Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Article
Published Online: 1 February 2002

Impaired Visuomotor Function in Schizophrenic Patients Compared With Control Subjects

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

Visuomotor function was studied in 36 schizophrenic patients treated with atypical antipsychotics and in 22 control subjects. Patients showed significant disturbances in ability to control movement direction when tracing objects on screen and in keeping pace with a moving target in tracking tests. The impairments were not related to medication dose or to extrapyramidal side effects. Visuomotor impairment may be part of illness-related pathology in schizophrenia.
Neurological abnormalities are common in schizophrenia1 and discriminate between patients and healthy subjects2 irrespective of treatment. Medication-induced extrapyramidal side effects may further impair neurological function. Disturbances in pursuit and saccadic eye movements are also common in schizophrenia and may be trait markers for the illness.36 Such disturbances may impair the ability to manually track moving visual targets.
In addition to normal ocular control, eye–hand coordination requires intact frontal-parietal-temporal connections,79 which are postulated to be disturbed in schizophrenia.10,11 Hence it can be predicted that visuomotor impairment, too, may be a part of the illness process.
We tested this prediction by studying visuomotor coordination in patients with schizophrenia, using a sensitive visuomotor testing system (VMT) developed for detection of early Parkinson's disease.12 To minimize potential confounds from drug-induced extrapyramidal side effects, only patients treated with atypical antipsychotics were studied and compared with normal control subjects.

METHODS

Subjects

Thirty-six patients (25 males, 11 females) and 22 control subjects (10 males, 12 females) were compared. Patients were recruited from inpatient and day patient populations of the Flugelman (Mazra) psychiatric hospital. All were physically healthy.
Patients fulfilled the DSM-IV criteria for chronic schizophrenia (10 paranoid type, 25 disorganized type, and 1 residual type) and were treated with atypical antipsychotics (risperidone: n=12, mean dose=4.3±1.5 mg; olanzapine: n=17, mean dose=14.7±3.7 mg; clozapine: n=7, mean dose=350±153 mg) for at least 6 weeks. Ten patients also received biperiden (mean dose=3.6±1.6 mg), and 6 received trihexyphenidyl (mean dose=10.8±4.9 mg).
Control subjects were drawn from hospital staff. All were neurologically healthy, had no psychiatric disorders, and received no psychoactive medication.
All subjects gave written informed consent after a full explanation of study procedures. The study was approved by the institutional ethics committee.
Patients were significantly younger than control subjects (mean±SD: 40.6±10.7 years vs. 55.3±12.0 years, P<0.01). There was no significant difference in gender distribution between the groups (chi-square, Fisher's exact test, P=0.1). The sociodemographic and illness characteristics of the patient population are shown in Table 1.

Clinical Assessment

The same experienced rater (N.S.) performed clinical assessment for all patients. General psychopathology was assessed with the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale,13 scored 0–6; negative symptoms with the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS);14 positive symptoms with the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS);15 and extrapyramidal side effects with the Neurological Rating Scales for Extrapyramidal Side Effects (SA)16 and the Abnormal Involuntary Movement Scale (AIMS).17

Instrumentation

All visuomotor tests employed a computerized system consisting of a digitizing tablet and a PC. The tablet was placed at a lower chest level and hidden from the subject's view by an overlying board, positioned 16 cm above it and supporting a computer monitor on which paths for tracing and for tracking were displayed. A screen cursor represented the location of an unseen dome-shaped manipulandum, containing the digitizer's stylus, which could be moved freely over the surface of the digitizing tablet. The location of the manipulandum was read every 10 ms, with a resolution of 0.05 mm. A one-to-one correspondence between movements of the manipulandum and movements of the screen cursor was maintained.

Tests

Tracing:

A path (described below) and a cursor were displayed on screen. The subjects brought the cursor to a designated starting point from which they moved it along the entire path, as accurately as possible, by use of the unseen manipulandum. No demands on speed were made, but the tracing test was stopped automatically 64 seconds after it started even if not completed entirely.

Tracking:

The same path and cursor were used. A 1-cm target circle was programmed to move along the path at a predetermined speed of 22 mm/s (square and circular path) and 19 mm/s (minimum=16 mm/s at the curved peaks, maximum=22 mm/s at the straight middles) along the sine-wave path. The subject needed to maintain the cursor within the target in order to keep it moving. Whenever the cursor left the target, the latter stopped moving (tracking interruption) until the cursor was returned into it.

