Site maintenance Wednesday, November 13th, 2024. Please note that access to some content and account information will be unavailable on this date.
Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 August 2010

Impact of Patient Language Proficiency and Interpreter Service Use on the Quality of Psychiatric Care: A Systematic Review

The expanding population of individuals with limited English proficiency in the United States presents a challenge to the health care system, given the national shortage of bilingual providers ( 1 ). Lack of English proficiency represents a serious barrier to communicating effectively in health care settings, providing high-quality care, and addressing health care disparities. Limited English proficiency is associated with poor access to medical care ( 2, 3, 4, 5, 6 ); lower-quality care, including more invasive management and excess hospitalizations, medical errors, and drug complications ( 7, 8, 9, 10, 11 ); and poor satisfaction with care ( 7, 12 ). Use of untrained interpreters may compound problems as a result of interpretation errors and the tendency among interpreters not to translate sensitive material ( 7 ). Individuals with limited proficiency may have worse access to psychiatric care ( 13 ), although findings are inconsistent ( 14, 15 ).
For a variety of reasons language proficiency may have a greater impact on the quality of psychiatric care than on the quality of medical care. Psychiatric evaluation hinges on obtaining a thorough history because many key symptoms are not associated with directly observable behaviors or signs of morbidity, and they can be elicited only via self-report. Laboratory and radiological testing are of limited utility, typically helpful only to rule out organic contributors. Language barriers may interfere with conducting a thorough mental status examination, which may mask disorders of speech and language (for example, aphasias and neologisms), thought process (for example, flight of ideas, disorganization, and tangentiality), thought content (for example, grandiosity, delusions, obsessions, and magical thinking), and perceptions (for example, hallucinations). Finally, language barriers may hinder the identification of important factors influencing the process of care, including stigma, shame, patients' explanatory models of illness, their acceptance of the diagnosis and treatment, and development of a therapeutic patient-provider alliance.
Even though there are numerous theoretical reasons to surmise that language proficiency may influence psychiatric care, little research has explored this phenomenon. This article reports results of a systematic literature review that was conducted to describe the effects of limited English proficiency and interpreter service use on the quality of care for psychiatric disorders.

Methods

PubMed, PsycINFO, and CINAHL (Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature) were searched to identify all articles in English published from inception of each database to April 7, 2009, that examined the effects of language proficiency or interpreter use on psychiatric care. A total of 321 articles were identified as a result of the searches, reviews of reference lists of included articles, and consultation with expert sources. Of these, 26 met inclusion criteria: peer-reviewed articles that reported primary data on the assessment or treatment of psychiatric disorders among patients with limited proficiency in English or in the providers' language. [The systematic search methods are described in more detail in an online supplement to this article at ps.psychiatryonline.org .]

Results

Results from 12 reports of cases ( 16, 17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27 ) are summarized in a second online supplement at ps.psychiatryonline.org .
Results from 14 empirical studies are organized according to the effects of language proficiency and interpreter use on three outcomes: psychiatric assessment and diagnosis (most studies), treatment, and patient-provider interaction. Table 1 shows the studies chronologically. When possible, results are presented separately for patients seen with or without interpreters or by bilingual providers; however, some studies provided insufficient information, and not all of these categories are represented in the literature. Ad hoc interpreters included anyone facilitating translation who was not trained in medical interpreting, such as bilingual hospital staff; friends or family, including minors; and other patients.
Table 1 Summary of empirical studies on the effects of language proficiency and interpreter use on psychiatric assessment and diagnosis, treatment, and patient-provider interaction

