

Results

The covariates that were continuous measures, including age, Activities of Daily Living scale, and Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale, were divided into tertiles and were roughly balanced between randomized arms. Some of the categorical covariates were not evenly distributed in the trials sample: 7% lived in long-term care, 48% had delusions or hallucinations, 54% were male, and 69%, 42%, 10%, and 8%, respectively, had prescriptions for cholinesterase inhibitors, memantine, trazodone, and lorazepam (Table 1).
TABLE 1. Distribution of Planned and Post Hoc Baseline Predictors in a Sample of Patients With Alzheimer’s Disease With Aggression or Agitation Treated With Citalopram or Placebo
 Total (N=186)Citalopram (N=94)Placebo (N=92)
MeasureN%N%N%
Residency status      
 Home or with relatives1739386918795
 Long-term care1378955
Psychosisa      
 No hallucinations or delusions975252554549
 Hallucinations and/or delusions894842454751
Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Activities of Daily Living scale      
 Highest tertile (≥54)653539412628
 Middle tertile (31–53)643428303639
 Lowest tertile (≤30)573127293033
Mini-Mental State Examination      
 Mild or no impairment (≥21)542932342224
 Moderate impairment (11–20)814445483639
 Severe impairment (≤10)512717183437
Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale      
 Lowest tertile (≤5)522829312325
 Middle tertile (6–8)603234362628
 Highest tertile (≥9)744031334347
Age group (years)      
 47–75573130322729
 76–82603229313134
 83–92693735373437
Male1015450535155
Memantine use784241443740
Lorazepam use15866910
Trazodone use1910111289
Cholinesterase inhibitor use1286962666672
a
Based on items 1 and 2 of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory.
Based on the univariate method, the effect of citalopram compared with placebo on CGIC response did not vary significantly between levels of any of the five planned and the six post hoc predictors except for residency status: patients living at home or with relatives experienced a significantly greater citalopram effect compared with placebo than patients in long-term care (likelihood ratio=2.28, 95% CI=1.14, 4.57, and likelihood ratio=0.11, 95% CI=0.01, 1.78, respectively; p=0.025) (Figure 1). Moreover, using the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale, the Activities of Daily Living scale, the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory, and the MMSE as response outcomes resulted in no significant interactions on any of the 11 candidate predictors of response except for age with the Cohen-Mansfield Agitation Inventory (data not shown).
FIGURE 1. Effect of Potential Predictors on Response on the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Clinical Global Impression of Change Scalea
a The first five predictors were prespecified, and the other six were post hoc. ADCS=Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study; CGIC=Alzheimer's Disease Cooperative Study–Clinical Global Impression of Change; LR=likelihood ratio; MMSE=Mini-Mental State Examination; NBRS=Neurobehavioral Rating Scale.
For the multivariate method, we assessed heterogeneity of the treatment effect using two stages. For the CGIC, five of the 11 baseline covariates met the selection criteria—residency status, MMSE, Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale, age, and treatment with lorazepam—and were used to compute the index score (see the online data supplement). The variation of the treatment effect across groups of different index scores is shown in Figure 2. The figure demonstrates as well that there is a subgroup of patients, those with index scores below the 62nd percentile, for whom there is 95% confidence that the treatment effect is at least as large as the estimated average effect for all patients.
FIGURE 2. Average Difference Between the Citalopram and Placebo Groups in Probability of Response on the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Clinical Global Impression of Change Scalea
a The 95% confidence intervals are shown after correction for multiple comparisons. For more than 62% of the patients, the 95% confidence interval was larger than the average 0.136 (seen at 100% on the x-axis). This can be interpreted clinically by viewing the distribution of covariates for these patients (see Table 2) and comparing this to the distribution of all patients. For a given value on the x-axis, for example, the 86th percentile, the corresponding value on the y-axis, 0.25, is the difference between the percent response in the citalopram group minus the percent response in the placebo group (estimated nonparametrically) for the patients with index scores below the 86th percentile.
This can be interpreted clinically by viewing the distribution of covariates for participants with different index score categories compared with the distribution of all patients (Figure 3 and Table 2). Figure 3 shows the nonparametrically estimated effect for patient groups combined by the deciles of the index score. A likelihood ratio test comparing the 10 deciles to a hypothesis that there is no heterogeneity suggested that the treatment effect truly varies by index score subgroup (p=0.002). This 10-mean model can be further reduced to a three-mean model of negative or placebo responders (decile 1), marginal or low responders (deciles 2–8), and high responders (deciles 9 and 10) which was still a significant improvement over the common mean model, but was not different from the 10-mean model.
FIGURE 3. Box Plots of the Index Score Deciles Based on the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Clinical Global Impression of Change Scalea
a The figure shows that two groups, deciles 9 and 10, show particularly strong effects favoring citalopram response compared with placebo, and one group, decile 1, shows a strong effect favoring placebo as more effective than citalopram. Most groups show essentially trivial effects for citalopram response.
TABLE 2. Table of Covariate Distributions for All Participants, and for Groups Defined by the Working Model Score, Including the Three Significant Subset Deciles and the Top 60% Index Scorersa
 All ParticipantsDecile 1Top 60% Index ScorersDecile 9Decile 10
CovariateN%N%N%N%N%
Residency status          
 Home or with relatives1559310481001001510017100
 Long-term care1271152000000
Mini-Mental State Examination          
 Mild or no impairment (≥21)4929314383864017100
 Moderate impairment (11–20)75451152363664000
 Severe impairment (≤10)4326733262632000
Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale          
 Lowest tertile (≤5)482952410100000
 Middle tertile (6–8)553315515196017100
 Highest tertile (≥9)64381571393964000
Age group (years)          
 47–7554321048232300635
 76–82533231440401387847
 83–9260368383737213318
Lorazepam use117733110000
a
For more on the deciles, see Figure 3.
Patients with the largest predicted treatment effects favoring citalopram were more likely to be living outside long-term care facilities, to have milder cognitive impairment (MMSE score range, 21–28), to have a middle level of baseline agitation (Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale score range, 6–8), to be within the middle age range of the trial population (76–82 years), and not to have been using lorazepam at baseline. By comparison, patients with the largest predicted treatment effect favoring placebo were more likely to be living in long-term care, to have moderate to severe cognitive impairment (MMSE scores ≤20), to have more severe baseline agitation (Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale score range, 9–14), to be within the youngest (47–75 years) or oldest age range (83–92), and to be treated with lorazepam. When using the above two-stage method for the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale response and the secondary outcomes, there was no analogous evidence of heterogeneity across index scores.

