Epidemiologic Studies
Rates of bipolar disorder in community samples are influenced by all of the aforementioned methodological caveats with some added ones. The time frame studied (lifetime, past year, past three months), whether parent and/or child (usually teen or older child) is interviewed and whether mania and/or hypomania is addressed, yields different rates. An important question has been whether rates in youths are the same as in adults. On the one hand, because bipolar disorder is an early-onset condition, rates should be similar. On the other hand, because first onsets continue to accumulate through adulthood, lifetime rates should increase with age.
To simplify the discussion, we focus on rates of mania/BP I. A comprehensive meta-analysis of rates of bipolar disorder in youths was recently published by
Van Meter et al. (2011), who questioned whether rates of early-onset BP I disorder are disproportionately higher in the United States than in Europe. The authors conclude that rates are similar. They also conclude that rates of mania are about 1.2%, similar to rates in at least some studies of BP I in adults (
Kessler et al. 1994;
Merikangas et al. 2007,
2011).
A closer look at the articles in the meta-analysis, however, reveals that very few studies provide satisfying information, and those that do have found lower rates. The study by
Kim-Cohen et al. (2003), done on the New Zealand Dunedin cohort, was included in error; it did not measure mania in youth under age 18. In the Netherlands study (
Verhulst et al. 1997), the ∼2% rate was achieved by adding the parent rate (1.1%) and the teen rate (0.9%). As the authors noted, there was very little overlap in symptom reports, and the authors themselves were quite skeptical of their results. Stringaris and colleagues (2010) required convergence between parent and child reports to diagnose bipolar disorder because they found a very low kappa (0.02%) between parent and child. Doing so yielded a rate of mania of 0.1%. In a much smaller community study using parent information to corroborate child data,
Carlson & Kashani (1988) also found low rates (0.7%). Although the Methods for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Mental Disorders study (
Goodman et al. 1998) was cited as having a rate of mania of 1.3%, symptoms from either parent or child were used. Reliability for mania was never examined because the condition was too rare. Rates were likely much lower than 1.3%—closer to 0.4% to 0.6% when convergence reports were required (P. Fisher, personal communication). Nevertheless, rates are variable even when using a single informant. Using only teen reports, the lifetime rate for mania was found to be 0.1% in the Oregon Adolescent Depression Project (
Lewinsohn et al. 1995) and 1.7% in the National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescents (
Merikangas et al. 2012). In summary, studies designed from a liberal perspective have generally produced rates of BP I that differ from the more conservative studies. Liberal studies, where there is an absence of confirming symptoms from another informant, found rates in youths to be similar to those of adults in most instances. Studies using more narrowly defined mania and convergent informants reported lower rates.
Even when teens report symptoms that meet mania criteria, there are questions about the validity of their reports.
Cicero and colleagues (2009) have raised concerns about the significance of self-reported manic symptoms in young adults. They used publicly available data from the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions, conducted during 2001 and 2002, and a follow-up sample collected three years later (
Natl. Inst. Alcohol Abuse Alcohol. 2001–2005). The sample included 43,093 noninstitutionalized, civilian respondents age 18 and over; 80% of the original sample completed the follow-up survey. The investigators found that there was a significant drop over the three-year time frame in the respondents reporting lifetime mania.
Kessler et al. (2009) used the same algorithm as the National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescents and found an overall rate of 1.15% for BP I. There was a very significant age gradient by age group, however, with highest rates being in those younger than age 25. In addition, between 50% and 75% of the sample no longer endorsed manic symptoms three years later (although there does not appear to be an age gradient in that observation). The authors concluded that there is a “developmentally limited” form of bipolar disorder (i.e., young people report a manic episode during young adulthood but never have another episode, hence the decline in rates). That conclusion certainly contradicts data from clinical samples suggesting that early-onset bipolar patients are more likely to be chronic or to have multiple episodes (
Birmaher et al. 2009a,
Carlson et al. 2012,
Geller et al. 2008,
Perlis et al. 2004). The alternative conclusion is that young people from epidemiologic studies who endorse manic symptoms (along with other comorbidities) may have mental health problems, but they do not correspond to the problems of manic patients seen in clinical practice. If that is true, then studies reporting high rates of bipolar disorder in youths need to be understood in that context.
Liberal and Conservative Views of Top-Down High-Risk Studies
Rates of bipolar disorder are significantly higher in the offspring of bipolar parents than in the offspring of normal controls. In contrast, studies differ on whether ADHD is more common in the offspring of bipolar parents than controls. For both disorders, rates vary widely, again depending on the age of the sample; whether mania or the bipolar spectrum is measured; whether the concept of mania/bipolar disorder is construed broadly or narrowly, and for this issue, the origin of the proband sample; and the nature of the comparison group (e.g., representative or “supernormal”;
Duffy et al. 2011).
