C hildhood abuse is a major risk factor in the development of psychopathology
1 –
4 and is also associated with a host of neuropsychological and neurocognitive consequences.
5 Recent studies suggest that clinical sequelae may stem, at least in part, from enduring adverse effects on brain development.
6 The nature and severity of the effects will likely depend on genetic predisposition,
7,
8 frequency, severity, and multiplicity of the stressors,
1,
8 –
10 gender,
9,
11 and timing of the insult.
9 Generally, early onset and longer duration of abuse have been associated with greater morphological change,
9 but this may be an oversimplification. An alternative hypothesis is that stress-susceptible brain regions have their own unique sensitive periods (or windows of vulnerability) to the effects of early stress.
12 In practice these two hypotheses may not appear that different, as longer periods of abuse may be more likely to intersect a sensitive period, and many brain regions probably have a relatively early (prepubertal) window of vulnerability. Nevertheless, this is a critical distinction which could substantially enhance our understanding of the neuropsychiatric effects of abuse and shed new light on the underlying temporal aspects of gene × environment interactions that lay at the heart of most psychiatric vulnerabilities.
The concept that brain regions go through stages when they are maximally sensitive to experience emerged from the landmark studies of Hubel and Wiesel.
13 They found that binocular deprivation affected development of the visual cortex in cats if it occurred early in postnatal life, but had no impact after puberty. Little evidence exists for sensitive periods in human brain development. Postnatal sensitive periods have been delineated for development of speech, language,
14 –
16 and binocular vision.
17 However, a vast array of stimuli and experiences are likely to affect brain development throughout a host of sensitive periods that await discovery.
Stress has been identified as a key experiential factor that programs and modifies brain development.
12,
18 Exposure to physical or sexual abuse resulting in psychopathology has been associated with attenuated left hemisphere maturation,
19,
20 diminished size of the corpus callosum,
9,
11,
20,
21 reduced hippocampal volume in adults
22 –
25 (but not children
9,
21,
26,
27 ), and alterations in gray matter volume, symmetry, and neuronal integrity of the frontal cortex.
21,
26,
28The aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that stress-sensitive brain regions have their own developmental time windows when they are maximally vulnerable to the effects of early stress. This is a relatively easy hypothesis to test in a preclinical study, where stress can be administered during specific developmental stages. It is much harder to test this hypothesis in humans as there is a great deal of heterogeneity in onset and duration of abuse and frequent co-occurrence of other forms of stress. A secondary aim of this study was to propose an analytical strategy to facilitate the exploration of sensitive period effects for complex phenomena such as childhood sexual abuse.
In order to test this hypothesis, measures of hippocampal, corpus callosum, frontal cortex, and amygdala size were obtained from MRI scans of female college students with self-reported histories of childhood sexual abuse that occurred at different ages, and from healthy socio-demographically comparable control subjects. The study was predicated on the hypotheses that the hippocampus would have an early period of vulnerability, based on the observation that hippocampal synaptogenesis is strongly influenced by variations in maternal care and availability.
29,
30 The slowly maturing frontal cortex
31 would have a late period of vulnerability and may be resistant to the effects of early stress given our observation that exposure to early isolation stress in rats affected synaptic density in the hippocampus but had no sustained effect on synaptic density in the prefrontal cortex.
30 The corpus callosum would likely have an intermediate period of vulnerability in females based on our observation that sexual abuse was associated with reduced corpus callosum area in females, while neglect (generally an earlier problem) was associated with reduced corpus callosum area in males.
11 Finally, we predicted that the amygdala would not have a prominent period of vulnerability, based on reports of normal amygdaloid volumes in abused subjects.
9,
22,
25,
26In this article, we provide evidence for discrete regional periods sensitive to the effects of childhood sexual abuse, with the hippocampus having the earliest period of vulnerability and the frontal cortex having the latest.
METHOD
Subjects
Physically healthy, unmedicated, right-handed individuals ages 18–22 years were recruited via advertisements looking for individuals interested in participating in “psychiatric research.”
5 Primary entry criterion was a history of three or more episodes of forced contact childhood sexual abuse that ended at least 2 years prior to enrollment. Childhood sexual abuse was defined as forced involuntary contact with the sexual part of the victim’s or the perpetrator’s body. Contact had to be accompanied by threats of harm to self or others, or feelings of fear or terror. History of childhood sexual abuse was supported by written response and by consistent results of a lengthy structured interview conducted by a certified clinician using the Traumatic Antecedents Questionnaire.