Discussion

Given the clinical heterogeneity of agitation and aggression in patients with Alzheimer’s disease, it is likely that any effective therapy would benefit only a subset of patients with these behaviors. In our planned, protocol-specified analysis of this randomized controlled trial, we found no individual covariates that predicted positive outcomes with citalopram but did find that residence in long-term care was associated with a negative outcome with citalopram.
Thus, we hypothesized incorrectly that subgroups defined by single covariate predictors would respond differentially to citalopram. This failure could be a result of small sample size, imbalances in the actual outcomes associated with the covariates, and the overall small statistical effect sizes of the clinical outcomes of the trial. For example, only 186 participants were randomly assigned to treatment, 7% were living in nursing homes, 7% were being treated with lorazepam as a rescue medication at baseline, and the statistically significant co-primary outcomes were a relatively small risk difference (0.136) for the CGIC and less than a 1-point difference on the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale between citalopram and placebo. Thus, differences in the single covariate-defined subgroups would have been difficult to discern or potentially unreliable at identifying study participants who may have benefited from citalopram.
The multivariate analysis, on the other hand, confirmed the main outcome of the trial, namely, that there was an average treatment effect or difference in response probability with citalopram treatment, that is, citalopram improved CGIC response compared with placebo by an average 13.6% difference in response probability (citalopram minus placebo). Secondly, and importantly, this analysis identified subgroups for which the treatment effects were larger or smaller than the average, that is, for which the treatment effect for citalopram compared with placebo was not homogeneous across participants.
Indeed, two groups (about 20% of the sample) showed particularly large effects, with differences of approximately 60%−70% in response probabilities for citalopram compared with placebo; one group (about 10% of the sample) showed a large negative effect, with a difference in response probability of approximately 70% favoring placebo; the other groups (about 70%) showed essentially trivial effects. The clinical characteristics of the two subgroups for which citalopram was most effective included outpatient status, milder cognitive impairment, “moderate” to “moderately severe” agitation scores on the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale (as compared with “mild” to “moderate” for the lowest tertile and “moderately severe” to “severe” for the highest tertile), and being neither in the youngest nor the oldest age group.
The characteristics of the subgroup for which assignment to placebo was most effective were residence in long-term care, being in the oldest or youngest age groups (47–75 years or 83–92 years), having moderate to severe cognitive impairment (an MMSE score ≤20), having “moderately severe” to “severe” agitation on the Neurobehavioral Rating Scale agitation subscale, and receiving treatment with lorazepam.
The identification of a subgroup that had markedly better outcomes on placebo suggests that patients with more severe agitation and cognitive impairment may be harmed by citalopram. Thus, the groups most responsive to citalopram had more mild cognitive impairment and less severe agitation at baseline.
Clinical trials usually can address only one clinical hypothesis with reasonable statistical power. The results of the multivariable post hoc analysis reported here highlight the limitations of limiting post hoc analyses to univariable methods, a characteristic of many trials, including this one. Allowing the inclusion of patients with a broad range of agitation and cognitive impairment may have led to outcomes that are difficult to apply to clinical practice: On average, the patients who were enrolled in CitAD benefited from citalopram, but some benefited more than others in a manner that was not random and not predicted. Here, our two-stage multivariate analysis suggested clinical implications and a way forward (see below). Limitations of the trial, analysis, and statistical methods included the small sample size and our knowledge of the results of the prespecified univariate analyses, because this informed the subsequent multivariate analysis. Importantly, the predictive covariates for composing the subgroups were chosen primarily because they were relatively stable baseline characteristics and because there were limited candidate predictors from which to choose. Since covariates that are predictive for treatment differences (i.e., in our analysis those with a threefold change in odds ratio) may both interact with the treatment assignment and relate to the outcome in either treatment group, it is possible that our model is misspecified or that interactions are not well defined, factors limiting the ability to make inferences about treatment effects. Ultimately, however, the validity of the interpretation of the results derives from the empirical effect sizes in the subgroups and does not depend on the selection criteria.
Although a statistically significant treatment interaction was detected only for residency status using the univariate method, the direction of the (nonsignificant) interactions was the same for both methods for all the subgroups included in the multivariate model. However, the empirical basis of the proposed multivariate approach, as compared with other, more model-dependent approaches, make the interpretation of the results more reliable.
Notwithstanding these limitations, the results support heterogeneity of clinical response to citalopram—specifically that outpatients with Alzheimer’s disease without severe agitation, who do not have major depression or psychosis for which antipsychotics may be required, may benefit from citalopram compared with placebo. Future trials of citalopram or similar drugs used for aggression or agitation might account for this heterogeneity by stratifying or including or excluding participants based on levels of cognitive impairment and severity of aggression or agitation. Although the small numbers within the decile group may not broadly inform the use of citalopram in patients in long-term care or those treated with lorazepam, it may nonetheless be prudent to avoid citalopram under these circumstances, and especially when there are other options. The finding that patients with more severe agitation or aggression responded better with placebo or poorly with citalopram raises further caution. Given these results, along with the established associations of citalopram with delayed cardiac repolarization (18, 20) and with cognitive impairment (7), and given safety concerns of antidepressants for depressed elderly patients (21) and the FDA’s recommendation to avoid citalopram dosages over 20 mg/day in patients over age 60, citalopram may have limited use for treating agitation in Alzheimer’s disease.
In sum, these analyses demonstrate that citalopram’s effect at 30 mg/day is heterogeneous, with maximal salutary effects for patients with relatively milder cognitive impairment and moderate agitation, and is without effect or potentially harmful for patients with more moderate to severe cognitive impairment and more severe agitation.

