Skip to main content
Depressive disorders are a leading cause of global disability, afflicting approximately 280 million individuals worldwide each year (1). In the United States, more than one in five individuals will experience a lifetime depressive disorder, diagnoses and service utilization are surging, and direct health care costs exceed $68 billion annually (26). These unfortunate observations underscore the need to develop a better understanding of the neural systems that underlie depression.
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a heterogeneous phenotype that typically emerges in late adolescence or early adulthood (7, 8). Clinical presentation can be transient or recurrent, with periods of waxing and waning impairment and distress. Comorbidity with anxiety disorders, substance misuse, and other illnesses is common (9, 10). Given this phenotypic, developmental, and—in all likelihood—etiological complexity, it is unsurprising that neuroimaging studies of MDD have implicated a diverse array of brain regions, including the amygdala, ventral striatum, thalamus, and cingulate (11, 12).
Among the regions linked to depression, the amygdala has received some of the most intense empirical scrutiny. This body of research has led many to conclude that amygdala hyperreactivity confers increased risk for MDD and other, often co-occurring internalizing illnesses (13). This hypothesis reflects three lines of evidence. First, moderately large cross-sectional studies of youth and young adults (sample sizes of 72–1,042 [1417]) suggest that amygdala function—including heightened reactivity and elevated resting activity—is most consistently associated with internalizing risk (e.g., familial, temperamental), not the severity of acute symptoms. Moreover, prospective longitudinal work (N=340 [18]) shows that heightened amygdala reactivity to fearful and angry faces is associated with the future emergence of self-reported mood and anxiety symptoms in young adults (controlling for baseline symptoms). Yet this prospective association is notably selective and only manifests among individuals exposed to negative life events (NLEs) during the follow-up period (i.e., Amygdala×NLEsInternalizing). Ancillary analyses show that this prospective association is 1) numerically greater for negative (“threat-related”) than neutral faces; 2) significant in both hemispheres (albeit more strongly in the right); and 3) significant for anhedonia (e.g., nothing interesting/fun) and anxious apprehension (e.g., nervous), but not depressive affect (e.g., sad, depressed) or anxious arousal (e.g., racing heart). While conceptually important and statistically significant (p=0.002), this association is far too small to be practically useful (d=0.34, R2=2.7%), a point we return to later. Second, clinically effective mood and anxiety treatments (e.g., SSRIs) dampen amygdala reactivity to negative faces and aversive challenges, consistent with a causal role (19). Third, three recent coordinate-based meta-analyses (CBMAs)—all adhering to methodological best practices and collectively encompassing dozens of studies and thousands of participants—provide convergent evidence of left amygdala hyperreactivity in individuals with MDD (11, 20, 21).
Despite this progress, it is clear that most of the work necessary to understand the nature and degree of the amygdala’s contribution to depression remains undone. Consider the CBMA evidence. To ensure an adequate number of studies, all of the meta-analytic teams were forced to engage in substantial “lumping,” and their results reflect a mixture of adults and youth, medicated and unmedicated cases, and a panoply of emotional and cognitive tasks. Janiri and colleagues found evidence of left amygdala hyperreactivity, but this was only evident at a liberal threshold, and only when pooling studies of MDD and anxiety (21). Li and Wang reported significant hyperreactivity in the left amygdala to emotional faces and scenes in individuals with current depressive disorders, but this was only found when aggregating positive and negative stimuli (11). In the most comprehensive analysis, McTeague and colleagues observed significant hyperreactivity in the left amygdala to emotional stimuli in individuals with interview-verified MDD or anxiety diagnoses (20). Ancillary analyses suggested that these effects were largely driven by studies of negative faces and scenes (20, 21). While these results clearly show that left amygdala reactivity to negative stimuli is elevated, on average at least, among individuals with MDD, it remains unclear whether this association reflects differences in the perception of negative faces, the generation of negative affect to aversive stimuli (e.g., unpleasant scenes, threat of shock), or some combination of the two (22).
But the most significant and often overlooked limitation of the CBMA evidence is the raw input, the grist for the meta-analytic mill. While all of the CBMAs have impressively large pooled samples, the size of the constituent imaging studies is worrisomely small. In the most recent CBMA (N=2,383 [11]), the median sample size was just 39 participants—19 cases and 20 controls—far too small to provide stable conclusions, even under the most generous (and frankly unrealistic) assumptions (23). For a benchmark “large” effect (d=0.80 or R2=14%) and a liberal whole-brain corrected threshold (αone-tailed=0.01, ZCritical=2.33), the power to detect case/control differences in activation is just above chance (53.1%). In the absence of publication, confirmation, or other biases favoring particular outcomes, CBMAs derived from underpowered studies are vulnerable to false negatives (24). But in the presence of such biases, underpowered studies will tend to capitalize on chance sampling variation and questionable research practices in ways that optimistically bias meta-analytic results—an outcome clearly demonstrated in the candidate gene literature (2528).