Paths:

Three path types were used: sine-wave, square, and circle. This combination affords the VMT a wide range of task difficulties, from simple straight movements (square path) through a constant change in direction (circular path) to a variable change in direction and speed (sine-wave path). Regarding the square path, the data recorded at the corners (i.e., at the points of directional change) were ignored. All three path types were used with each hand for tracing and for tracking. The VMT results (below) are given as grand averages across all path types.

Scores

Performance was evaluated offline by use of the following measures:
1.
Mean total time (MnTrkT) of tracking test performance.
2.
Mean distance (MnDist) between the model path and the path traversed by the hand.
3.
The directional error (DirEr) of hand movement. This consisted of the instantaneous movement component heading in a direction perpendicular to the model path, expressed as percentage of the total movement vector. The percentage of movement time during which the DirEr exceeded a level of 50% (PT50%) was calculated. This number expressed the relative test time during which movement advanced in a direction more perpendicular than parallel to the model path.
4.
Mean hand movement velocity during tracing (MnVtrc).
5.
The mean number of tracking interruptions (MnNints).
In addition, a global measure of performance (GPM) was constructed, consisting of the PT50% in all tracing tests and MnNints in all tracking tests, as two independent cardinal measures of performance18 according to the following equation: GPM=√[(MnPT50%)2+(MnNints)2].

Procedure

Each subject was tested on tracking of the sine-wave path with the right hand and then with the left hand. The subjects then traced the same path with each hand. The same sequence was repeated using the square path and then, again, with the circular path. Before actual testing began, each subject performed a trial session of tracking along a sine-wave path with the right and left hand.

Statistical Analysis

For statistical analyses we used SPSS software. Performance differences between patients and control subjects were assessed by analysis of variance.
The relationship of test performance to independent clinical and treatment variables was determined through linear regression using Pearson's r. Two-tailed significance was used throughout.

RESULTS

Table 2 shows the performance of patients and control subjects on the five parameters tested. Schizophrenic patients performed significantly worse than control subjects on all measures except velocity of tracing. VMT performance was not influenced by age or sex. In the patient group there was no significant correlation between VMT measures and extrapyramidal side effects or involuntary movements. (GPM vs. SA score: r=–0.17 right hand, r=–0.11 left hand, not significant; GPM vs. AIMS score: r=0.12 for both hands, not significant). There was no significant correlation between VMT measures and scale scores, except for BPRS, which showed a correlation with PT50% right hand (r=0.37, P=0.03).
There was no consistent significant correlation between VMT performance and illness parameters. These included age at first admission (except for correlation with PT50% left hand: r=0.37, P=0.03), illness duration (except for correlation with MnDist left hand: r=0.44, P=0.008), number of admissions, and accumulated time in hospital (except for correlation with PT50% right hand: r=0.34, P=<0.05, and MnDist right hand: r=0.49, P=0.003).
Right hand performance was worse than left in both groups.
There was no difference in VMT performance in patients taking different atypical antipsychotics or between patients with or without anticholinergic treatment.