Psychiatric assessment and diagnosis

Four small-scale studies demonstrated that psychiatric assessment may be compromised among patients who are seen without interpreters ( 28, 29, 30, 31 ). In South Africa, clinicians interviewing Xhosa-speaking inpatients without interpreters tended to use closed-ended questions and elicit brief replies, subsequently concluding that patients lacked intellectual capacity or that they had impoverished thoughts ( 31 ).
Studies of standardized evaluations of Spanish-speaking inpatients with schizophrenia that were conducted both in English (without interpreters) and in Spanish reported contradictory findings in overall scores on the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) ( 28, 29, 30 ). Marcos and colleagues ( 28 ) found higher BPRS scores among patients assessed in English than among those assessed in Spanish, whereas Price and Cuellar ( 30 ) reported lower scores among those assessed in English. Several factors may have contributed to this discrepancy. Patients' responses to some questions differed in each language—for example, they endorsed symptoms in English but denied them in Spanish ( 29 ). Patients spoke in the present tense more often in English than in Spanish, suggesting current rather than past symptoms ( 29 ). In the study by Marcos and colleagues ( 28, 29 ), psychiatrists viewed recordings of evaluations conducted without an interpreter; patients were Spanish speaking but conversant in English. Evaluations conducted in each language were evaluated by psychiatrists who spoke the same language (English or Spanish). Thus the effects of rater and patient language were confounded. Higher scores were given to patients assessed in English (by English-speaking psychiatrists) than those assessed in Spanish (by Spanish-speaking psychiatrists) on the subscales anxiety, tension, mannerisms and posturing, somatic concern, emotional withdrawal, depressive mood, and hostility. Scores on these subscales may have been influenced by characteristics of speech, such as fluency, rate, and productivity ( 28, 29 ). In Price and Cuellar's study ( 30 ), patients were rated in both languages by bilingual raters who may have attributed speech disturbances in English to challenges of communicating in a second language rather than to psychopathology. However, because detailed BPRS score profiles were not reported, it is not possible to compare the total scores between the two studies or to determine which subscales were elevated.
In Germany, Turkish and German psychotic patients were interviewed with the Schedules for the Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN) by a monolingual German-speaking psychiatric trainee without an interpreter and by a bilingual psychiatric trainee ( 32 ). The trainees disagreed on the diagnosis for 4% of German patients and 19% of Turkish patients. The relative percentages of disagreement were similar for Turkish patients with "good" and "bad" German proficiency; however, the method of assessing proficiency was not described. The authors speculated that diagnostic uncertainty may be more closely linked to patients' level of acculturation than to their language proficiency. Alternately, psychometric properties of the Turkish and German versions of the SCAN may differ, accounting for some observed differences.
Taken together, these studies suggest that psychiatric assessments conducted in a nonnative language may be less reliable, although the effect on overall impressions of psychopathology may vary according to symptom type ( 28, 30, 32 ).
Seven of the 14 empirical studies addressed the effects of interpreters on psychiatric assessment and diagnosis. In two studies, researchers audiotaped evaluations of patients in which ad hoc interpreters were used ( 33, 34 ). Errors in interpretation resulted from interpreters' inadequate language proficiency; their lack of psychiatric knowledge, which led to normalization of patients' disordered thought process; interjection of their attitudes or editorializing comments; and their providing answers for patients without first interpreting the question for them ( 33, 34 ). [Table A2 in the second online supplement to this article, available at ps.psychiatryonline.org, categorizes types of errors with illustrative examples.] Interpreters with less English proficiency made more errors ( 34 ). A hospital orderly with no interpreter training but some experience made more addition errors (that is, interjecting phrases), whereas a less experienced orderly made more distortions of clinicians' questions ( 34 ). Interpretation errors were twice as likely to occur when patients spoke than when physicians spoke and were more likely with acutely psychotic patients than with nonpsychotic patients ( 34 ). Similarly, when patients provided lengthy or convoluted replies, omissions were especially likely. Interpreters may have difficulty registering and remembering a patient's statement if they cannot discern its meaning ( 34 ). Similarly, in South Africa, both ad hoc interpreters (for example, bilingual nurses) and professional interpreters failed to translate psychotic patients' actual statements ( 31 ). Bilingual nurses were less likely than professional interpreters to report that they could not follow the patient; instead they asserted their opinion that the patient was psychotic. Clinicians, however, preferred working with these nurses over professional interpreters who withheld judgments ( 31 ).
In England evaluations of patients with good or limited English proficiency were audiotaped and transcribed; the patients underwent two evaluations—one by an English-speaking psychiatrist with a professional interpreter and one by a bilingual psychiatrist ( 35 ). The professional interpreter made errors, including omissions, condensations, conceptual substitutions, and miscommunications that were attributed to the interpreter's inadequate language skills. Nevertheless, both psychiatrists generated similar overall checklist ratings of patients' mental state. Methodological limitations may have obscured potential differences. Specifically, the interpreter knew that the study aimed to compare the two types of evaluations, and the same psychiatrist assessed patients and transcribed encounters.
Two studies reviewed medical records from initial health screenings of refugees seeking asylum in Switzerland ( 36, 37 ). Use of professional interpreters was associated with increased disclosure of traumatic events and psychological symptoms, compared with use of ad hoc interpreters or no interpreter ( 36 ). Patients reported few physical or psychological symptoms in encounters without an interpreter or when providers reported that communication was poor, and patients reported more symptoms during encounters with professional interpreters ( 37 ). However, the presence of an ad hoc interpreter, typically a family member or friend, was associated with reports of many physical symptoms but few psychological symptoms ( 37 ). Thus disclosure of psychological symptoms may be more sensitive to language barriers than disclosure of physical symptoms.
In Saudi Arabia a review of charts of primary care patients found that Arabic-speaking physicians and non-Arabic-speaking physicians using interpreters (professional or ad hoc) diagnosed mental disorders at similar rates ( 38 ). Although this finding suggests that some physicians made psychiatric diagnoses after evaluating patients whose language they did not speak (patients' primary language was not reported), this study provided little insight into either the accuracy of physicians' assessments or physicians' ability to differentiate among psychiatric disorders, both critical prerequisites for high-quality care.
Results from these empirical studies indicate that interpreter-mediated encounters are prone to errors, although the clinical significance of errors varied among studies. Nevertheless, interpreters (especially professional ones) appear to facilitate more complete disclosure, which is a crucial component in ensuring accurate psychiatric assessments.

