While some children “grow out” of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) by adulthood, many do not. Prospective studies have found that 15%−45% of children with ADHD will have the full syndrome into adulthood, and a further 25%−48% continue to have impairing symptoms (
1–
5). This variable clinical outcome prompts the following question: How does the adult brain differ in those whose ADHD persists after childhood compared with those who remit? Identifying the “plastic” brain regions that drive remission could provide targets for interventions to improve the clinical course of the disorder.
A prominent neurodevelopmental model posits that ADHD onset is linked to early aberrations in the striato-thalamic circuitry and associated deficits in “bottom-up” cognitive processes that persist into adulthood, regardless of clinical outcome (
6). In support of this model, adult anomalies in subcortical, thalamic activation during response preparation (a bottom-up cognitive process) were found to reflect a childhood history of ADHD even in those who remitted (
7). The other tenet of this model is that the variable adult outcome of ADHD is tied to developmental trajectories of the prefrontal cortex and associated “top-down” cognitive processes (
6). This is in line with behavioral observations indicating that remission from ADHD is associated with normalization in higher-order cognitive functions (
2). However, the delineation of the underlying prefrontal cortical circuitry and its links with adult outcome of childhood ADHD are currently not well characterized.
In the present study, we examined adults with a childhood history of ADHD followed prospectively. Using two imaging modalities, we delineated brain activation patterns during a “top-down” task of motor inhibition. We focused on motor inhibition because it has been viewed as a core deficit in ADHD, associated particularly with impulsivity (
8,
9). We used the stop-signal task, which—in functional MRI (fMRI) studies—reliably activates both prefrontal cortical (in particular the inferior frontal cortex) and caudate regions, central to neurodevelopmental models of ADHD (
10–
13). However, motor inhibition unfolds rapidly, in hundreds of milliseconds. While fMRI provides excellent spatial resolution, its temporal resolution is on the orders of seconds. Therefore, to provide better temporal resolution, we incorporated magnetoencephalography, an imaging technique that maps the magnetic fields evoked by populations of activated cortical neurons. Thus, magnetoencephalography provides a direct observation of neuronal activity during inhibitory processes, which has temporal resolution in the order of milliseconds.
We tested the hypothesis that the variable adult outcome of childhood ADHD is linked to differences in cortical activity. We primarily focused on associations between adult outcome of childhood ADHD and activity in the inferior frontal cortex during motor inhibition. By contrast, we predicted that a childhood history of ADHD would result in fixed anomalies in striatal processing that are present even among those who remit.
Method
Participants
A total of 181 individuals participated in the study, with 63 completing the fMRI and magnetoencephalography task versions, 85 completing the fMRI version only, and 33 completing the magnetoencephalography version only. Eighty-two participants (46%) had a childhood history of ADHD; the remainder had no history of ADHD (“never affected”). Current adult ADHD symptoms were assessed using the clinician-administered ADHD Rating Scale, Version IV, providing prompts appropriate for late adolescent and young adult groups (
14). Seventy-three (89%) of the individuals with ADHD entered National Institutes of Health research studies as children, when they received a diagnosis using the Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents for parents (
15). Nine participants entered the study as young adults, and their childhood history of ADHD symptoms was confirmed through parental interviews. In line with DSM-5, the persistent ADHD group was defined by the presence of five or more impairing symptoms of inattention, five or more symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity, or both (
16). Remission was defined by having four or fewer symptoms in each of the two symptom dimensions. The presence of other psychiatric disorders was established through the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (
17). Two experienced clinicians (P.S. and W.S.) conducted the interviews (interrater reliabilities; kappa >0.9). Contrasts were made against 99 individuals never affected by ADHD, drawn from a longitudinal study of typical brain development. General exclusion criteria were a full-scale IQ <80 (estimated from age-appropriate versions of the Wechsler intelligence scales), neurological disorders known to affect brain structure, current substance dependence, or psychotic disorders. Psychostimulant medication was used among 48.6% of adults with persistent ADHD and 12.7% of those with four or fewer symptoms (remitted). To mitigate acute effects, all participants stopped psychostimulant medication the day prior to scanning. The institutional review board of the National Human Genome Research Institute approved the research protocol, and written informed consent was obtained.