Footnotes

ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT00898807.
Dr. Schneider has received grants and clinical trials support from NIH, Abbott, AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, GlaxoSmithKline, Johnson & Johnson, Lundbeck, Merck, NIH, Novartis, Pfizer, and Roche and consultancy fees, including for data monitoring committees and adjudication committees, from Abbott, AbbVie, AC Immune, AstraZeneca, Baxter, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Elan, Eli Lilly, Forest Laboratories, Forum, GlaxoSmithKline, Johnson & Johnson, Lundbeck, Merck, Merz, Novartis, Orion, Otsuka, Pfizer, Roche, Servier, Takeda, Toyama/FujiFilm, and Zinfandel. Dr. Devanand has served on advisory boards for AbbVie, Astellas, and Lundbeck and as an adviser for Intracellular Therapies. Dr. Mintzer has received grants from the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study, Avanir, Eli Lilly, Genentech, Grifols, Merck, Pfizer, Roche, Takeda, and Transition Therapeutics and consulting fees from Insys RX; he is a majority owner of BioPharma Connex, a company that currently has active contracts with Avanir, Roche, and Eli Lilly, and he is Vice President of Medical Affairs at NeuroQuest. Dr. Mulsant has received research support from Brain Canada, Bristol-Myers Squibb (medications for an NIH-funded clinical trial), the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health Foundation, Eli Lilly (medications for an NIH-funded clinical trial), NIH, and Pfizer (medications for an NIH-funded clinical trial), and Pfizer/Wyeth (medications for an NIH-funded clinical trial); he owns stock in General Electric. Dr. Pollock has served on a board for Lundbeck Canada and as a consultant for Wyeth, and he received travel and accommodation expenses from Lundbeck International Neuroscience Foundation. Dr. Porsteinsson has received grants from AstraZeneca, Avanir, Baxter, Biogen, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eisai, Elan, EnVivo, Genentech/Roche, Janssen Alzheimer Initiative, Medivation, Merck, Pfizer, Toyama, Transition Therapeutics, NIH, NIMH, the National Institute on Aging, and the Department of Defense; he has served as a consultant for Elan, Janssen Alzheimer Initiative, Lundbeck, Pfizer, and TransTech Pharma and as a speaker for Forest; he has served on data safety and monitoring boards for Quintiles, Functional Neuromodulation, and the New York State Psychiatric Institute; and he has participated in the development of educational presentations for PRI and CME, Inc. Dr. Rabins has provided legal testimony for Janssen. Dr. Rosenberg has received research support from Eli Lilly, Functional Neuromodulation, and Merck, travel support from Eli Lilly, and CME development support from Vindico. Dr. Weintraub has received research funding or support from Avid Radiopharmaceuticals, the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study, the Department of Veterans Affairs, the International Parkinson and Movement Disorder Society, the Michael J. Fox Foundation for Parkinson’s Research, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, and Novartis; honoraria from AbbVie, Acadia, Biotie, the CHDI Foundation, Clintrex, Otsuka, Teva Pharmaceuticals, and UCB; license fee payments from the University of Pennsylvania for the Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in Parkinson’s Disease short form and the Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders in Parkinson’s Disease–Rating Scale; royalties from Wolters Kluwer; and fees for legal consultation for a lawsuit related to antipsychotic prescribing in a patient with Parkinson’s disease. Dr. Lyketsos has received grant support from AstraZeneca, the Associated Jewish Federation of Baltimore, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eisai, Elan, Forest, Functional Neuromodulation, GlaxoSmithKline, Lilly, the National Institute on Aging, NIMH, Pfizer, Ortho-McNeil, the National Football League, Novartis, and the Weinberg Foundation; payment as consultant or adviser from AbbVie, Adlyfe, Astellas, AstraZeneca, Avanir, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eisai, Elan, Eli Lilly, Forest, Genentech, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen, Lundbeck, Merz, the NFL Players Association, the NFL Benefits Office, Novartis, Orion, Otsuka, Pfizer, Servier, Supernus, Takeda, Wyeth, and Zinfandel; and honoraria or travel support from Forest, GlaxoSmithKline, Health Monitor, and Pfizer. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2015.15050648.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Aalten P, de Vugt ME, Jaspers N, et al: The course of neuropsychiatric symptoms in dementia, part I: findings from the two-year longitudinal Maasbed study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005; 20:523–530