Treatment

A review of administrative data found that among Latino patients with schizophrenia, limited English proficiency was associated with greater adherence to antipsychotic medication, lower rates of excess prescription filling, and fewer hospitalizations ( 39 ). In contrast, the same study found that among Asian Americans, limited English proficiency was associated with lower adherence, greater nonadherence, and excess prescription filling. The authors speculated that cultural differences in family support structures and beliefs about mental illness and treatment may have accounted for these findings.
In Switzerland asylum seekers were more likely to be referred for psychiatric care when providers were bilingual or when professional interpreters were used than when ad hoc or no interpreters were used ( 36, 37 ). However, the greater likelihood of psychiatric referral did not remain significant when the analysis controlled for effects of demographic variables, symptom severity, and disclosure of prior trauma. It is important to note, however, that professional interpreter use was associated with greater disclosure of psychological symptoms and trauma, suggesting that improved disclosure may have mediated the association between professional interpreter use and referral, although this was not tested. Finally, rates of referral for psychiatric care but not for medical care were higher with adequate patient-provider language concordance than with no concordance.

Patient-provider interaction

A heterogeneous group of studies, reviewed under the theme of patient-provider interaction, addressed how language proficiency or interpreter use affects the process of psychiatric care from the perspective of patients or providers.
Two studies that used video-recordings of Spanish-speaking patients found less verbal production by psychotic patients with limited English proficiency during English-language evaluations than during evaluations conducted in Spanish ( 29, 30 ). During evaluations conducted in English, patients made significantly more short replies to questions that were identical in the Spanish interview, demonstrated slower speech and more pauses and disturbances (such as incomplete sentences, repetition, stuttering, and incoherent sounds) ( 29 ), and scored lower on ratings of self-disclosure ( 30 ).
Another study examined clinicians' impressions of their ability to create case formulations for Latino outpatients in one of two situations: in-person encounters with patients during which an ad hoc interpreter assisted or watching videotaped interviews with the help of an ad hoc interpreter (the interviews were conducted in Spanish by other clinicians) ( 40 ). In both situations, clinicians reported a high degree of confidence in their assessments and believed that the interpretations provided to them by the interpreters were accurate and free from bias. Most clinicians reported that use of an interpreter led to assessments of diagnosis and functioning that were of equal or greater severity than assessments they would have made without an interpreter. However, patients' language proficiency was not assessed, and the assessments of English-speaking and Spanish-speaking clinicians were not compared, limiting the ability to determine the accuracy of clinicians' assessments. Similarly, there was no direct assessment of the interpretations to determine whether errors or bias may have been undetected by clinicians.
A study of nurse evaluations of asylum seekers in Switzerland found that only professional interpreters had a beneficial impact on communication ( 36 ). Nurses rated communication as "poor" or "fair" in 84% of evaluations without an interpreter, 72% of those with ad hoc interpreters, and only 6% of those with professional interpreters.