32Rigorous exclusion criteria were applied to select subjects in whom differences in brain morphology could be most clearly attributable to childhood sexual abuse. They included neurological disorders, medical disorder affecting growth or development, treatment with corticosteroids, pregnancy, past or present alcohol/substance abuse, premature birth or complications during mother’s pregnancy or delivery, in utero exposure to alcohol or drugs, or a history of physical abuse. Exclusion criteria also included exposure to any other forms of preceding or subsequent trauma (e.g., motor vehicle accidents, natural disasters, fires, near drowning, witnessing abuse, animal attacks, gang violence, robbery). These criteria excluded 95% of the 732 initial respondents to our advertisement. Twenty-eight percent of the subjects who completed all of the prescreening instruments (N=564) had a self-reported history of childhood sexual abuse, but only 9.5% of the prescreened sample had a history of childhood sexual abuse unaccompanied by exposure to other forms of abuse.
Using these criteria, 26 abused women (mean age=20.0 years, range=18–22) and 17 healthy female comparison subjects (mean age=19.4 years, range=18–22) were enrolled. Comparison subjects, selected from the same pool of respondents, had no current or past DSM-IV Axis I disorder on Structured Clinical Interviews
33 and had no history of abuse or exposure to other traumatic events. Subjects were predominantly middle class or above (96%) and the two groups were similar in measures of socioeconomic status (Hollingshead index
34 : 2.3±0.9 versus 2.0±0.6; F=1.95, df=1, 41, p=0.17). Subjects were paid for participation and provided written, informed consent. The study was approved and monitored by the McLean Hospital Institutional Review Board. Specific information about each subject is presented in
Table 1 . None of the subjects recruited had more than a minimal history of drug or alcohol use, and no subject met criteria for borderline personality disorder. None of the subjects enrolled were seeking treatment.
Rationale for Selection Procedure
Subjects were selected based on exposure history regardless of psychiatric outcome (except for substance abuse, which could directly affect brain development). This differs from prior studies that focused on abused subjects with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).
9,
19,
22,
23 Selecting subjects with childhood sexual abuse regardless of outcome facilitates the relatively unbiased assessment of the morphometric effects of abuse during specific developmental stages. Preselection of subjects based on a history of PTSD (or other psychopathologies) would bias morphometric findings toward those brain regions involved in the disorder. This distinction is important when the goal is to ascertain the possible effects of exposure to early stress at key developmental points and less relevant if the goal is to delineate the neurobiology of an underlying psychiatric disorder such as PTSD.
Imaging Methods
T1-weighted coronal sections (3-D, spoiled gradient recalled acquisition in the steady state; TR=40 msec; TE=5 msec; NEX=2; flip angle 40°; fov 24 cm; matrix 256×128; 124 sections of 1.5 mm thickness, no gaps) were acquired using a 1.5 T magnetic resonance scanner (Echospeed; GE Medical Systems). Raters were blind to the identity and history of each subject and provided results from images in which definitive measurements could be made based on the distinctiveness of landmarks, borders, image quality, and motion artifact.
The hippocampus and amygdala were traced in their entirety according to the method detailed by Pruessner et al.
35 This technique yielded excellent reliability (intrarater ICC 0.91 to 0.95, interrater 0.83 to 0.94), though raters rejected 20% of the sample because of ambiguity in delineating one or more borders. Manual tracing is currently considered optimal for measuring the volume of these two regions.
36The midsagittal corpus callosum area was manually traced using the NIH Image program and an automated algorithm divided it into seven regions as defined by Witelson.
37 Previous studies indicated that midbody regions 3–6 were most significantly affected by abuse or neglect.
9,
11,
21 Region 3, rostral body, was selected for sensitive period analysis as this region showed the greatest overall vulnerability to childhood sexual abuse (regardless of age of abuse) in the present sample.
Gray matter volume of frontal cortex was assessed using a semiautomated program for cortical surface-based analysis (FreeSurfer).
38 –
40 A composite measure of average frontal lobe gray matter volume was obtained by combining measures from all parts of the inferior frontal, middle frontal, superior frontal, orbital, suborbital, transverse, frontopolar and cingulate gyri, and sulci.