2.
Geda YE, Schneider LS, Gitlin LN, et al: Neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer’s disease: past progress and anticipation of the future. Alzheimers Dement 2013; 9:602–608
3.
Kales HC, Zivin K, Kim HM, et al: Trends in antipsychotic use in dementia, 1999–2007. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2011; 68:190–197
4.
Schneider LS, Dagerman K, Insel PS: Efficacy and adverse effects of atypical antipsychotics for dementia: meta-analysis of randomized, placebo-controlled trials. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2006; 14:191–210
5.
Rosenberg PB, Drye LT, Martin BK, et al: Sertraline for the treatment of depression in Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2010; 18:136–145
6.
Brodaty H: Antidepressant treatment in Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 2011; 378:375–376
7.
Porsteinsson AP, Drye LT, Pollock BG, et al: Effect of citalopram on agitation in Alzheimer disease: the CitAD randomized clinical trial. JAMA 2014; 311:682–691
8.
Pollock BG, Mulsant BH, Rosen J, et al: Comparison of citalopram, perphenazine, and placebo for the acute treatment of psychosis and behavioral disturbances in hospitalized, demented patients. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:460–465
9.
Pollock BG, Mulsant BH, Rosen J, et al: A double-blind comparison of citalopram and risperidone for the treatment of behavioral and psychotic symptoms associated with dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2007; 15:942–952
10.
Cai T, Tian L, Wong PH, et al: Analysis of randomized comparative clinical trial data for personalized treatment selections. Biostatistics 2011; 12:270–282
11.
Drye LT, Ismail Z, Porsteinsson AP, et al: Citalopram for agitation in Alzheimer’s disease: design and methods. Alzheimers Dement 2012; 8:121–130
12.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198
13.
Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, et al: The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology 1994; 44:2308–2314
14.
Schneider LS, Olin JT, Doody RS, et al: Validity and reliability of the Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study–Clinical Global Impression of Change. The Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1997; 11(suppl 2):S22–S32
15.
Levin HS, High WM, Goethe KE, et al: The Neurobehavioural Rating Scale: assessment of the behavioural sequelae of head injury by the clinician. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1987; 50:183–193
16.
Werner P, Cohen-Mansfield J, Koroknay V, et al: The impact of a restraint-reduction program on nursing home residents. Geriatr Nurs 1994; 15:142–146
17.
Galasko D, Bennett D, Sano M, et al: An inventory to assess activities of daily living for clinical trials in Alzheimer’s disease. The Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1997; 11(suppl 2):S33–S39
18.
Drye LT, Spragg D, Devanand DP, et al: Changes in QTc interval in the citalopram for agitation in Alzheimer’s disease (CitAD) randomized trial. PLoS One 2014; 9:e98426
19.
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE): Donepezil, Galantamine, Rivastigmine, and Memantine for the Treatment of Alzheimer’s Disease (NICE Technology Appraisal Guidance TA214). London, NICE, Mar 23, 2011. http://www.nice.org.uk/nicemedia/live/13419/53619/53619.pdf
20.
US Food and Drug Administration (FDA): FDA Drug Safety Communication: Revised recommendations for Celexa (citalopram hydrobromide) related to a potential risk of abnormal heart rhythms with high doses. Silver Spring, Md, FDA, March 28, 2012. http://www.fda.gov/Drugs/DrugSafety/ucm297391.htm
21.
Coupland C, Dhiman P, Morriss R, et al: Antidepressant use and risk of adverse outcomes in older people: population based cohort study. BMJ 2011; 343:d4551