Patients and psychiatrists may have differing views of evaluations conducted with use of interpreters ( 41 ). Compared with Spanish-speaking patients seen without an interpreter, significantly more patients seen with an interpreter (type not specified) reported that they gained self-understanding and found the visit helpful. In contrast, the psychiatric residents who conducted the evaluations were unanimous in feeling that they provided less help to patients whom they saw with interpreters than to those whom they saw without interpreters. Most patients who were seen with an interpreter wanted a return visit; however, a minority of the residents believed that these patients wanted to return, and only one reported feeling comfortable seeing a patient with an interpreter for ongoing care. The authors postulated that residents projected their discomfort with treating patients who had limited English proficiency onto the patients, which prevented the residents from acknowledging patients' feelings of being helped and their desire for continued care. In contrast to the positive experiences of patients in this study, some patients in South Africa with limited English proficiency refused to utilize interpreters for various reasons: they were insulted that their English skills were perceived as inadequate, they wanted to avoid interference by third parties, or they had overt paranoia regarding the interpreter ( 31 ).
Overall, psychiatric care of patients with limited English proficiency that is provided without interpreters may lead to incomplete patient disclosure and thus limit the effectiveness of evaluation and treatment. Results are mixed on how ad hoc interpreters may affect patient-provider interaction, with some studies suggesting negative effects and others indicating a benefit. Research addressing how professional interpreters influence patient-provider interaction is lacking; however, one study did report improved patient-provider communication. Patients and providers may hold divergent views of the benefits of interpreter-mediated visits.

Summary of findings

Patients provided longer replies with greater disclosure when interviewed in their first language compared with a nonnative language ( 29, 30 ). When evaluating patients in English without interpreters, clinicians may alter their interview style to discourage lengthy replies, thus biasing assessments ( 31 ). Clinicians may understand short replies to signify a hostile or guarded mental state, intellectual impairment, impoverished thoughts, withdrawal, or tension. In one study Spanish-speaking psychotic patients who were conversant in English were rated similarly on positive symptoms when evaluations were conducted in English or in Spanish, but in the English evaluations they received higher ratings (indicating more severe symptoms) on domains most likely to be influenced by communicating in a nonprimary language ( 28 ). However, another study found that clinicians documented less overall psychopathology among bilingual patients during English-language evaluations than during Spanish-language evaluations ( 30 ). Therefore, English-language evaluation of patients with limited English proficiency may obscure mental status findings among patients with schizophrenia ( 28, 30 ), and the effects of language proficiency may vary according to symptom type ( 28 ). For example, it is not known whether language proficiency affects the communication of symptoms of depression or anxiety in ways that differ from the communication of psychotic symptoms. Finally, studies using structured interviews ( 28, 29, 30, 32, 35, 36, 37 ) do not represent clinical practice, which suggests the need for research in naturalistic settings.
Use of ad hoc interpreters may impede disclosure of sensitive material ( 33, 37 ) and contribute to distortions and errors ( 33, 34 ). Errors have been shown to occur more often for acutely ill patients ( 34 ) and may lead to over- or underestimation of psychopathology ( 33 ). Nevertheless, some clinicians are confident in assessments conducted with such interpreters ( 40 ). Compromised disclosure with ad hoc interpreters may yield fewer referrals for follow-up care ( 36, 37 ), which may have an impact on treatment and health outcomes. Professional interpreters, however, may facilitate improved disclosure ( 36, 37 ). Although errors also occur in encounters mediated by professional interpreters, their clinical impact may be less substantial ( 35 ). Standards for mental health interpreting do not exist, and both ad hoc and professional interpreters may lack sufficient language skills to facilitate the communication needed for high-quality psychiatric care ( 34, 35 ). More systematic study of the clinical consequences is warranted.