Data Analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted to provide evidence for sensitive periods in a clinical population where abuse occurred over a variable number of years. Subjects with childhood sexual abuse had experienced abuse for an average of 4.2±2.4 years. In most cases abuse occurred in continuous years (70%). The remainder had an average gap of 5.0±2.7 years between clusters of abuse. This situation presents challenges to conventional between-group designs, so alternative statistical methods were used.
The first method was multiple regression analysis (SPSS), in which the primary assumption was that abuse during different stages of development would exert additive effects on regional brain morphometry. If this assumption is true, then it is possible to determine whether abuse during one or more stages of development was associated with a particularly significant reduction in regional brain size. The reality may be more complicated; there may be interactive effects between abuse at different stages, but an additive multiple regression model provides a parsimonious initial approach and is a good starting point if the model provides a reasonable fit to the available data.
Subjects were characterized by the density of childhood sexual abuse they experienced during sequential developmental stages. Density of childhood sexual abuse was defined as number of years of abuse experienced during the stage divided by number of years in the stage. The designated stages we compared were preschool (3–5 years), latency (6–8 years), prepubertal (9–10 years), pubertal (11–13 years), and adolescent (14–16 years), based on the ages of exposure. The numbers of subjects who experienced abuse during sequential stages were 13, 16, 7, 10, and 7, respectively. Stages were selected to be relatively short to make it possible to detect multiple distinct windows of vulnerability during the prepubertal period, as preclinical studies suggest that vulnerable periods may be brief.
41Additional independent variables of potential significance were intracranial volume, midsagittal area, total gray matter volume, socioeconomic status, list recall, history of depression, and history of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). List recall was selected as a potential covariate for hippocampal volume given the role of the hippocampus in verbal declarative memory, and observation of a strong correlation between hippocampal volume and measures of list recall.
42 Multiple regression analyses were performed without data transformations as all of the morphometric measures were normally distributed (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, hippocampus: z=0.457, p>0.9; amygdala: z=0.622, p>0.8; corpus callosum: z=0.820, p>0.5; frontal cortex: z=0.508, p>0.9).
Path analysis was performed using structural equation modeling with Amos Graphics (http://www.assess.com/Software/AMOS.htm) as a confirmatory statistical procedure. Path analysis is a specific form of structural equation modeling in which there are no latent variables (all variables are directly measured). This approach makes several assumptions, particularly that there are an adequate number of known correlations or covariances as inputs to generate a sensible set of results, and that there is a unique or best solution. Path analysis was guided by the robust results of the multiple regression analysis and made feasible with limited sample size given the strength of the associations and normality of the data. Structural equation modeling provides a sophisticated analysis that accounts for the tendency of abusive experiences to carry over into subsequent stages. Further, structural equation modeling controls for problems with multiple comparisons by placing the entire analysis into a single statistical model. Full-information maximum likelihood estimation was used to minimize the discrepancy fit function defined by the available data points, pathways and coefficients.
With structural equation modeling, the null hypothesis is that the model fits the data. Statistically, the absolute fit of the model is tested using a chi-square procedure in which a p value <0.05 leads to the rejection of the model. With a global chi-square p value >0.05 one can then provisionally accept the given model along with p values indicating the significance of the individual pathways. The Tucker-Lewis Index and Comparative Fit Index served as measures of relative fit to ascertain how parsimoniously the model fit the data in comparison to other models.
Data from all subjects were used but brain sizes for all regions could not be ascertained with confidence in every subject. Thus, the number of subjects for analysis were 21 abuse subjects/16 comparison subjects for hippocampus and amygdala, 23 abuse subjects/16 comparison subjects for corpus callosum, and 21 abuse subjects/15 comparison subjects for frontal cortex gray matter volume. Path analysis evaluated associations between morphometric measures and density of abuse during each stage. Density of abuse was used instead of presence or absence of abuse during each stage, as Amos Graphics cannot use simple dichotomous variables, and density of abuse was the simplest acceptable alternative.
