Skip to main content

Abstract

The authors examined the severity and type of deficits in remote memory in patients with probable Alzheimer's disease (AD). In the first study, 40 AD patients showed significantly more severe deficits on both the free-recall and the recognition sections of the Remote Memory Scale (which measures memory for famous people and well-known events) compared with normal control subjects. In the second study, 25 AD patients showed significantly more deficits on the free-recall section of the Autobiographical Memory Scale compared with normal control subjects. Remote memory deficits in AD may be related to both retrieval deficits and damage to memory traces.
Although deficits in anterograde memory are the hallmark of Alzheimer's disease (AD), the prevalence and clinical correlates of remote memory deficits in this disease have been less extensively studied. Wilson and Cockburn1 assessed AD patients with a facial identification task and a test of recall and recognition of public events and famous people of past decades. They and others25 found that AD patients performed significantly worse than normal control subjects across all decades in both tests. Sagar et al.2 examined remote memory in patients with mild to moderate AD, using recall and recognition tests of public events. They found a gradient of deficits in the recall of past public events (remote events being less affected than recent events), and the severity of the memory deficit was related to the severity of dementia. In a recent study, Greene and Hodges6 assessed AD patients with a neuropsychological battery designed to assess face recognition, identification, and naming; they found that even those with mild dementia had significant deficits on all tests of remote memory. A recent study by Storandt et al.7 assessed dementia and control patients with a recognition task for remote events. Patients with dementia performed worse than control subjects, but there was no temporal gradient.
Autobiographical memory (ABM) in AD has been typically assessed by asking patients to retrieve and date autobiographical memories in response to a list of cue words.34 One limitation of this technique is that patients might report memories only for restricted time periods and not from the whole life span. Moreover, memories produced by patients are not verified for accuracy with other sources familiar with the patient's life, and whether the memories produced are real or fabrications cannot easily be determined in those studies. The assessment of ABM was improved by Kopelman,4 who examined ABM in a group of 16 patients with AD by using two instruments: the Personal Semantic Memory Schedule, which probes patients' knowledge of facts about their past, and the Autobiographic Incidents Schedule, which probes patients' capacity for producing a memory of a specific incident. Kopelman4 found that AD patients had significant deficits across all time periods as compared with age-comparable normal control subjects, although deficits for late time periods were greater than for earlier time periods. Dall'Ora et al.8 examined 19 AD patients with mild dementia with an ABM task that included five questions for each of the following three periods: childhood and adolescence, early adulthood, and late adulthood. AD patients had an overall lower score than a group of age- and education-matched normal control subjects, but there was no significant time gradient; that is, recall for early and late time periods was similar. Dall'Ora et al.8 found no significant correlations between scores on the ABM and performance on tests assessing verbal span, episodic and semantic memory, or abstract reasoning and suggested that ABM deficits may result from defective organization of search. This hypothesis was further investigated by Greene and colleagues9,10 and Kopelman;11 both found a significant association between tests of executive functions and ABM, as well as a modest correlation between tests of retrograde and anterograde memory. However, correlations with other neuropsychological tasks were not calculated, and whether other cognitive domains are also relevant to deficits in ABM was not determined. The finding of Greene et al.9 that ABM deficits were also present after cued questions led the author to suggest that ABM deficits may result from both degradation of individual memory traces and retrieval deficits. A better approach to the issue of retrieval-versus-degradation of memory traces is to examine performance on free recall and recognition versions of the same questions, but to our knowledge this pairing has not been assessed. Studies of remote memory in AD are summarized in Table 1.
The present study examined remote memory in a series of AD patients with very mild, mild, or moderate dementia, using a newly designed set of tests that included the Remote Memory Scale (RMS) for famous people and well-known events and the Autobiographical Memory Scale (AMS). We also assessed our patients with a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, which allowed us to examine correlations between performance on specific cognitive domains and deficits of remote memory.