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 465 - 472
PubMed: 26771737

History

Received: 18 May 2015
Revision received: 19 August 2015
Accepted: 21 October 2015
Published online: 15 January 2016
Published in print: May 01, 2016

Authors

Details

Lon S. Schneider, M.D., M.S.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Constantine Frangakis, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Lea T. Drye, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
D.P. Devanand, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Christopher M. Marano, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Jacob Mintzer, M.D., M.B.A.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Benoit H. Mulsant, M.D., M.S.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Cynthia A. Munro, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Jeffery A. Newell, B.A.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Sonia Pawluczyk, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Gregory Pelton, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Bruce G. Pollock, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Anton P. Porsteinsson, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Peter V. Rabins, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Lisa Rein, Sc.M.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Paul B. Rosenberg, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
David Shade, J.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Daniel Weintraub, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Jerome Yesavage, M.D.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
Constantine G. Lyketsos, M.D., M.H.S.
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.
for the CitAD Research Group
From the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and the Department of Neurology, Keck School of Medicine of University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Biostatistics, the Department of Epidemiology, and the Center for Clinical Trials and Evidence Synthesis, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore; the Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and Columbia University Medical Center, New York; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins Medicine, Baltimore; the Clinical Biotechnology Research Institute, Roper St. Francis Healthcare, Charleston, S.C.; the Campbell Institute, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, and the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto; the Culture and Mental Health Laboratory, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; the Department of Psychiatry, University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry, Rochester, N.Y.; the Biostatistics Consulting Center, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee; the Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia; the Department of Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Palo Alto, Calif.; and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Schneider ([email protected]).

Funding Information

National Institute on Aging10.13039/100000049: P50AG05142, R01AG031348
Supported by National Institute on Aging and NIMH grant R01 AG031348 and in part by NIH grant P50 AG05142.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share