A meta-analysis of studies of bipolar offspring completed before 1995 (LaPalme et al. 1997) found rates of BP I disorder to be 5.4% compared to 0% in the offspring of parents without bipolar disorder. Since then, a number of studies have been done, all but one of which used a prospective follow-up design, and the findings are much more variable (
Carlson 2013). Conservatively diagnosed assessment approaches reported rates of 2% to 6% for BP I disorder (
Duffy et al. 2007,
Mesman et al. 2013,
Meyer et al. 2004,
Nurnberger et al. 2011,
Shaw et al. 2005). These rates contrast with another group of high-risk studies using either the WASH-U-KSADS or the KSADS-E in which rates of bipolar I disorder were 10% to 33% in offspring (
Chang et al. 2000,
Henin et al. 2005,
Hirshfeld-Becker et al. 2006,
Singh et al. 2007).
Duffy and co-investigators (2011, 2012) reviewed the high-risk literature to explain the range of rates of bipolar disorder and ADHD in various studies. They found that the source of the parent probands made a significant difference. Where parents were identified through neurobiologic research, with parent and child diagnosis obtained through best-estimate procedures, rates of parent comorbidity were lower, rates of psychopathology in the nonproband parent were lower, and rates of BP I were lower in the offspring, who themselves had an adolescent or older age of onset and low rates of ADHD. Self-referred bipolar probands recruited from advertisements or recruited from tertiary care clinics, who were diagnosed with structured interviews and whose children were assessed only with structured interviews, were found to have more offspring with psychopathology, including higher rates of ADHD and a younger age of onset of mental health problems.
The meta-analysis by Faraone and colleagues (2012) of 27 family genetic studies examining the comorbidity between ADHD and BP I disorder illustrates how findings are shaped by interpretation, study design, and selection of studies. The meta-analysis concluded that the relative risk of ADHD in bipolar probands is 2.2 (95% CI 2.1–3.2), with rates of ADHD in offspring of bipolar versus comparison probands of 27% versus 9.6%, in siblings 30.1% versus 11.0%, and in parents 16.5% versus 4.5%. Conversely, the relative risk of bipolar disorder in ADHD probands is 1.8 (95% CI 1.3–2.6), with rates of bipolar disorder in offspring of ADHD probands versus comparison probands being 6.8% versus 3.5%, in siblings 5.9% versus 2.8%, and in parents 5.1% versus 3.1%.
Pertinent to this discussion, the highest relative risks of ADHD in offspring and other relatives of probands with bipolar disorder were from studies using the WASH-U-KSADS and the K-SADS-E. Closer inspection revealed that in two of the studies (i.e.,
Carlson & Weintraub 1993,
Duffy et al. 2007), offspring had learning problems or ADHD symptoms but not an ADHD diagnosis. Additionally, the study by
Zahn-Waxler et al. (1988) stated that two of seven offspring of bipolar parents had ADHD. Not stated was that this conclusion was drawn from a parent rating scale. Follow-up interviews of the parent and child yielded no children with ADHD. Neither of the Dutch bipolar high-risk studies (e.g.,
Hillegers et al. 2005,
Wals et al. 2001) nor the Amish study (
Shaw et al. 2005), all of which reported low rates of ADHD, were included in the meta-analysis. All told, it would appear that rates of ADHD in bipolar offspring were about the same as population rates (around 10% when all studies are considered). What made the odds ratio high were the low rates in comparison groups that were usually selected to have no psychopathology (“supernormals”).
Finally, in
Faraone’s (2012) meta-analysis, just as studies of ADHD among relatives of bipolar disorder found the highest relative risks using the WASH-U-KSADS or K-SADS-E, so did studies of bipolar disorder among relatives of ADHD probands. The one exception came from a study where 2 of 112 (1.78%) fathers with ADHD were said to have mania, basically the same as the population rate (
Bhatia et al. 1991). No mention was made of rates of ADHD in the comparison group, which precludes an accurate risk ratio.
In summary, rates of BP I in offspring of BP I probands are almost always higher than in a nonbipolar comparison group. How high they are depends on whether mania is defined conservatively or liberally and on how complicated and strictly diagnosed the bipolar disorder in the parent probands are. These conclusions apply equally to the relationship of ADHD and bipolar disorder, whether the focus is on rates of BP I disorder in samples with ADHD or on rates of ADHD in samples of BP I disorder.