METHODS

Patients and Procedure

A consecutive series of 44 patients meeting the NINCDS-ADRDA12 criteria for probable AD participated in the study. Twenty-one patients completed both the RMS and the AMS, 19 patients completed only the RMS, and 4 patients completed only the AMS. Patients with a history of stroke, one or more focal lesions on MRI (T1-weighted), or a Hachinski Ischemic score greater than 4 were excluded from the study.
After informed consent was obtained, patients and control subjects were assessed with the Remote Memory and Autobiographical Memory Scales. Because of methodological limitations, fewer patients could be assessed with the AMS. AD patients were also given a comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation as described below.

Remote Memory Scale

The pilot RMS consisted of 80 famous names and 80 well-known events belonging to the 1950s, '60s, '70s, and '80s (20 names and 20 events from each decade) and consisted of free-recall and recognition sections. Thus, the free-recall section consisted of 160 questions presented in a randomized order. The recognition section included all of the 160 randomized questions and four options for each question (the correct answer, two related distractors, and one irrelevant answer).
The pilot RMS was assessed in 20 normal control subjects (mean age±SD= 49.5±18.3 years, range 24–83; 55% females; 50% high [≥10 years] and 50% low [<10 years] educational level). The questions that showed a significant difference for gender (4) or educational level (2), as demonstrated by t-tests using gender or educational level as grouping variables, were deleted, and the final RMS version included only those questions with a success rate (percentage of correct answers) between 50% and 95%. (This wide cutoff was selected to avoid ceiling effects for the control group and floor effects for the AD group.) From those questions meeting this criterion, we randomly selected 40 questions to obtain two sections (free recall and recognition), each employing the same 40 questions (10 for each decade: 5 famous names and 5 well-known events).
Figure 1 shows the performance of young normal control subjects (n=10; mean age=25.4±1.5 years, range 24–35) and older normal control subjects (n=10; mean age=58.2±1.3 years, range 36–83) on the free recall section of the pilot RMS. A three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures (factor 1: group [young vs. old]; repeated measures: decades [1950s to 1980s] and item [famous names vs. well-known events]) demonstrated a significant group effect (F=13.2, df=1,18, P=0.001): older control subjects remembered significantly more items than young control subjects; a significant effect for decades (F=6.63, df=3,54, P<0.001): there was a significantly lower recall for the 1950s compared with the other three decades; and the expected significant group×decade interaction (F=10.0, df=3,54, P<0.0001): young control subjects had a significantly lower recall for the 1950s, '60s, and '70s, but not for the '80s, than older control subjects. There were no significant effects for test (i.e., free recall vs. recognition), test×decade interaction, or group×test×decade interaction. The lack of a significant test effect on the free recall section may be explained by a ceiling effect for the old control subjects on the free recall section and a floor effect for the young control subjects to early-decades stimuli. Test-retest reliability was assessed by giving the RMS to 10 AD patients (MMSE mean score=24.2±5.4) twice, 1 month apart, and proved to be high (r=0.98). No reliability measures were calculated for the control group.

Autobiographical Memory Scale (AMS)

The pilot AMS consisted of 100 questions about specific dates, places, events, individuals, etc., from personal history from five periods: childhood (6 to 12 years), adolescence (12 to 18 years), early adulthood (18 to 35 years), adulthood (35 to 60 years), and recent events. There were 20 questions for each period, and each question included a confidence scale that was rated as “completely sure,” “almost sure,” “not very sure,” or “unsure.”
The pilot AMS was assessed in first-degree relatives of 10 normal control subjects (not assessed with the RMS; usually a sibling; one informant per control individual; age range 52 to 74 years; 60% females). For the final AMS version, we selected only those questions for which the agreement between the normal control subjects and their respective relatives was above 90% and the confidence scale score was either “almost sure” or “completely sure.” Based on these criteria, the final version of the AMS consisted of 39 questions: 6 assessing childhood, 6 adolescence, 9 youth, 9 adulthood, and 9 recent events. There was a free recall section, where patients answered each question, and a recognition section, which used the above 39 questions with three different answers: the correct one (provided by the family member) and two semantic distractors. The percentage of questions correctly answered for each period serves as the metric. The test-retest reliability of the scale, examined by giving the scale twice with a 1-month interval to 10 AD patients (MMSE mean±SD=22.5±4.6), was high (r=0.96). This was not the same group of AD patients used to test reliability for the RMS. We further examined the reliability of the scale by calculating its internal consistency, which was also high (Cronbach's α=0.81).