DISCUSSION

The main finding of the present study was that schizophrenic patients showed impaired visuomotor function compared with normal subjects. The deficits were mainly in the ability to control movement direction when tracing simple patterns and in keeping pace with a moving target in tracking tests. Velocity of movement, however, was not impaired.
The patients' impairment was not due to extrapyramidal side effects: the patients were on atypical antipsychotics, they had no or few extrapyramidal symptoms (EPS) as measured on the SA scale, and there was no relationship between SA scores and VMT performance. Likewise, anticholinergic co-administration did not influence results. Nonetheless, the possibility that subtle effects of medication not detected by scales contributed to the findings cannot be entirely excluded.
There was no consistent relationship between VMT performance and illness characteristics such as age at first admission, length of illness, or cumulative hospitalization time, although some correlations were noted. Nor was there an overall significant relationship with levels of positive or negative symptoms.
The lack of relationship to these illness variables suggests that visuomotor deficit may be a traitlike characteristic in schizophrenia, linked to basic CNS pathology similar to eye movement abnormalities.5 However, the possibility that factors related to illness chronicity contributed to the findings cannot be excluded. In this regard, nonspecific factors such as poor motivation or generalized motor slowing did not appear to be significant confounds, since the patients cooperated willingly and their movement velocity on tracing tests did not differ from that of control subjects.
Of the many changes that underlie and accompany schizophrenia, the reduced connectivity between remote cortical regions10,11 is likely to be a major factor in reducing visuomotor capabilities. As already noted, normal visuomotor function depends on availability of visuospatial information, processed in posterior parietal and superior temporal areas, to premotor regions.79 Reduced corticocortical connectivity is likely to attenuate this information flow to a level that impairs performance. Similarly, reduced interhemispheric cross-talk may result in greater manual asymmetry during execution of tasks that are controlled preferentially by one hemisphere or the other. Visuomotor coordination is thought to be related to right hemisphere processes19 and is therefore likely to be more impaired in the right hand when interhemispheric communication is disrupted. However, we did not perform a detailed assessment of performance by hand.
Impaired control of eye movements, well documented in schizophrenia,2022 may also contribute to visuomotor deficits. Normal control of eye movements is necessary for successful visually guided hand movements. In addition, the same neural pathology causing abnormal saccades and visual tracking may be involved in reduced coordination,23 so that impaired eye movements may interfere with visuomanual coordination directly and through a shared neural abnormality.
There is evidence that eye movement dysfunction, in particular the characteristic deficit in velocity discrimination,5,24,25 may be localized to motion-sensitive areas of the parietal lobe, middle temporal (MT), and medial superior temporal (MST) areas and their associated networks,5,24,25 including in the prefrontal cortex26 and the occipital lobe.27 It is possible that defects in these areas may also underlie VMT abnormalities.
Impaired attention, common in schizophrenia,28,29 may also reduce VMT. Lowered attentional resources are likely to increase the number of tracking interruptions and may be manifested in greater distance from the desired path during tracing. Our results show that patients with schizophrenia perform much worse than normal subjects on both variables.
In summary, the present study shows a marked reduction in the visuomotor capabilities of individuals with schizophrenia, despite the absence of extrapyramidal side effects and despite treatment with atypical neuroleptics. The possibility that this deficit may be a core attribute of schizophrenia that can be related to other well-documented functional changes in this disease merits further study.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors acknowledge the competent assistance of Henia Ben David and thank Rena Kurs for assistance in preparation of the manuscript.
TABLE 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the patient population
TABLE 2. Visuomotor coordination in schizophrenia patients versus control subjects

