Skip to main content
Full access
Editorials
Published Online: 1 January 2017

The Eyes as Window to the Mind

In this issue, Moriuchi et al. report on a fascinating study (1) on a long-standing question: Why do children with autism look less at other people’s eyes, relative to typically developing children? The authors pit two theories against each other: gaze aversion and gaze indifference. Their results point to the latter, not the former, although the question of why remains.
One now quite old idea is that children with autism don’t see other people’s eyes as a window to the mind (2). Their well-established difficulties in attributing mental states to other people may mean that they see other people’s eyes as parts within the face that move, but they struggle to interpret what these moving parts mean. Whereas the typical child pays a lot of attention to where others are looking because they understand that what someone looks at indicates their mental state (what that person is interested in), children with autism may not appreciate the mentalistic significance of a person’s eyes. Nor might people with autism understand that a person’s eyes are a reflection of a range of mental states—cognitive, volitional, or affective.
Consider how when we try to interpret a person’s basic emotions, such as happy, sad, angry, afraid, disgust, or surprise (Figure 1A), people with or without autism score equally well if they are presented with the eyes or the mouth, although seeing the whole face leads to better performance. But when the task is to judge a complex emotion, such as arrogant, guilt, thoughtful, or revenge (Figure 1B), seeing the eyes alone results in as good performance as seeing the whole face, and both eyes and whole face are better than the mouth alone. Moreover, on such challenging tests, adults with Asperger’s syndrome show impaired performance when shown just the eyes.
FIGURE 1. Examples of Basic and Complex Emotional Stimulia
a Panels A and B show full face, eye, and mouth stimuli for basic and complex emotions, respectively. From Baron-Cohen et al. (3).
Using the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test, collecting data online, our group found that the impairment in autism is very clear, as is the sex difference (female superiority) in typical participants and the absence of any sex difference in autism (4), confirming earlier studies (5, 6) (Figure 2).
FIGURE 2. Results From a Study in Which the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test Was Administered to Males and Females With and Without Autisma
a Data from Baron-Cohen et al. (4). Panel A shows the mean scores on the test for males and females with and without autism. Panel B is a sample item from the test; participants were asked to select which word best describes what the pictured individual is thinking or feeling.
Performance on the Eyes test is associated with activity in the amygdala and in the left inferior frontal gyrus (7, 8); the female advantage on the test is associated with greater activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus, and the autistic deficit is associated with reduced activity in this same area. This deficit is also seen in parents of such children, suggesting that it arises for genetic reasons (9, 10). Lesion studies confirm the importance of these brain regions in mentalizing the eyes (11).
Candidate gene studies suggest that sex steroid pathway genes play a role in performance on the Eyes test (12), and a whole genome study of more than 88,000 people (13) found an association between performance on the Eyes test and chromosome 3p26.1 in typical females. The heritable aspect of mentalizing the eyes is apparent in at-risk infants who are siblings of children with autism (14). A unique study (15) also showed that performance on the Eyes test is negatively associated with levels of prenatal testosterone, which may explain the typical sex differences on this test. Administering testosterone to typical women reduces their scores on the Eyes test to typical male levels (16) and reduces brain functional connectivity (17). Administering the hormone oxytocin seems to have the opposite effect, improving scores on the Eyes test (18). Naturally, individual differences in reading the mind in the eyes are not just a function of our biology, as postnatal social experience likely amplifies these prenatal determinants. For this reason, we should expect that impaired performance on this test might be observed in a range of clinical groups, not just in autism, for diverse reasons.

References

1.
Moriuchi JM, Klin A, Jones W: Mechanisms of diminished attention to eyes in autism. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174:26–35
2.
Baron-Cohen S: Perceptual role taking and protodeclarative pointing in autism. Br J Dev Psychol 1989; 7:113–127
3.
Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Jolliffe T: Is there a “language of the eyes”? Evidence from normal adults and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Vis Cogn 1997; 4:311–331
4.
Baron-Cohen S, Bowen DC, Holt RJ, et al: The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test: complete absence of typical sex difference in ∼400 men and women with autism. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0136521
5.
Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, et al: Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997; 38:813–822
6.
Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, et al: The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42:241–251
7.
Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Wheelwright S, et al: Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 1999; 11:1891–1898
8.
Holt RJ, Chura LR, Lai M-C, et al: “Reading the Mind in the Eyes”: an fMRI study of adolescents with autism and their siblings. Psychol Med 2014; 44:3215–3227
9.
Baron-Cohen S, Ring H, Chitnis X, et al: fMRI of parents of children with Asperger syndrome: a pilot study. Brain Cogn 2006; 61:122–130
10.
Baron-Cohen S, Hammer J: Parents of children with Asperger syndrome: what is the cognitive phenotype? J Cogn Neurosci 1997; 9:548–554
11.
Shaw P, Bramham J, Lawrence EJ, et al: Differential effects of lesions of the amygdala and prefrontal cortex on recognizing facial expressions of complex emotions. J Cogn Neurosci 2005; 17:1410–1419
12.
Chakrabarti B, Dudbridge F, Kent L, et al: Genes related to sex steroids, neural growth, and social-emotional behavior are associated with autistic traits, empathy, and Asperger syndrome. Autism Res 2009; 2:157–177
13.
Warrier V, Grasby K, Uzefovsky F, et al: Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions, and brain anatomy. (bioRxiv preprint first posted online Oct 19, 2016)
14.
Elsabbagh M, Mercure E, Hudry K, et al: Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Curr Biol 2012; 22:338–342
15.
Chapman E, Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, et al: Fetal testosterone and empathy: evidence from the empathy quotient (EQ) and the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test. Soc Neurosci 2006; 1:135–148
16.
van Honk J, Schutter DJ, Bos PA, et al: Testosterone administration impairs cognitive empathy in women depending on second-to-fourth digit ratio. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011; 108:3448–3452
17.
Bos PA, Hofman D, Hermans EJ, et al: Testosterone reduces functional connectivity during the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 68:194–201
18.
Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM, et al: Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2010; 67:692–694

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1 - 2
PubMed: 28041004

History

Accepted: October 2016
Published online: 1 January 2017
Published in print: January 01, 2017

Authors

Details

Simon Baron-Cohen, Ph.D.
From the Autism Research Centre, Cambridge University, Cambridge, U.K.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Baron-Cohen ([email protected]).

Funding Information

The author reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share