From this perspective, the new report from Tamm and colleagues (29) in this issue is a welcome addition to the literature. Leveraging data acquired from >20,000 older UK Biobank participants (median age=64 years), the authors estimated associations between three depression phenotypes and amygdala reactivity to negative faces with an unprecedented degree of statistical precision (27). Depression phenotypes included an ad hoc four-item self-report scale of acute (past 2 weeks) depressive symptoms, self-reported lifetime depression diagnosis, and probable lifetime major depression based on a diagnostic questionnaire. None of the assessments employed trained interviewers, and only the last used formal diagnostic criteria (for a detailed critique of depression phenotyping in the UK Biobank, see Cai et al. [30]). Individual differences in amygdala reactivity were quantified in an unbiased manner using a bilateral amygdala region-of-interest. Notably, both the data and code are publically available, facilitating future use by other investigators.
Tamm and colleagues’ analyses revealed null associations between amygdala reactivity to negative faces and self-reported symptoms and lifetime diagnoses. Relations between amygdala reactivity and the much stricter diagnostic questionnaire were numerically stronger and statistically significant (p=0.01). Nonetheless, the magnitude of this association was vanishingly small (d=0.03, R2=0.03%) and nonsignificant in models that included demographic covariates (p=0.13). The authors conclude by noting that “an association between depression and amygdala responses to negative faces is not likely to be as large as previously suggested….[and] that amygdala responses to negative facial expressions should not be considered an important feature/biomarker of depressive symptoms, at least not in the general population.”
Tamm and colleagues’ observations add to a growing body of psychiatric imaging research demonstrating that amygdala hyperreactivity and other popular candidate biomarkers explain statistically significant but quantitatively negligible amounts of disease-relevant information—risk, status, treatment response, course, and so on—in large samples. This pessimistic conclusion is hardly specific to the amygdala. A recent meta-analysis demonstrated that dampened ventral striatum reactivity to reward is significantly (p=0.007) and consistently (nine studies; median N=91) associated with the future emergence of depression, consistent with a causal role (12). Yet the strength of this small-but-reliable association is far too weak (R2=1%) to be useful for screening, clinical, or treatment development purposes.
From a conceptual perspective, Tamm and colleagues’ diagnostic-questionnaire results are reasonably well aligned with work in younger populations (reviewed above), suggesting that 1) higher levels of amygdala reactivity to negative faces probabilistically increase the likelihood of anhedonia and anxiety symptoms among individuals exposed to NLEs; 2) amygdala reactivity is more strongly associated with state-independent risk than acute symptoms; and 3) on average, amygdala reactivity to negative faces and scenes is elevated in groups of individuals with verified acute MDD. Individuals with MDD show a wide variety of clinical presentations, and this body of evidence is consistent with the possibility that amygdala hyperreactivity is only etiologically relevant for a subset of patients and symptoms. Determining whether this is true or simply wishful thinking is a key challenge for the future. From a mechanistic perspective, the small-but-reliable “hits” uncovered by Big Data studies—including Tamm and colleagues’ diagnostic-questionnaire results—do not preclude much larger effects with targeted biological interventions (12, 31). Indeed, work in animals demonstrates that focal perturbations of specific amygdala cell types can have dramatic, complex, and even opposing consequences for reward- (“wanting”) and anxiety-related behaviors (32, 33).
In sum, work conducted over the past decade has yielded steady advances in our understanding of depression. Yet the underlying neurobiological mechanisms remain elusive, actionable biomarkers remain out of reach, existing treatments are far from curative, and relapse and recurrence are common (3436). Tamm and colleagues’ report serves as a sober reminder that simple box-and-arrow neurobiological explanations—which equate amygdala hyperreactivity with depression independent of clinical presentation, severity, disease stage, developmental period, adversity exposure, imaging technique, and fMRI paradigm—are no longer tenable.
Addressing these challenges will require an increased investment in psychiatric research, one commensurate with the staggering burden that depression and anxiety impose on global public health. UK Biobank and other Big Data studies (e.g., ABCD, All of Us) clearly have an important role to play in overcoming these challenges, but to be maximally useful the next generation of biobank and large-scale psychiatric studies will need to overcome the significant limitations of existing ones. This will require the recruitment of demographically representative samples and adequate representation of severe psychopathology, rigorous psychiatric phenotyping, and reliable imaging approaches—three notable limitations of the Tamm study (10, 30, 3740). To really move the needle on our understanding—and ultimately on clinical practice—we will need to move beyond negative-face paradigms and other kinds of tried-and-true experimental challenges (31). Even if the amygdala is mechanistically involved in the development of maladaptive anhedonia or anxiety—as suggested by prior work in humans and animals—then conventional negative-face paradigms are fundamentally the wrong experimental assay. Some of these challenges can be overcome by appropriately focused “Medium Data” projects (N=200–2,000; e.g., Tulsa 1000) or by pooling data via existing consortia (e.g., ENIGMA). It is also worth reminding ourselves that the amygdala is a heterogeneous collection of nuclei linked by a network of microcircuits (41). Fully understanding the amygdala’s relevance to depression and other illnesses requires that future studies more fully embrace this neuroanatomical complexity. From the perspective of prediction, it is clear that cross-validated multivariate machine-learning approaches and related techniques—which quantitatively synthesize multiple sources of imaging and nonimaging information at the population or patient levels—are more likely to yield clinically useful tools than studies focused on isolated “hot spots” of brain function or structure (31). A greater emphasis on reliable dimensional phenotypes (e.g., anhedonia) and the development of integrative cross-species models promises to further accelerate efforts to alleviate the suffering caused by depression (12, 31).