References

1.
Heinrichs DW, Buchanan EW: Significance and meaning of neurological signs in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1988; 145:11-18
2.
Arango C, Bartko JJ, Gold J, et al: Prediction of neuropsychological performance by neurological signs in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1349-1357
3.
Holzman PS, Proctor LR, Hughes DW: Eye tracking patterns in schizophrenia. Science 1973; 181:179-181
4.
Holzman PS, Proctor LR, Levy DL, et al: Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenic patients and their relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1974; 31:143-151
5.
Holzman PS: Eye movements and the search for the essence of schizophrenia. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 2000; 31:350-356
6.
Clementz BA, Sweeney JA: Is eye movement dysfunction a biological marker for schizophrenia? A methodological review. Psychol Bull 1990; 108:77-92
7.
Schwartz AB: Distributed motor processing in cerebral cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1994; 4:840-846
8.
Sakata S, Taira M, Kusunoki M, et al: The TINS Lecture: the parietal association cortex in depth perception and visual control of hand action. Trends Neurosci (TINS) 1997;20:350-357
9.
Johnson PB, Ferraina S, Caminiti R: Cortical networks for visual reaching. Exp Brain Res 1993; 97:361-365
10.
Frith C: Functional imaging and cognitive abnormalities. Lancet 1995; 346:615-620
11.
Liddle PF: Functional imaging: schizophrenia. Br Med Bull 1996; 96:486-494
12.
Hocherman S: Quantitative clinical evaluation of parkinsonism based on visuomotor tracking and tracing, in Instrumental Methods and Scoring in Extrapyramidal Disorders, edited by Prezuntek H, Kraus PH, Klotz P, et al. New York, Springer, 1995, pp 196-202
13.
Overall JE, Gorham DR: The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychol Rep 1962; 10:799-368
14.
Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, IA, The University of Iowa, 1983
15.
Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, IA, The University of Iowa, 1984
16.
Simpson GM, Angus JWS: A rating scale for extrapyramidal side effects. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 1970; 212:11-19
17.
Guy W (ed): ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology (Publ No ADM 76-338). Washington, DC, US Department of Health, Education and Welfare, 1976
18.
Hocherman S, Aharon-Peretz J: Two-dimensional tracing and tracking in patients with Parkinson's disease. Neurology 1994; 44:111-116
19.
Haaland KY, Harrington DL: Hemispheric asymmetry of movement. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1996; 6:796-800
20.
Lieberman JA, Jody D, Alvir JMJ, et al: Brain morphology, dopamine, and eye-tracking abnormalities in first-episode schizophrenia: prevalence and clinical correlates. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993; 50:357-368
21.
Ross DE, Thaker GK, Holcomb HH, et al: Abnormal smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenic patients are associated with cerebral glucose metabolism in oculomotor regions. Psychiatry Res 1995; 95:53-67
22.
Rosenberg DR, Sweeney JA, Squires-Wheeler E, et al: Eye-tracking dysfunction in offspring from the New York High-Risk Project: diagnostic specificity and the role of attention. Psychiatry Res 1997; 66:121-130
23.
Krauzlis RJ, Stone LS: Tracking with the mind's eye. Trends Neurosci 1999; 22:544-550
24.
Chen Y, Palafox GP, Nakayama K, et al: Motion perception in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:149-154
25.
Chen Y, Levy DL, Nakayama K, et al: Dependence of impaired eye tracking on deficient velocity discrimination in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:155-161
26.
Levin S: Frontal lobe dysfunctions in schizophrenia: eye movement impairments. J Psychiatr Res 1984; 18:27-55
27.
Plant GT, Laxer KD, Barbaro NM, et al: Impaired visual motion perception in the contralateral hemifield following unilateral posterior cerebral lesions in humans. Brain 1993; 116:1303-1335
28.
Schatz J: Cognitive processing efficiency in schizophrenia: generalized vs. domain-specific deficits. Schizophr Res 1998; 30:41-49
29.
Schreiber H, Stolz-Born G, Heinrich H, et al: Attention, cognition, and motor perseveration in adolescents at genetic risk for schizophrenia and control subjects. Psychiatry Res 1992; 44:125-140

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 72 - 76
PubMed: 11884658

History

Published online: 1 February 2002
Published in print: February 2002

Authors

Details

Henry Silver, M.B.B.S., B.Med.Sci., D.P.M., F.R.A.N.Z.C.P.
Received July 7, 2000; revised October 10, 2000; accepted October 19, 2000. From Flugelman (Mazra) Psychiatric Hospital, Mobile Post Ashrat, Israel; and Bruce Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel. Address correspondence to Dr. Silver, Sha'ar Menashe Mental Health Center, Mobile Post Hefer 38814, Israel. E-mail: [email protected].
Nili Shlomo, M.D.
Received July 7, 2000; revised October 10, 2000; accepted October 19, 2000. From Flugelman (Mazra) Psychiatric Hospital, Mobile Post Ashrat, Israel; and Bruce Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel. Address correspondence to Dr. Silver, Sha'ar Menashe Mental Health Center, Mobile Post Hefer 38814, Israel. E-mail: [email protected].
Miguel Schwartz, M.D.
Received July 7, 2000; revised October 10, 2000; accepted October 19, 2000. From Flugelman (Mazra) Psychiatric Hospital, Mobile Post Ashrat, Israel; and Bruce Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel. Address correspondence to Dr. Silver, Sha'ar Menashe Mental Health Center, Mobile Post Hefer 38814, Israel. E-mail: [email protected].
Shraga Hocherman, Ph.D.
Received July 7, 2000; revised October 10, 2000; accepted October 19, 2000. From Flugelman (Mazra) Psychiatric Hospital, Mobile Post Ashrat, Israel; and Bruce Rappaport Faculty of Medicine, Technion Institute of Technology, Haifa, Israel. Address correspondence to Dr. Silver, Sha'ar Menashe Mental Health Center, Mobile Post Hefer 38814, Israel. E-mail: [email protected].

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share