RESULTS
Multiple regression analysis identified three variables that were significantly associated with hippocampal volume (overall r=0.837, adjusted r
2 =0.580, F=5.84, df=10, 25, p<0.0001;
Table 2 ). Density of abuse at index stage 3–5 years old (p<0.0004) was associated with reduced volume. List recall (p<0.002) and intracranial volume (p=0.001) were positively correlated with hippocampal volume. Density of abuse at stage 11–13 years old (p=0.054) was marginal. No other stage of abuse significantly enhanced goodness of fit, nor did a history of PTSD or depression. List recall on the Memory Assessment Scale
43 was a highly significant covariate. Subjects with childhood sexual abuse in this sample had measures of list recall that were as high as healthy comparison subjects (11.56±1.75 versus 10.59±2.09, F=2.65, df=1, 40, p>0.10). This covariate did not compensate for group differences but reduced the degree of scatter between subjects in both groups. A measure of list recall was also found to be highly correlated with hippocampal volume by Tischler et al.
42 While including list recall in the multiple regression analysis enhanced goodness of fit, and is appropriate, the same basic results were obtained without this covariate (e.g., overall correlation: r=0.740, p=0.006; density of abuse stage 3–5 years old: β=-0.535, p=0.004).
Multiple regression analysis failed to identify any index stage that was associated with a significant effect on the total volume of the amygdala.
Two variables were associated with reduced callosal area (r=0.691, adjusted r 2 =0.321, F=3.05, df=9, 30, p<0.01). These were midsagittal area (p<0.002) and density of abuse at index stage 9–10 years old (p=0.03).
Multiple regression analysis identified two variables that were associated with frontal cortex gray matter volume (r=0.793, adjusted r 2 =0.512, F=5.08, df=9, 26, p<0.001). The first was total gray matter volume (p<0.0001) and the second was density of abuse at index stage 14–16 years old (p<0.01).
Figure 1 illustrates the composite path analysis derived from structural equation modeling. The model provides a satisfactory fit to the data (χ
2 =13.82, df=9, p>0.1). Hippocampal volume was significantly influenced by density of abuse at stages 3–5 years old (p<0.0001) and 11–13 years old (p<0.05). Corpus callosum was influenced by density of abuse at 9–10 years old (p<0.005) and frontal cortex gray matter volume was influenced by abuse at 14–16 years old (p<0.005). There was also a strong tendency for abuse (or lack of abuse) to carry over from one stage to another until age 13 (p<0.005 in all cases). Altogether, this model accounted for 52%, 32%, and 37% of the variance in the covaried measures of hippocampal volume, corpus callosum area, and frontal cortex gray matter volume, respectively. A simpler model that included only paths with p≤0.05 in all cases from the multiple regression analysis provided a robust fit (χ
2 =22.13, df=20, p>0.30) with a reasonable degree of parsimony (Tucker-Lewis=0.94 and Comparative Fit Index=0.96).
These statistical approaches indicated that hippocampus, corpus callosum, and frontal cortex were maximally affected by abuse at ages 3–5, 9–10, and 14–16 years, respectively.
Table 3 provides volume or area measures in subjects with childhood sexual abuse during these stages versus comparison subjects and subjects with childhood sexual abuse during all other stages.
DISCUSSION
Episodes of repeated childhood sexual abuse were associated with alterations in regional brain size during specific stages. Both analytic techniques lead to the same conclusions. Hippocampal volume was most strongly related first to abuse reportedly occurring between ages 3 and 5 years old, and second to abuse between 11 and 13 years old. In contrast, corpus callosum area was associated with abuse reportedly occurring during ages 9–10, and frontal cortex with abuse during ages 14–16. It is worth emphasizing that regional differences in sensitivity across age occurred in the same group of subjects.
The apparent vulnerability of the hippocampus to early stress is consistent with preclinical observations that exposure of the immature hippocampus to corticotropin-releasing hormone, a key limbic stress modulator, results in a delayed and progressive effect on cell survival and dendritic branching.
44 Further, there is a special population of cells in the immature hippocampus, but not in the adult hippocampus, that can release corticotropin-releasing hormone in response to stress,
45 potentially explaining the heightened sensitivity of the hippocampus to abuse during early childhood. Early effects of abuse on the hippocampus are consistent with evidence from humans and primates that the hippocampus matures rapidly and is functional very early in childhood.
46 This is also consistent with morphometric measures that show that the hippocampus has obtained about 85% of adult volume by 4 years of age.
47 In contrast, functional ontogeny of the prefrontal cortex may not emerge until puberty
31 or may go through continuous changes during childhood associated with pruning and strengthening of synaptic connections.