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge assistance from K. DeYoung, L. Friedman, and J. Smith and critical feedback from A. Etkin and N. Kalin.

References

1.
GBD 2019 Diseases and Injuries Collaborators: Global burden of 369 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet 2020; 396:1204–1222 (interactive dashboard at https://vizhub.healthdata.org/gbd-compare/)
2.
Dieleman JL, Cao J, Chapin A, et al: US health care spending by payer and health condition, 1996–2016. JAMA 2020; 323:863–884
3.
SAMHSA: Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: results from the 2018 National Survey on Drug Use and Health. Rockville, MD, Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, 2019
4.
Olfson M, Blanco C, Wall MM, et al: Treatment of common mental disorders in the United States: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions-III. J Clin Psychiatry 2019; 80:18m12532
5.
Hasin DS, Sarvet AL, Meyers JL, et al: Epidemiology of adult DSM-5 Major Depressive Disorder and its specifiers in the United States. JAMA Psychiatry 2018; 75:336–346
6.
Brody DJ, Gu Q: Antidepressant use among adults: United States, 2015–2018. NCHS Data Brief 2020; 377:1–8
7.
Cai N, Choi KW, Fried EI: Reviewing the genetics of heterogeneity in depression: operationalizations, manifestations and etiologies. Hum Mol Genet 2020; 29:R10–R18
8.
Kessler RC, Bromet EJ: The epidemiology of depression across cultures. Annu Rev Public Health 2013; 34:119–138
9.
Caspi A, Houts RM, Ambler A, et al: Longitudinal assessment of mental health disorders and comorbidities across 4 decades among participants in the Dunedin birth cohort study. JAMA Netw Open 2020; 3:e203221
10.
Barr PB, Bigdeli TB, Meyers JL: Prevalence, comorbidity, and sociodemographic correlates of psychiatric disorders reported in the All of Us Research program. JAMA Psychiatry (Epub April 20, 2022). doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.0685
11.
Li X, Wang J: Abnormal neural activities in adults and youths with major depressive disorder during emotional processing: a meta-analysis. Brain Imaging Behav 2021; 15:1134–1154
12.
Nielson DM, Keren H, O’Callaghan G, et al: Great expectations: a critical review of and suggestions for the study of reward processing as a cause and predictor of depression. Biol Psychiatry 2021; 89:134–143
13.
Shackman AJ, Tromp DPM, Stockbridge MD, et al: Dispositional negativity: an integrative psychological and neurobiological perspective. Psychol Bull 2016; 142:1275–1314
14.
Barbour T, Holmes AJ, Farabaugh AH, et al: Elevated amygdala activity in young adults with familial risk for depression: a potential marker of low resilience. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 2020; 5:194–202
15.
Young KS, Bookheimer SY, Nusslock R, et al: Dysregulation of threat neurociruitry during fear extinction: the role of anhedonia. Neuropsychopharmacology 2021; 46:1650–1657
16.
Kaczkurkin AN, Moore TM, Ruparel K, et al: Elevated amygdala perfusion mediates developmental sex differences in trait anxiety. Biol Psychiatry 2016; 80:775–785
17.
Kaczkurkin AN, Moore TM, Calkins ME, et al: Common and dissociable regional cerebral blood flow differences associate with dimensions of psychopathology across categorical diagnoses. Mol Psychiatry 2018; 23:1981–1989
18.
Swartz JR, Knodt AR, Radtke SR, et al: A neural biomarker of psychological vulnerability to future life stress. Neuron 2015; 85:505–511
19.
Shackman AJ, Stockbridge MD, Tillman RM, et al: The neurobiology of dispositional negativity and attentional biases to threat: implications for understanding anxiety disorders in adults and youth. J Exp Psychopathol 2016; 7:311–342