Neuropsychological Battery

AD patients were also assessed with the following neuropsychological battery by examiners blind to results on the RMS and AMS scales:
Buschke Selective Reminding Test:13 This test measures verbal learning and memory on a 10-trial list-learning task.
Digit Span:14 This test examines auditory attention.
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test:15 This test measures the ability to acquire new concepts and shift sets.
Controlled Oral Word Association Test:16 This test examines access to semantic information with time constraint.
Boston Naming Test:17 This test measures the ability to name line drawings of high and low frequency objects.
Token Test:18 This test measures comprehension and execution of commands of increasing complexity.
Raven's Progressive Matrices:19 This test measures reasoning in the visual modality.
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE):20 The MMSE is an 11-item examination found to be valid and reliable in assessing a limited range of cognitive functions in a global way.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was carried out by using means and SDs, analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures, and post hoc t-tests. Regressions were calculated by using a forward stepwise regression analysis. Frequency distributions were calculated with chi-square tests and a Yates' correction for expected cell sizes less than 5. All P-values are two-tailed.

RESULTS

Study 1: Memory for Famous People and Well-Known Events

Demographic Findings:

We assessed with the RMS a group of 40 AD patients and a group 40 normal control subjects (not the same control subjects assessed for the pilot RMS study). Control subjects were spouses of AD patients or healthy elderly volunteers from the community.
No significant differences were found in age, gender, and years of education between AD patients and control subjects (mean years of age: AD, 72.0±7.3, control, 69.6±11.5; gender (% females): AD, 48%, control, 65%; mean years of education: AD, 10.7±4.5, control, 11.8±5.4).

Remote Memory Scale:

Figure 2 shows findings on the free recall and recognition sections of the RMS for AD and control subjects. A three-way ANOVA with repeated measures (factor 1: group [AD vs. control subjects]; repeated measures: decades [1950s, '60s, '70s, and '80s], and test [free recall vs. recognition]) showed a significant group effect (F=38.1, df=1,78, P<0.0001): AD patients had a significantly lower score on the RMS than normal control subjects; a significant test effect (F=281.9, df=1,78, P<0.0001): the free recall section was significantly more difficult than the recognition section for all subjects; and a significant effect for decades (F=5.28, df=3,23, P<0.05): there was a significantly lower recall for the 1950s and '60s than for the '80s (P<0.01 and P<0.0001, respectively) for the overall group of AD patients and control subjects. On the other hand, there were no significant group×decade, group×test, or group×decade×test interactions.
To examine whether the profile and severity of remote memory deficits were related to the severity of dementia, we separated AD patients into groups with very mild (MMSE scores 24–30; n=20), mild (MMSE scores 17–23; n=10), or moderate dementia (MMSE scores 10–16; n=10). A three-way ANOVA with repeated measures (group×decade×test) showed a significant group effect (F=24.4, df=3,76, P<0.0001): normal control subjects had a significantly better performance than the three AD groups, which were also significantly different from each other (Figure 3). There also was the expected significant effect for test (F=232.6, df=1,76, P<0.0001): recognition better than free recall, and a significant effect for decades(F=3.84, df=3,228, P<0.01): there was a significantly worse recall for the 1950s and '60s than for the '80s for the overall group of AD patients and control subjects.