Behavioral Task
The stop-signal task was a rapid, mixed-trial, event-related paradigm used in previous studies (
18,
19). On all trials, a fixation cross appeared centrally against a black background for 500 ms, followed by a white “X” or “O” go signal for 1,000 ms. Participants were instructed to press the right button on a button box for an “X” and the left button for an “O.” During the unexpected, infrequent stop trials (25% of trials, randomly dispersed), after the “X” or “O” appeared the background changed from black to red (stop signal), and participants were instructed to inhibit their motor response. The first stop signal appeared 250 ms after the go signal. In subsequent stop trials, the time interval between the go and stop signals (i.e., inhibit delay) adapted to individual performance. It became 50 ms longer after a successful trial, making it harder to inhibit, and 50 ms shorter after an unsuccessful trial, making it easier to inhibit. This algorithm ensured that the task was equally challenging for all individuals, resulting in approximately 50% successful and 50% failed stop trials. Trials were separated by 750 ms, with 86 trials in each run. Participants completed four runs (344 trials) in the fMRI and eight runs (688 trials) in the magnetoencephalography experiment.
fMRI Acquisition
Gradient echo planar images were acquired on a 3-T GE Signa scanner (General Electric, Milwaukee, Wisc.): 26 contiguous, 4 mm axial slices obtained with single-shot gradient echo T2*-weighting, aligned to the anterior-posterior commissure. Images were aligned to a T1-weighted anatomical scan (see the data supplement accompanying the online version of this article).
Magnetoencephalography Acquisition
Magnetoencephalography data were acquired using a 275-gradiometer whole-helmet magnetoencephalography system (CTF Systems, Coquitlam, British Columbia, Canada) at 600 Hz. The locations of three fiducial points (nasial and left/right auricular) were determined for localization of magnetoencephalography signal sources.
Data Analysis
Behavioral data.
Performance was measured by 1) go trial accuracy and reaction time, 2) stop trial accuracy, 3) “inhibit delay” or the time interval between the onset of the go and stop signals, and 4) the stop-signal reaction time calculated as the mean go reaction time minus the mean inhibit delay in participants who inhibited successfully on 50% of the stop trials (
8,
19). If a participant’s stop trial accuracy deviated from 50%, an interpolation algorithm was used to compute stop-signal reaction time: the mean inhibit delay was subtracted from the go reaction time at the Xth percentile of the go reaction times, where X is the participant’s percent accuracy on stop trials.
fMRI data.
fMRI data were analyzed using Analysis of Functional Neuroimages (
https://afni.nimh.nih.gov/). Preprocessing included slice timing correction, motion correction, spatial normalization to Talairach space, and smoothing (kernel full width at half maximum=8 mm) (see the
online data supplement).
In the event-related fMRI analysis, brain activation was considered for three contrasts. First, successful inhibition compared with successful go trials: this contrast holds constant (successful) performance but differs on task demand and motor response. Second, failed inhibition compared with successful go: these two conditions have the same motor response but differ on task demand and performance. Third, failed compared with successful inhibition: this contrast holds the task demands constant, but the conditions differ on motor response and performance. These contrasts parse out neural activation pertaining to task demands, performance success, and motor response and identify circuitry mediating successful and failed inhibition. Incorrect go trials were very infrequent and not included in the analyses.
We focused on two regions of interest implicated in motor inhibition by meta-analyses of fMRI and human lesion studies, namely the inferior frontal cortex and the caudate (
10,
11,
20). These were defined using the Talairach-Tournoux Atlas, applied to each participant’s normalized brain.
Differences between the persistent, remitted, and never-affected adults were calculated for each of the three contrasts using factorial analyses of variance (ANOVAs). Given the four regions of interest, significance was set at p<0.05/4=0.01; significant group differences were examined in pairwise, post hoc analyses using Fisher’s least significant difference. Direct comparisons between adults with and without a childhood history of ADHD were made by categorizing individuals according to the presence or absence of a childhood diagnosis for ADHD using t tests. We additionally explored associa-tions with the number of adult inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms using correlation analysis.
Finally, we conducted exploratory whole-brain analyses. Results were corrected for multiple testing using the most current version of 3dClustSim (
https://afni.nimh.nih.gov/pub/dist/doc/program_help/3dClustSim.html), which corrects for issues with parametric approaches for preventing type 1 errors (
21). Significance was declared at a voxel-wise p value <0.05 and a cluster-corrected alpha level <0.05 (k=1, minimum cluster size=512 voxels).
We repeated all analyses comparing those with childhood ADHD (i.e., combining persistent and remitted participants into one group) against the group of never-affected adults.
Magnetoencephalography data.