20.
McTeague LM, Rosenberg BM, Lopez JW, et al: Identification of common neural circuit disruptions in emotional processing across psychiatric disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2020; 177:411–421
21.
Janiri D, Moser DA, Doucet GE, et al: Shared neural phenotypes for mood and anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of 226 task-related functional imaging studies. JAMA Psychiatry 2020; 77:172–179
22.
Hur J, Kuhn M, Grogans SE, et al: Anxiety-related frontocortical activity is associated with dampened stressor reactivity in the real world. Psychol Sci 2022; doi: 10.1177/09567976211056635
23.
Poldrack RA, Baker CI, Durnez J, et al: Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat Rev Neurosci 2017; 18:115–126
24.
Salimi-Khorshidi G, Smith SM, Keltner JR, et al: Meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a comparison of image-based and coordinate-based pooling of studies. Neuroimage 2009; 45:810–823
25.
Kvarven A, Strømland E, Johannesson M: Comparing meta-analyses and preregistered multiple-laboratory replication projects. Nat Hum Behav 2020; 4:423–434
26.
Genon S, Eickhoff SB, Kharabian S: Linking interindividual variability in brain structure to behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 2022; 23:307–318
27.
Schönbrodt FD, Perugini M: At what sample size do correlations stabilize? J Res Pers 2013; 47:609–612
28.
Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, et al: Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature 2022; 603:654–660
29.
Tamm S, Harmer CJ, Schiel J, et al: No association between amygdala responses to negative faces and depressive symptoms: cross-sectional data from 28,638 individuals in the UK Biobank cohort. Am J Psychiatry 2022; 179:509–513
30.
Cai N, Revez JA, Adams MJ, et al: Minimal phenotyping yields genome-wide association signals of low specificity for major depression. Nat Genet 2020; 52:437–447
31.
Shackman AJ, Fox AS: Getting serious about variation: lessons for clinical neuroscience (a commentary on ‘The Myth of Optimality in Clinical Neuroscience’). Trends Cogn Sci 2018; 22:368–369
32.
Warlow SM, Berridge KC: Incentive motivation: ‘wanting’ roles of central amygdala circuitry. Behav Brain Res 2021; 411:113376
33.
Fox AS, Shackman AJ: The central extended amygdala in fear and anxiety: closing the gap between mechanistic and neuroimaging research. Neurosci Lett 2019; 693:58–67
34.
Cuijpers P, Stringaris A, Wolpert M: Treatment outcomes for depression: challenges and opportunities. Lancet Psychiatry 2020; 7:925–927
35.
Woo CW, Chang LJ, Lindquist MA, et al: Building better biomarkers: brain models in translational neuroimaging. Nat Neurosci 2017; 20:365–377
36.
Gordon JA, Redish AD: On the cusp. Current challenges and promises in psychiatry; in Computational Psychiatry: New Perspectives on Mental Illness. Edited by Redish AD, Gordon JA. Cambridge, MA, MIT Press, 2016, pp 3–14
37.
Coleman JRI: The validity of brief phenotyping in population biobanks for psychiatric genome-wide association studies on the biobank scale. Complex Psychiatry 2021; 7:11–15
38.
Kennedy JT, Harms MP, Korucuoglu O, et al: Reliability and stability challenges in ABCD task fMRI data. NeuroImage 2022; 252:119046
39.
Finn ES: Is it time to put rest to rest? Trends Cogn Sci 2021; 25:1021–1032
40.
Elliott ML, Knodt AR, Hariri AR: Striving toward translation: strategies for reliable fMRI measurement. Trends Cogn Sci 2021; 25:776–787
41.
Fox AS, Oler JA, Tromp DPM, et al: Extending the amygdala in theories of threat processing. Trends Neurosci 2015; 38:319–329