Neuropsychological Correlates of Remote Memory in AD:

A forward stepwise regression analysis was calculated, using RMS scores as the dependent variable and age and neuropsychological scores as the independent variables. Results of the overall regression analysis were statistically significant (R2=0.60, F=37.0, df=3,36, P<0.0001), and both the Boston Naming test (R2=0.48, P<0.0001) and the Buschke long-term recall (R2=0.08, P<0.01) accounted for significant parts of the variance.

Study 2: Autobiographical Memory

Demographic Findings:

This study included a group of 25 AD patients and a group of 20 normal individuals (10 control subjects assessed for the pilot AMS and a new group of 10 control individuals). There were no significant differences between AD patients and normal control subjects in gender (% females: 61% and 50%, respectively), and years of education (mean=12.4±4.2 and 11.0±3.6, respectively). The control group was older than the AD group (mean=64.8±9.1 years and 72.4±7.2, respectively; t=–3.6, P<0.05). The mean MMSE score for the entire AD group was 22±5.2 (range 11–30).

Autobiographical Memory Scale:

A two-way ANOVA with repeated measures for the AMS free recall (group [AD vs. control subjects]×time [childhood, adolescence, youth, adulthood, and recent events]) showed a significant group effect (F=16.0, df=1,53, P<0.001): AD patients had a significantly worse overall performance than normal control subjects; a significant time effect (F=4.47, df=4,212, P<0.001): early periods were better recalled than later ones; and a significant group×time interaction (F=3.77, df=4,212, P<0.005): AD patients showed significantly lower scores than normal control subjects in the recall of adolescence (P<0.001), youth (P<0.001), adulthood (P<0.001), and recent events (P<0.01), but not childhood events. AMS free recall scores for the AD group and the control group are shown in Figure 4.
A two-way ANOVA with repeated measures for the two sections of the AMS for the AD group showed a significant test effect (F=11.7, df=1,24, P<0.01), with patients scoring significantly higher on the multiple choice section than the recall section, and a significant time effect (F=8.85, df=4,96, P<0.0001), with lower recall for adolescence (P<0.05), youth (P<0.0001), adulthood (P<0.0001), and recent events (P<0.0001) compared with childhood. Free recall and recognition scores for the AD group are shown in Figure 5.

Neuropsychological Correlates of Autobiographical Memory in AD:

To examine the neuropsychological mechanisms that may be involved in the recall of autobiographical facts, a forward stepwise regression analysis was conducted with AMS scores as the dependent criterion variable and the neuropsychological scores as the independent predictor variables. A significant regression model was found (R2=0.77, F=12.3, df=6,23, P<0.0001), and both the Boston Naming Test (R2=0.57, P<0.0001) and the Token Test (R2=0.06, P<0.05) entered the model.

Correlation Between AMS and RMS Scores:

Twenty AD patients were examined with both remote memory scales. Whereas there was a significant correlation between RMS and AMS scores (r=0.42, P<0.05), a forward stepwise regression analysis with AMS scores as the dependent variable and the RMS and neuropsychological scores as the independent variables showed a significant correlation only with the Boston Naming Test (R2=0.52, P<0.0001); the RMS did not explain a significant part of the variance.