Longitudinal Course of Bipolar Disorder
The most pressing question about childhood-onset bipolar disorder (and especially mania/BP I disorder) is whether it is continuous with bipolar disorder in adulthood. Manic symptoms, whether or not they occur in episodes, are clearly impairing in children. The question is one of homotypic versus heterotypic continuity: Is mania the same disorder starting younger, does it evolve into something else, was it something else all along, or, in some cases, does it remit completely? These issues are further complicated by the fact that BP I disorder in adults is heterogeneous in its phenomenology and outcome, and although there are many short-term outcome studies, very few are longer than four years. In child- and adolescent-onset studies, a minimum of a four-year follow-up is necessary for the samples to enter adolescence and in some cases young adulthood.
A number of follow-up studies of adults with BP I disorder have examined overall outcome. Although when the study took place, the sample composition and definitions of outcome differ and some studies have sample overlap, it is possible to combine information into (
a) recovered, remitted, good outcome; (
b) incomplete remission or fair outcome; and (
c) chronic or poor outcome. With this metric, a little less than half of the samples do fairly well (they experience no recurrences, or if there is a recurrence, good functioning resumes when the episodes remit), approximately one-quarter of the samples do poorly (they are chronically ill in one or another mood state, they attempt or commit suicide, or they become chronically psychotic), and outcomes for the remaining one-quarter of the samples are in between (
Angst & Sellaro 2000;
Angst et al. 2003;
Carlson et al. 1974,
2012;
Coryell et al. 1998;
Goldberg & Harrow 2011;
Morrison et al. 1973;
Stephens & McHugh 1991;
Tsuang & Dempsey 1979).
Only three of the studies cited above have followed participants for four or more years and provide the kind of sample and follow-up detail that allows some comparison with child and adolescent follow-up studies: Angst’s Zurich sample, the Collaborative Depression Study, and the Suffolk County Mental Heath Project (
Angst et al. 2003,
Solomon et al. 2010, and
Carlson et al. 2012, respectively). In these studies, where follow-up duration ranges from 4 to more than 30 years, 40% to 50% of adult participants were male, 89% to 100% had been initially hospitalized, and 55% to 100% were psychotic at their index episode. Their age at the time of study entry was late 20s to early 30s; age of onset was approximately 25 years. As noted above, overall outcome is decent for almost half the sample, and the rate of chronicity and poor outcome is approximately 20% to 25%. Comorbidity was not a focus of these studies, although in two studies that used data from other papers on the same samples, ADHD/externalizing disorders occurred in a little over 20% of the adult samples (
Carlson et al. 2002,
Winokur et al. 1993). Median manic episode duration was 7 to 16 weeks, and median time ill over follow-up was about 20%.
The impact of childhood psychopathology—either prior externalizing disorders (ADHD and/or conduct disorders) or other childhood psychopathology (anxiety disorders, depressive symptoms, adjustment disorders)—on outcome in bipolar disorder has been examined in the Suffolk County Mental Health Project (
Carlson et al. 2002,
2012). The study reported that childhood psychopathology prior to the onset of BP I contributed substantially to the global assessment of functioning at both the 24-month and 48-month follow-ups and was more predictive of functioning and length of time ill than was age of onset. In terms of influence on episode duration and recurrence, among the significant predictors of time to remission were young age of onset and childhood psychopathology. Almost two-thirds of remitters relapsed by four-year follow-up, mostly to a manic episode. The predictors of time to relapse were depressive symptoms during childhood (
Bromet et al. 2005).
Adolescent-onset bipolar studies have smaller samples and shorter-term follow-up (four to five years) (
Jairam et al. 2004,
Srinath et al. 1998,
Strober et al. 1995). As with adult studies, half the patients in these studies were male and were hospitalized for their index episode, which was usually manic or mixed. They were ages 14 to 16 and had an adolescent onset. Rates of psychosis varied from 28% to 63%. Rates of externalizing comorbidity were low (4% to 15%). The index manic episode duration was 8 to 16 weeks. Recovery rates were almost 100% if follow-up was long enough, but relapse over follow-up ranged from 44% to 67%—higher if subthreshold symptoms are counted (
Strober et al. 1995). In the two samples from India, about half the teens had a good recovery, about 20% were chronically ill, and the remainder of the teens were somewhat impaired. These adolescent samples, then, appeared to be roughly similar to their adult counterparts in terms of their relatively classic description and outcome.