Using MNE-Python (
22) and Analysis of Functional Neuroimages, eye blinks and other subject-related artifacts in the raw signal were removed, and data were visually inspected to rule out other anomalies. Because we were interested in the neural processes underlying inhibition, only inhibit trials were analyzed further. Each trial was parsed into individual epochs such that time point 0 marked the onset of the stop signal. Data were averaged across trials within condition (successful inhibition, unsuccessful inhibition) and then source-localized using DICS (
23) for the frequency bands: delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), low gamma (30–55 Hz), and high gamma (65–100 Hz). Further details are presented in the
online data supplement. ANOVAs compared neural responses between the outcome groups, and correlation analyses tested for associations with ADHD symptoms. We applied a false discovery rate (q=0.05) to correct for multiple comparisons over each brain image (
24). Robustness analysis tested whether results held when we considered possible effects of psychostimulant medication.
Results
Do Cortical Rather Than Subcortical Anomalies Reflect the Adult Outcome of Childhood ADHD?
The three outcome groups did not differ behaviorally in the fMRI task version (
Table 1) but did in the magnetoencephalography version, which had twice as many trials (N=688 trials). In the magnetoencephalography version, the persistent ADHD group was less accurate on go trials and had a longer stop signal response time compared with the never-affected group (
Table 2).
We tested the hypothesis that activation in the cortical (inferior frontal cortex) but not subcortical region of interest (caudate) was tied to the degree of persistence of childhood ADHD into adulthood. First, categorical contrasts were drawn between the persistent ADHD, remitted ADHD, and never-affected groups. In the fMRI data, group differences emerged in inferior frontal cortex activation during failed inhibition when contrasted against either successful inhibition (left: F=4.37, df=2, 145, p=0.01; right [fell short of statistical significance]: F=3.67, df=2, 145, p=0.03) or go trials (left: F=5.07, df=2, 145, p=0.007). The group effects were due to reduced inferior frontal cortex activity during failed inhibition in the persistent group compared with both the remitted group (right: p=0.01; left: p=0.004) and the never-affected group (left: p=0.009) (
Figure 1). The remitted and never-affected groups did not differ significantly (p>0.05). As predicted, no outcome group differences emerged for the subcortical regions of interest and the left and right caudate.
We followed up this finding with symptom-level analyses. Higher levels of adult hyperactivity-impulsivity were associated with decreased activation in the bilateral inferior frontal cortex during failed inhibition contrasted against either successful inhibition (right: r=–0.27, p=0.03; left: r=–0.28, p=0.03) or go trials (left: r=–0.33, p=0.009) (see Figure S1 in the online data supplement). There were no significant associations with symptoms of inattention.
In the whole-brain fMRI analysis, no clusters showed significant differences between the three groups. However, we identified a bilateral cerebellar cluster (799 voxels) where increased adult hyperactive-impulsive symptoms were associated with decreased activation during successful inhibition (contrasted against go trials) (
Figure 2). An inferior parietal cluster (492 voxels) showed a nearly significant association (cluster-corrected α<0.06) with adult hyperactive-impulsive symptoms.
In summary, as hypothesized, anomalous activity in the inferior frontal cortex, but not the caudate, was associated with the adult outcome of childhood ADHD during failed inhibition. Links also emerged between the degree of adult persistence of childhood hyperactive-impulsive symptoms and activation during inhibitory processes in both the right inferior frontal cortex and cerebellum.
Do Subcortical Rather Than Cortical Anomalies Reflect Childhood ADHD?
Next, we categorized individuals according to the presence or absence of a childhood diagnosis for ADHD, predicting differential activation in the subcortical but not cortical regions of interest. The group with a history of childhood ADHD showed reduced right caudate activity when successfully inhibiting (t=2.28, df=1, 146, p=0.02) and increased activity in the right caudate when failing to inhibit (t=–2.18, df=1, 146, p=0.03). As hypothesized, no differences were found for the cortical region of interest (inferior frontal cortex) (all p values >0.1). Furthermore, we did not detect any differences that reflected a childhood history of ADHD in whole-brain analyses.
The Patterns of Neuronal Activation Tied to the Adult Outcome of Childhood ADHD
Given our fMRI findings, we used magnetoencephalography to characterize further the cortical inhibitory processes that reflect adult outcome groups, with additional focus on associations with the degree of adult persistence of childhood hyperactivity-impulsivity. We examined the neural processes relating to successful and failed inhibitory events separately, using exploratory whole-brain analysis.
We found significant differences between the three groups during successful inhibition. These differences occurred in two frequency bands at 500 ms–600 ms after the stop signal in cerebellar and precuneal/parietal regions; these regions overlapped with those identified in the fMRI analysis. Specifically, in the delta frequency band the three groups differed in evoked responses 550 ms–600 ms after the stop signal (minimum: F=6.05, df=2, 93, p<0.05, false discovery rate-corrected) in two clusters. The first cluster (697 voxels) encompassed the cerebellum (
Figure 3); the second cluster (69 voxels) was centered on the left precuneus (see Figure S2 in the
online data supplement). Pairwise group analyses showed that neural activity was highest in never-affected individuals and lowest in the persistent ADHD group, with the remitted group showing intermediate levels of activation.