DISCUSSION

This study examined the presence of deficits in remote memory in patients with AD by using newly designed scales for autobiographical memory and memory for famous people and well-known events. There were several important findings. First, even patients with very mild AD had significantly more severe deficits in memory for public events (RMS) than age-comparable normal control subjects. Deficits were similar across decades, and no significant gradient was found. Second, AD patients had significantly more severe deficits in autobiographic memory than age-comparable normal control subjects. A significant gradient was found, with AD patients having significantly better recall of early memories than more recent ones. Third, AD patients showed a significantly better performance on the recognition section of the RMS compared with the free recall, suggesting that retrieval deficits and damage to memory traces may both play an important role in the impairment of remote memory in AD. Fourth, deficits in both types of remote memory were significantly correlated with deficits in verbal naming.
Our study has some methodological improvements compared with previous studies of remote memory in AD. First, we developed a test for remote memory that fulfilled critical criteria21 such as including four time periods, showing a temporal gradient (i.e., young normal subjects remembered significantly fewer names or events from decades when they were too young to have experienced them as compared to older normal subjects), and including both a free recall and a recognition section. Moreover, we assessed a relatively large sample of AD patients and age-comparable normal control subjects, and all of the AD patients were also examined with a comprehensive neuropsychological battery. We found that AD patients had significantly more severe remote memory deficits than age-comparable normal control subjects, and these deficits were observed on both the free recall and the recognition sections of the RMS. Moreover, these deficits were already present in patients with very mild dementia. Similar to Sagar et al.2 and Greene and Hodges,10 we found a significant association between more severe impairments on the RMS and more severe dementia.
Sagar et al.2 suggested that the early stages of AD are characterized by deficits in episodic memory, which may produce a temporally extensive remote memory deficit with gradient. They further suggested that as dementia progresses and semantic amnesia becomes more prominent, deficits of remote memory may become more severe, with an equal involvement of all decades. To test this hypothesis we divided our AD sample into groups with very mild, mild, and moderate dementia. Although we found a significant group effect (i.e., patients with moderate dementia had a significantly worse performance than patients with very mild dementia), there was no significant group-by-decade interaction, demonstrating that deficits in remote memory were similar for all four decades. Thus, we could not find a significant gradient for remote memory deficits in the early stages of AD in the free recall or the recognition sections of the RMS. We also found a significant (albeit low) correlation between performance on the RMS and a test of anterograde verbal memory. Whether the disruption of a common mechanism may underlie the deficits in both remote and anterograde memory in patients with AD should be examined in future studies (e.g., through a longitudinal examination of the same group of patients at different stages of the illness). There was also a significant correlation with a naming task, suggesting that remote memory deficits in AD may be partially explained by impairments of lexical retrieval.
Kopelman4 examined autobiographical memory in AD and found a significant gradient (relatively better preservation of early versus recent memories) in both free recall and recognition tasks. Greene et al.9 also found a temporal gradient for the incident component of the Autobiographic Memory Inventory. Our AD patients showed significantly more severe impairments than age-comparable normal control subjects on questions about adolescence, early adulthood, adulthood, and recent events, but not on questions about childhood. However, the decline in autobiographical memory was not graded; recall dropped abruptly after childhood. On the other hand, we could not find a significant gradient for the recall of famous people and well-known events, and this discrepancy may be related to differing complexity levels between RMS and AMS.
Weiskrantz22 suggested that improvements of biographical memory on recognition testing as compared to free recall may indicate a retrieval deficit, and Dall'Ora et al.8 suggested that autobiographical memory deficits may result from defective organization of search. We found significantly better performance in our AD patients in the recognition condition of the AMS as compared to free recall, suggesting that retrieval deficits may play an important role in the autobiographical memory deficits of AD patients. We could not compare AD and control subjects on the recognition condition because control subjects scored at a ceiling level.
An important though rarely studied issue is the association between autobiographical and anterograde memory deficits. Kopelman4 found a significant correlation between performance on the logical memory section of the Wechsler Memory Scale (a measure of verbal anterograde memory) and scores on a scale of autobiographical incidents (r=0.52, P<0.05). In the present study, there was a positive correlation between AMS scores and tests of anterograde and retrograde nonautobiographical memory, but a regression analysis demonstrated that after variance in AMS performance due to naming deficits was controlled for, anterograde and remote memory deficits accounted for no additional variance in AMS scores. A similar lack of significant correlations between autobiographic memory and a test of public memory was recently reported by Greene and Hodges.10
Finally, we could not find a significant association between frontal lobe tests (such as verbal fluency and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) and scores on the remote memory tasks. Kopelman11 and Greene and Hodges6 found a significant (albeit low) correlation between frontal lobe tasks and remote memory; however, other neuropsychological tasks (e.g., language, attention, praxis) were not entered into the regression equation in their studies.

