Research into many brain diseases, including stroke,
1 Alzheimer's disease,
2 Parkinson's disease,
3 epilepsy,
4 Huntington's disease,
5 and AIDS,
6 has shown significant associations between these conditions and the presence of depressive disturbances. These associations are important for a variety of reasons. First, the occurrence of depression in neurologic disease is a natural experiment whose study continues to shed light on our understanding of the role of the brain in depressive illnesses. Second, the co-occurrence of depression and neurologic disease magnifies morbidity. Third, treatment of depression in neurologic disease may greatly improve prognosis.
Perhaps the most interesting issue regarding the association between depression and neurologic disease is whether depression is caused by neurologic disease. Two general types of causal links should be distinguished. In the first, depression arises as a psychological reaction to the impairments or social disruption produced by the neurologic disease, in the same way that depression might arise in any individual faced with the adversities produced by a serious disease. In the second, depression is a specific symptom of the neurologic disease and is intimately tied to the pathophysiology of the disease. The relationship between stroke and depression has received sufficient investigation to make it a suitable model in discussing such potential causal relationships.
In this article we review research on depression and stroke to illustrate how to approach causal links between neurologic disease and mental syndromes. Our major conclusion is that stroke lesions, under certain circumstances, cause depression through a direct but unknown pathophysiologic process.
BACKGROUND
Dr. Allan House recently has argued
7 that there are few data to support the hypothesis that depression is a “specific complication of stroke.” Although he did not overtly acknowledge his assumptions, he began with the premise that depression is a natural psychological reaction to the adversity of stroke.
Dr. House contested the existence of “a specific syndrome of poststroke depression,” which has been proposed by Robinson and others.
1 His argument against the poststroke depression hypothesis is based on the refutation of certain predictions that he believes flow naturally from the hypothesis.
Specifically, Dr. House has postulated
7 that if there were such a specific complication of stroke as “poststroke depression,” then three things would be expected. First, the clinical picture of depression after stroke (such as its constituent symptoms or frequency) would be different from depression in the absence of stroke or after other general medical conditions. Second, depression would have a different etiology after stroke. Third, depression associated with stroke would have “different characteristics in response to treatment.” After reviewing the empirical evidence in these three areas, Dr. House concluded that none of these predictions has been consistently supported by the existing literature. He implied that we cannot, as yet, prove that depression is a specific complication of stroke.
We disagree with Dr. House's postulates and feel that he has finessed the central issue in this debate. The critical question regarding the association of depression and stroke is not whether poststroke depression fulfills Dr. House's postulates, but rather, “What is the cause of depression after stroke?” Dr. House's criteria to assess causality are inadequate to make a judgment about this issue.
For some patients, depression, anxiety, and other symptoms commonly follow stroke and are indicative of the individual coming to terms with an adverse event. These natural human emotions do not imply a particular pathophysiology of the stroke or the depression. In the sense that they resemble symptoms seen after any adverse event, they are not specific to stroke. Their cause is not the stroke per se, but rather the characteristics of the stroke that make it an adverse event to that particular individual, at that particular time, in his or her particular life.
In other patients, a different kind of depression emerges after stroke. This is major depression, a well-defined disturbance, which most psychiatrists operationalize as a syndromic category
8 based on disjunctive criteria (5 of 9 possible symptoms). These criteria characterize severe depression due to genetic causes (probably multiple types), environmental stressors, personal vulnerability, or symptomatic response to physical factors ranging from reserpine medication to structural lesions. It is the syndrome of major depression after stroke that we propose might at times be caused directly by the pathophysiology of stroke. Dr. House suggested that clinical presentation, pathophysiological mechanisms, and response to treatment would all be unique if major depression was a specific consequence of stroke. However, the syndrome of major depression can probably be caused by a range of pathologies that affect the brain—as is suggested by work in other diseases
2–6—as well as by genetic predispositions. One would not
a priori predict that poststroke major depression would manifest itself in a unique clinical presentation, any more than one would presume that every form of pneumonia could be distinguished on clinical examination alone. In fact, it was shown by Lipsey et al.
9 that poststroke major depression has a very similar clinical presentation to major depression in elderly patients without structural brain lesions.
Dr. House's criterion of unique response to treatment is similarly inappropriate. For instance, cases of congestive heart failure arising from different causes may respond as well to the same diuretic, just as antihypertensive agents are often effective across a wide etiologic spectrum. Antidepressant medication has been shown in controlled treatment trials to be effective in treating disorders ranging from pure familial depressive disorder to social phobia to obsessive-compulsive disorder. The fact that poststroke depression responds to antidepressant therapy provides no evidence for or against a causal relationship between stroke and depression.
Dr. House's criterion for specificity is somewhat closer to the mark. We agree that if a specific form of poststroke depression exists, it by definition results from some mechanism directly related to the brain injury. However, it is clear that depression following stroke might at times have other (or even multiple) causes.
It is precisely the similarity of major depression after stroke to major depression in other settings that raises doubts about whether major depression after stroke is
always simply a human reaction to the adversity brought on by illness. A human reaction to adversity is typically universal and nonspecific. Major depression after stroke has not been shown to respond to nonspecific psychological or psychosocial interventions,
7 but it has been shown to respond to specific pharmacologic treatment for depression.
10,11 If a patient developed chest pain after stroke, along with joint pain, anxiety, and indigestion, we might think of the chest pain as a “somatization” of his or her distress and attempt to treat it with a psychological intervention. If, however, a patient developed chest pain with radiation to the left hand, accompanied by nausea, we might recognize the syndrome of angina and be concerned that the patient has heart disease. We make a similar argument about depressed mood (or anhedonia) as part of a major depressive syndrome, as opposed to depression as part of nonspecific distress and demoralization.
DIRECTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
We do not argue that depression after stroke is always a “specific complication” of stroke. Rather, there are sufficient data to support the hypothesis that, in some circumstances, stroke causes depression through a pathophysiological process that may some day be identified and specifically treated. Although House argues that poststroke depression is no more than an understandable response to life-threatening physical illness, the cumulative data speak for themselves.
What is needed now is not an argument about whether stroke can cause depression, but further study of how to identify the patients who are most at risk, what mechanism may mediate this depressive disorder, and whether specific targeted treatments can be developed to ameliorate the depression as well as the associated increased mortality and delayed physical recovery that are associated with poststroke depression.
31–33Future research addressing causal links of stroke to other neuropsychiatric disorders (for example, anxiety disorders) should also use operational criteria, such as the ones proposed here, to assess causality between stroke and these other disorders. It would also be important to assess the mechanisms of poststroke major depression in patients without left anterior lesions and whether these overlap with the depression provoked by strategic lesion location.
Research should also examine whether stroke could be used as a model to identify the risk factors and anatomical substrates of depression in other physical disorders, such as traumatic brain injury, multiple sclerosis, epilepsy, Huntington's disease, AIDS, and Parkinson's disease, to mention only a few. Psychiatrists and neuroscientists have been working for many years to identify the structural and physiological basis of primary depressive disorder. Functional imaging studies
30,34 have frequently identified left prefrontal cortical abnormalities of metabolic activity in patients with primary depression. Structural imaging studies of depression in elderly patients have identified hyperintensities on MRI scan in subcortical white matter and basal ganglia.
35 These findings suggest overlap in the anatomic substrates of primary and poststroke depression. The insights that may be gained from studying patients with symptomatic depressions may shed light on the pathophysiological mechanisms and anatomical substrates of depressive disorder in patients without known neuropathology.