In the theta band, the three outcome groups also showed differences in neural activity at 500 ms–550 ms after the stop signal (minimum: F=5.25, df=2, 93, p<0.05, false discovery rate-corrected) in a bilateral cerebellar cluster (1,002 voxels) that overlapped with the cerebellar cluster found in the delta band (see
Figure 3). As in the delta band, the lowest amplitude was observed in the persistent ADHD group, and the highest in the never-affected group, with the remitted group showing intermediate levels of activation. Further clusters showing differences between outcome groups were noted in the right inferior parietal cluster (341 voxels) and right cuneus/posterior cingulate cluster (172 voxels) (see Figure S3 in the
online data supplement).
In analyses of adult symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity, we detected associations with neural responses in the right inferior prefrontal cortex during successful inhibition; this region overlapped with the cortical region of interest in the fMRI analysis. This theta band cluster (42 voxels; minimum: t=4.06, p<0.05, false discovery rate-corrected) was confined to the time window during which successful inhibition typically occurs (i.e., 300 ms–350 ms after the stop signal). During this “inhibitory” time window, individuals with persistent ADHD exhibited decreased theta activity within the right inferior frontal cortex, compared with those who had remitted (
Figure 4). In summary, the degree of persistence into adulthood of hyperactive-impulsive symptoms was associated with neuronal activity during the time window of the act of inhibition.
Discussion
By combining fMRI and magnetoencephalography, we have pinpointed the location and time window of neuronal activity that is tied to the adult outcome of childhood ADHD. This detailed spatiotemporal mapping of the neural substrate tied to remission could guide more targeted interventions for improving the clinical course of ADHD. We separated the cortico-cerebellar processes that underpin adult outcome from the striatal processes that do not. Specifically, we found inhibition-related anomalies in the inferior frontal cortex to be associated with the adult outcome of childhood ADHD, particularly the persistence of hyperactive-impulsive, rather than inattentive, symptoms. Leveraging the exquisite temporal resolution of magnetoencephalography, we pinpointed these prefrontal anomalies to the time window covering the act of inhibition. Additional activations in cerebellar and precuneal/inferior parietal regions also reflected adult outcome. Here, magnetoencephalography indicated that neuronal activation differences occurred relatively late after the onset of the stop sign, compatible with disruption of cerebellar performance monitoring processes. Not all neural differences were tied to adult outcome. Subcortical, right caudate anomalies reflected a childhood history of ADHD and were present even in those who had remitted as adults.
The Neurobiology of the Variable Adult Outcome of Childhood ADHD
What do these findings tell us about the processes underpinning adult remission compared with the persistence of childhood ADHD? Our results support a model in which prefrontally mediated cognitive processes are tied to the adult outcome of ADHD (
6,
10). In general, the remitted ADHD group did not differ from the never-affected group, compatible with the concept that remission is underpinned by a normalization of inferior frontal cortex activity. Although not suggested by the original model (
6), it is noteworthy that cerebellar and parietal regions also showed partial normalization with remission from ADHD. Also noteworthy is that adult severity of hyperactive-impulsive symptoms rather than inattentive symptoms accounted for these findings. This is in line with an influential model of ADHD, which links inhibitory deficits with hyperactive-impulsive symptoms. While there is empirical support from neuropsychological studies for this model (
25–
28), there are also some conflicting reports (
29–
31). Prior neuroimaging studies have primarily examined ADHD-related inhibitory deficits using diagnostic categories and did not include separate analyses of hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive symptom dimen-sions. Mapping the neural substrate of inhibition onto symptom dimensions, as we attempted in the present study, is an area of active interest. Additionally, more recent models have linked inattention, particularly deficient sustained attention and increased distractibility, with intrusions of the default mode network (prominent during introspective processing) into neural activity tied to task performance (
32–
34). A future goal is to examine whether such dysregulated interactions between brain networks account for the persistence of inattention into adulthood.
As expected, only caudate anomalies were related to childhood symptoms of ADHD irrespective of later outcome. This is consonant with a recent, independent finding of atypical subcortical—specifically thalamic—activity among adults with a childhood diagnosis of ADHD, regardless of adult status (
7). This supports the concept that fixed subcortical abnormalities drive the onset of ADHD and remain relatively unchanged throughout development and recovery (
6). We further demonstrate that these subcortical anomalies are not only present during bottom-up cognitive processes but also in higher-order processes of cognitive control.