CONCLUSION

Our study presented a new scale for the measurement of remote memory, which assessed both famous names and well-known events by using a free recall and a recognition format. We found that AD patients had significant deficits in remote memory. These were already present in the stage of mild dementia and were similarly prevalent across the four decades assessed. We also designed a scale to measure biographical memory deficits in AD. Patients with AD showed significant biographical memory deficits compared with age-comparable normal control subjects, with a relatively better preservation of older than more recent memories. Future studies may examine the brain topographical correlates of biographical and nonbiographical remote amnesia in AD.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This study was partially supported by grants from the Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research and the Fundación Perez Companc. The authors thank Fred Bylsma, Ph.D., for his helpful suggestions.
FIGURE 1. Remote Memory Scale (free recall), pilot version, young vsolder control subjects: young control subjects (ages 24–35 years) showed significantly lower free recall for well-known events and famous names of the 1950s, '60s, and '70s compared with older control subjects (ages 36–83). *Group×decade, F=10.0, df=3,54 P<0.0001.
FIGURE 2. Remote Memory Scale (free recall and recognition), AD vscontrol subjects: Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients scored significantly lower than normal control subjects on both the free recall (FR) and multiple choice (MC) sections. Group F=38.1, df=1,78, P<0.0001.
FIGURE 3. Remote Memory Scale (free recall), AD subgroups vscontrol subjects: Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients with either very mild (MMSE 24–30), mild (MMSE 17–23), or moderate dementia (MMSE 10–16) scored significantly lower than normal control subjects on the free recall section. MMSE=Mini-Mental State Examination. Group F=24.4, df=3,76, P<0.0001.
FIGURE 4. Autobiographical Memory Scale (free recall), AD vscontrol subjects: Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients showed significantly more deficits than age-comparable normal control (NC) subjects on the free recall section of the AMS. *F= 16.0, df=1,53, P<0.001.
FIGURE 5. Autobiographical Memory Scale (free recall vsrecognition), AD patients: Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients showed significantly more deficits on the free recall (FR) than on the recognition (REC) section of the AMS. *Test effect F=11.7, df=1,24, P<0.01.
TABLE 1. Studies of remote memory in Alzheimer's disease (AD)

References

1.
Wilson BA, Cockburn J: The Prices Test: a simple test of retrograde amnesia, in Practical Aspects of Memory: Current Research and Issues, edited by Gruneberg MM, Morris PE, Sykes RN. Chichester, UK, Wiley, 1988
2.
Sagar HJ, Cohen NJ, Sullivan EV, et al: Remote memory function in Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease. Brain 1988; 111:185–206
3.
Beatty WW, Salmon DP, Butters N, et al: Retrograde amnesia in patients with Alzheimer's disease or Huntington's disease. Neurobiol Aging 1988; 9:181–186
4.
Kopelman MD: Remote and autobiographical memory, temporal context memory and frontal atrophy in Korsakoff and Alzheimer patients. Neuropsychologia 1989; 27:437–460
5.
Leplow B, Dierks C, Herrmann P, et al: Remote memory in Parkinson's disease and senile dementia. Neuropsychologia 1997; 35:547–557
6.
Greene JDW, Hodges JR: Identification of famous faces and famous names in early Alzheimer's disease: relationship to anterograde episodic and general semantic memory. Brain 1996; 119:111–128
7.
Storandt M, Kaskie B, Von Dras DD: Temporal memory for remote events in healthy aging and dementia. Psychology Aging 1998; 12:4–7
8.
Dall'Ora P, Della Sala S, Spinnler H: Autobiographical memory. Its impairment in amnesic syndromes. Cortex 1989; 25:197–217
9.
Greene JW, Hodges JR, Baddeley AD: Autobiographical memory and executive function in early dementia of Alzheimer type. Neuropsychologia 1995; 33:1647–1670
10.
Greene JW, Hodges JR: The fractionation of remote memory: evidence from a longitudinal study of dementia of Alzheimer type. Brain 1996; 119:129–142
11.
Kopelman MD: Frontal dysfunction and memory deficits in the alcoholic Korsakoff syndrome and Alzheimer-type dementia. Brain 1991; 114:117–137
12.
Mc Khann G, Drachman D, Folstein MF, et al: Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1984; 34:939–944
13.
Buschke H, Fuld PA: Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology 1974; 24:1019–1025
14.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Manual. New York, The Psychological Corporation, 1955
15.
Nelson HL: A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects. Cortex 1976; 12:313–324
16.
Benton AL: Differential behavioral effects in frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia 1968; 6:53–60
17.
Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S: The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, Lea and Febiger, 1983
18.
De Renzi E, Faglioni P: Development of a shortened version of the Token Test. Cortex 1978; 14:41–49
19.
Raven JC, Court JH, Raven J: Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. London, HK Lewis, 1986
20.
Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198
21.
Mayes AR: Human Organic Memory Disorders. New York, Cambridge University Press, 1988
22.
Weiskrantz L: Neuroanatomy of memory and amnesia: a case of multiple memory systems. Human Neurobiology 1987; 6:93–105