48 Volumetrically, the prefrontal cortex grows at a slow rate until about 8 years old and then has a rapid growth spurt between ages 8–14. The lack of apparent effect of exposure to early childhood sexual abuse on young adult frontal cortex volume is consistent with findings in rodents, which showed vulnerability of the hippocampus, but not prefrontal cortex, to early isolation stress.
30The observation that the corpus callosum was vulnerable to the effects of childhood sexual abuse between 9 and 10 years old is consistent with a diffusion tensor imaging study that showed substantial changes in fractional anisotropy and diffusivity occurring between 8 and 12 years old.
49Lack of a discernible sensitive period for the amygdala is consistent with several other studies that have failed to find an effect of childhood abuse on the volume of this region.
9,
21 –
23,
25 The amygdala is at full adult size in females at 4 years old
47 and may have an earlier sensitive period than we were able to assess.
This study is limited by the relatively small number of subjects who experienced abuse during each stage. However, finding distinctly different stages of vulnerability for these brain regions, within the same sample, provides support for the sensitive period hypothesis. It should be noted that the association between abuse and morphometric change is correlational and does not provide direct evidence of a cause-effect relationship.
Another limitation is that although we focused on exposure to childhood sexual abuse and excluded subjects with exposure to other forms of abuse, such as witnessing domestic violence, we did not specifically exclude subjects who may have experienced neglect or other types of emotional maltreatment such as exposure to parental verbal aggression.
10 This could lead to an error in interpretation if subjects exposed to childhood sexual abuse during one stage had a greater degree of exposure to neglect or emotional maltreatment than subjects exposed to childhood sexual abuse during a different stage. This was not the case. First, perpetrators of abuse were rarely parents or step-parents (three cases). Second, subjects were in upper-middle-class families and were all enrolled in college. This upbringing may have protected them from some of the complexities borne by others exposed to childhood sexual abuse, as none of the subjects in this carefully selected sample with childhood sexual abuse had any significant history of neglect. Third, only nine subjects were exposed to levels of parental verbal aggression that we have come to define as abusive (parental verbal aggression ≥40, on the verbal abuse scale
10 ). Ten percent of young adults without exposure to childhood sexual abuse or physical abuse report this degree of exposure to verbal abuse. There were no differences in average parental verbal aggression scores based on stage of exposure to childhood sexual abuse (F=0.15, df=1, 22, p>0.90). Hence, it seems unlikely that morphometric effects related to childhood sexual abuse during specific stages were an artifact of differential exposure to other forms of maltreatment.
Childhood abuse has been associated with vulnerability to a host of psychiatric disorders and behavioral problems. Based on the present findings there may be different abuse-related syndromes associated with particular ages of abuse and specific regional brain changes. For example, we found using regression analysis that current symptoms of depression on the Kellner Symptom Questionnaire
50 were specifically associated with abuse during stage 3–5 years old (r=0.459, p=0.001), but with no other stage. In contrast, PTSD-like symptoms as measured by the revised Mississippi Civilian PTSD scale
51 were only associated with abuse during stage 9–10 years old (r=0.349, p=0.02). It will be useful in future studies to ascertain if a sensitive period approach can more specifically delineate morphometric changes associated with psychopathology.
Identifying sensitive periods may also provide insight into key ages at which stimulation or environmental enrichment may optimally benefit the development of specific brain regions. Theoretically, periods of maximal sensitivity to early stress could occur during phases of rapid development, during times of high plasticity, or during times when systems are programmed to establish enduring set-points.
52Acknowledgments
Drs. Andersen and Tomada contributed equally as first authors. This research was presented at the New York Academy of Science, Mount Sinai Medical School, on September 11, 2005; at the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry in Toronto, Ontario, on October 23, 2005; at the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology in Washington, D.C., on November 9, 2005; at the Society of Biological Psychiatry on May 18, 2006; and at the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies on November 5, 2006.
This study was supported by RO1 awards from National Institute of Mental Health (MH-53636, MH-66222) and National Institute of Drug Abuse (DA-016934, DA-017846) to Dr. Teicher. We thank Ross J. Baldessarini, M.D., for constructive comments. Cynthia E. McGreenery, Danielle Webster, R.N., M.S., C.S., and Hanako Suzuki, M.A., provided assistance with subject recruitment, diagnostic interviews, and data management, respectively.