Mapping the Patterns of Neuronal Activation Sensitive to the Adult Outcome of Childhood ADHD
Using magnetoencephalography, we showed that the link between adult hyperactive-impulsive symptoms and right inferior prefrontal neuronal activation was confined to 300 ms–350 ms following the stop signal. This time window overlaps with the time it takes an individual to successfully stop an already initiated response (
35–
37). This confirms prior event-related potential studies in healthy individuals showing that right inferior frontal cortical activation during successful inhibition occurs during this time window (
38). Here, we further demonstrate that this epoch of prefrontal inhibitory processing is sensitive to the adult outcome of childhood ADHD.
Activation differences associated with adult ADHD symptoms extended to the cerebellum, in both fMRI and magnetoencephalography. This finding is congruent with the role of the cerebellum in response inhibition. Prior positron emission tomography studies found cerebellar activation during the inhibition of prepotent responses in Stroop tasks, while deficits in the Stroop task are seen in patients with cerebellar lesions (
39,
40). Similarly, slower go-signal reaction times in the go/no-go task have been linked to the extent of cerebellar lesions (
41). Such human lesion studies implicate the cerebellum in response inhibition and align with previous reports of subtle cerebellar anatomic anomalies in adults with persistent ADHD (
42). Using magnetoencephalography, we additionally find that ADHD symptoms were associated with cerebellar activity in a time window 500 ms–600 ms after the stop signal. This time window points to anomalies in the processing of feedback around successful inhibition, which is reliant on prefrontal-cerebellar interactions (
43,
44). It suggests that adults with persistent symptoms show impairment in such processing, whereas those who have remitted may have more typical performance monitoring.
Electrophysiological differences that tracked with adult outcome lay in the theta and delta bands. Interestingly, event-related potential studies also show augmented power in both theta and delta frequencies during inhibition (
45). Specifically, frontal theta activation has been observed during response inhibition (
46,
47), while delta oscillations are observed during the evaluation of motor inhibition (
46). Indeed, frontal-midline theta is associated with a wide range of cognitive processes, including working memory, spatial navigation, and episodic memory, thus indicating a general role in action monitoring (
46). Our findings support the emerging picture that both theta and delta bands are sensitive both to increased cognitive demands during response inhibition and to the severity of adult symptoms of ADHD (
48).
Limitations
This study has several limitations. First, magnetoencephalography is best suited to detect cortical activation and could not specify the characteristics of the caudate anomalies we found using fMRI. Second, nine participants were not followed from childhood but recruited in adulthood. Consequently, we had to establish the presence of a childhood history of ADHD retrospectively. Nevertheless, whenever possible, we also collected collateral information about childhood history and the results held when we reanalyzed the sample excluding the individuals who entered the study as adults (see the
online data supplement). Third, follow-up assessments in adulthood were not blind. Consequently, there may have been a bias toward underestimating ADHD symptoms in adults who entered as typically developing children and toward overestimating adult ADHD symptoms in those who entered the study as children with the diagnosis. This is of relevance given the recent interest in the possibility of adult-onset ADHD (
49–
51). Finally, regional neural activation is partly task-dependent (
52), and it is interesting to consider whether the same general finding of cortical anomalies in those with persistent symptoms, but typical activity in remitters, would emerge using different cognitive probes. One pertinent study found greater integration of thalamo-cortical activity in remitters compared with individuals whose symptoms persisted during a task of response preparation, while childhood history was characterized by a general hypoactivation (
7). This adult imaging study embedded within a longitudinal study starting in childhood provides hypotheses about mechanisms of recovery. Ideally, these should be tested and refined through the future collection of longitudinal imaging data.
Clinical Implications
We show that the “plasticity” underpinning remission from ADHD lies in inferior prefrontal/cerebellar rather than striatal regions. This implies that cognitive or pharmacological interventions targeting prefrontal or cerebellar functions (such as executive or temporal information processes) may prove more successful than interventions that “train” more automatic processes, supported by the striatum. In devising novel interventions, it is noteworthy that both fMRI and magnetoencephalography have been successfully used as real-time neurofeedback devices, allowing participants to modulate task-related brain activation and oscillatory rhythms associated with awareness and attention (
53,
54). For example, it is possible to provide real-time magnetoencephalography feedback on the right inferior frontal gyrus during critical time windows for response inhibition (
55,
56). Thus, the tools we use to map the cognitive deficits underlying persistent symptoms of ADHD could feasibly also be used for their remediation. Finally, the findings are consonant with studies that tie recovery from ADHD to a convergence toward typical brain structure and function (
5).