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 454 - 457

History

Accepted: 6 May 2022
Published online: 1 July 2022
Published in print: July 2022

Keywords

  1. amygdala
  2. depression
  3. fMRI
  4. internalizing disorders
  5. major depressive disorder (MDD)
  6. Depressive Disorders
  7. Neuroimaging

Authors

Details

Shannon E. Grogans, M.S.
Department of Psychology (Grogans, Shackman), Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program (Shackman), and Maryland Neuroimaging Center (Shackman), University of Maryland, College Park; Department of Psychology and California National Primate Research Center, University of California, Davis (Fox).
Andrew S. Fox, Ph.D.
Department of Psychology (Grogans, Shackman), Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program (Shackman), and Maryland Neuroimaging Center (Shackman), University of Maryland, College Park; Department of Psychology and California National Primate Research Center, University of California, Davis (Fox).
Alexander J. Shackman, Ph.D. [email protected]
Department of Psychology (Grogans, Shackman), Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program (Shackman), and Maryland Neuroimaging Center (Shackman), University of Maryland, College Park; Department of Psychology and California National Primate Research Center, University of California, Davis (Fox).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Shackman ([email protected]).

Funding Information

Supported in part by the California National Primate Center; National Institutes of Health (MH121409, MH121735, MH128336, MH129851, OD011107); ASAP Foundation; University of California, Davis; and University of Maryland.Dr. Shackman reports serving on a scientific advisory board for Hoffmann-La Roche AG, with a focus on the development of novel anxiolytic compounds. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share