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 490 - 497
PubMed: 10570763

History

Published online: 1 November 1999
Published in print: November 1999

Authors

Details

María Flavia Dorrego, Ph.D.
Received September 16, 1998; revised December 14, 1998; accepted April 21, 1999. From the Department of Neuropsychiatry, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Buenos Aires, Argentina; and Unite 324 INSERM, Paris, France. Address correspondence to Dr. Dorrego, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Montañeses 2325, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina; e-mail: [email protected]
Liliana Sabe, Ph.D.
Received September 16, 1998; revised December 14, 1998; accepted April 21, 1999. From the Department of Neuropsychiatry, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Buenos Aires, Argentina; and Unite 324 INSERM, Paris, France. Address correspondence to Dr. Dorrego, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Montañeses 2325, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina; e-mail: [email protected]
Agustina García Cuerva, Ph.D.
Received September 16, 1998; revised December 14, 1998; accepted April 21, 1999. From the Department of Neuropsychiatry, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Buenos Aires, Argentina; and Unite 324 INSERM, Paris, France. Address correspondence to Dr. Dorrego, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Montañeses 2325, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina; e-mail: [email protected]
Gabriela Kuzis, Ph.D.
Received September 16, 1998; revised December 14, 1998; accepted April 21, 1999. From the Department of Neuropsychiatry, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Buenos Aires, Argentina; and Unite 324 INSERM, Paris, France. Address correspondence to Dr. Dorrego, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Montañeses 2325, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina; e-mail: [email protected]
Cecilia Tiberti, Ph.D.
Received September 16, 1998; revised December 14, 1998; accepted April 21, 1999. From the Department of Neuropsychiatry, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Buenos Aires, Argentina; and Unite 324 INSERM, Paris, France. Address correspondence to Dr. Dorrego, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Montañeses 2325, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina; e-mail: [email protected]
François Boller, M.D.
Received September 16, 1998; revised December 14, 1998; accepted April 21, 1999. From the Department of Neuropsychiatry, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Buenos Aires, Argentina; and Unite 324 INSERM, Paris, France. Address correspondence to Dr. Dorrego, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Montañeses 2325, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina; e-mail: [email protected]
Sergio E. Starkstein, M.D., Ph.D.
Received September 16, 1998; revised December 14, 1998; accepted April 21, 1999. From the Department of Neuropsychiatry, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Buenos Aires, Argentina; and Unite 324 INSERM, Paris, France. Address correspondence to Dr. Dorrego, Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, Montañeses 2325, 1428 Buenos Aires, Argentina; e-mail: [email protected]

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share