There is one follow-up study of children (
Wozniak et al. 2011) and two follow-up studies of children and adolescents (
Birmaher et al. 2009a,
Geller et al. 2008) that are four years or longer. In studies by Wozniak and co-investigators (2011) and Geller and colleagues (2008), the samples were diagnosed with current mania/BP I at the time of study entry. In the Course of Bipolar Youth study (
Axelson et al. 2006,
Birmaher et al. 2009a), it is unclear how many subjects were symptomatic with mania at study entry because worst episode rather than current status was reported. Age comparisons included the entire bipolar spectrum (BP I, BP II, and BP-NOS), but in the four-year follow-up data, BP I subjects were described separately from the BP II and BP-NOS subjects.
Compared to the adolescent-onset and adult studies reported above, children in the Massachusetts General Hospital (
Wozniak et al. 2011) and Washington University (
Geller et al. 2008) bipolar outcome studies were all outpatients, predominantly male, and had very high rates of ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder (>80%). These studies did not delineate episodes and counted the same symptoms toward ADHD and mania. Most of the children did not recover or had very long first episodes of mania (mean 142.7 ± 139.4 weeks). Most of the Massachusetts General Hospital sample (93.6%), who were still children and young adolescents at the four-year follow-up, either continued to meet full diagnostic criteria for BP I disorder (73.1%), or continued to have persistent subthreshold BP I disorder (6.4%). In the Washington University study, where more than half the sample was over age 18 at follow-up, respondents spent 60% of the follow-up period with mood disorder symptoms. As is usual with data on percent time ill, it isn’t clear how this was distributed across the sample (e.g., whether most youths were ill for 60% of the time or if some were continuously well and others ill constantly). By the age of 18, 44% had either been continuously ill or had recurrence of the same symptoms. Although tables in the manuscript (
Geller et al. 2008, p. 1130) describe the sample in their first, second, and third episodes, no data described whether the children remitted completely from their mania, remained somewhat ill, or were depressed between episodes. No functional follow-up data were described (
Geller et al. 2008).
In liberally diagnosed samples, follow-up studies of children with ADHD reveal comparatively high rates of development of bipolar disorder. For instance, the Washington University mania sample had an ADHD comparison group: In a six-year follow-up of these children, 22.2% developed BP I or BP II (morbidity risk of 28.5%). Interestingly, the use of stimulants was found to be a protective factor (
Tillman & Geller 2006). In the
Biederman et al. (2006) 10-year follow-up study of children with ADHD, 29% developed bipolar disorder compared to 3% of controls. By contrast, in other long-term follow-up studies of ADHD (
Barkley et al. 2002,
Carlson et al. 2000,
Klein et al. 2012,
Weiss et al. 1985), conservatively diagnosed bipolar disorder occurred at population rates. One interpretation of the findings in the Washington University and Massachusetts General samples is that untreated ADHD is, in fact, worsening, and some children are developing higher rates of aggressive, irritable, dysregulated behavior rather than mania.
In the Course and Outcome of Bipolar Youth study (
Birmaher et al. 2009c), which used a more conservative definition of mania, the childhood-onset BP-spectrum group had more males and higher rates of ADHD than did the teen-onset group, although rates of oppositional defiant disorder were much lower in the childhood-onset group, suggesting that chronic irritability was less of a problem. The duration of mania was at least one year. The youths spent 56% of the time over the course of the follow-up with either full-syndromal or subsyndromal mood symptoms. Those with the earliest onsets had more episodes, polarity changes, and comorbidity than the teen-onset samples. Even when episodes are more central to the diagnosis of mania, childhood onset, comorbidity, and chronicity appear to be inextricably intertwined.
The best way to explain the high comorbidity of ADHD and mania in some studies, then, is to suggest that it results from counting ADHD symptoms toward both mania and ADHD. Thus, the liberal studies show poor outcome for very-early-onset bipolar disorder because many of their bipolar subjects really have severe ADHD, which is chronic. Although the co-occurrence of mania and ADHD symptoms may represent a complex bipolar subtype, the co-occurrence may also simply represent severe ADHD with mood lability. Regardless, it is both stable and chronically impairing. Interestingly, ADHD researchers are finally paying attention to the separate impact of emotion dysregulation on outcome, and the impact is not favorable (
Barkley & Fischer 2010,
Martel 2009,
Sobanski et al. 2010).
Although the conservative studies report lower rates of comorbidity and outcomes that are not uniformly poor, even some individuals conservatively diagnosed with bipolar disorder have poor outcomes. Individuals with poor outcomes are likely to have comorbid disorders (or, in adults, a childhood history of nonbipolar psychopathology).