Discussion

There has been little systematic inquiry into the impact of language proficiency or interpreter service use on the quality of psychiatric care. A few studies have directly examined communication during interpreter-mediated encounters ( 31, 33, 34, 35 ); however, none of these studies addressed contemporary psychiatric practice in the United States.
Past studies provide the background for future systematic study of the potential impact of language barriers on quality of care, which is needed to develop an evidence base to inform mental health services delivery and policy. Evidence suggests that persons from ethnic minority groups have fewer psychiatric disorders and that mental health disparities are related to receipt of lower-quality care ( 42 ). Thus understanding how language barriers hinder care represents an important direction in elucidating and eliminating disparities.
There has been insufficient examination of how language barriers influence care for patients with various presentations and diagnoses. Interpretation errors occur during psychiatric assessments, but their clinical significance is poorly understood. Patient, provider, and interpreter characteristics that influence the likelihood of errors are not known, nor is it known how best to minimize errors and their impact. Moreover, authors disagree on whether interpreters should provide literal translations only and avoid attempts to clarify the speaker's intent ( 43 ) or play an expanded role as cultural brokers ( 40 ). Similarly, it is not known whether specific methods of data gathering enhance the likelihood that providers will elicit an accurate history.
This review has focused on analysis at the level of patient-provider encounters, which has been described as the gold standard for assessment of health care quality ( 44 ). However, analysis at broader levels of the health care system, such as care teams, organizations, and the external environment—including health policy, regulation, financing, and accreditation—will also inform understanding of the effects of language proficiency and interpreter use on psychiatric care ( 44, 45 ). For example, recent research has examined methods to assess the need for interpreters ( 46 ), although more study along these lines and in psychiatric settings is indicated. How policies for language services, including interpreting standards or laws governing reimbursement, affect the provision and quality of mental health services warrants systematic evaluation.

Conclusions

A systematic review of the literature on the impact of language barriers on the quality of psychiatric care revealed multiple potential sources of miscommunication and distortion resulting from gaps in communication, particularly when no interpreters or ad hoc interpreters are used. There is insufficient evidence to determine whether quality of care is compromised and under what circumstances high-quality psychiatric care can prevail in the presence of language barriers. Complementing the robust literature on medical interpreting, a small body of literature suggests that use of professional interpreters during psychiatric encounters facilitates disclosure of sensitive material and leads to greater patient satisfaction and self-understanding, thereby reinforcing the cornerstones of high-quality psychiatric care.

Acknowledgments and disclosures

Dr. Bauer is supported by a National Research Service Award (2T32-MH019733; principal investigator, Richard Frank, M.D.). This study was supported by grant 1P50-MHO-73469 from the National Institute of Mental Health and grant P60-MD0-02261 from the National Center for Minority Health and Health Disparities.
The authors report no competing interests.

Footnote

Dr. Bauer is affiliated with the Department of Psychiatry, Cambridge Health Alliance, 1493 Cambridge St., Cambridge, MA 02139 (e-mail: [email protected]). Dr. Alegría is with the Center for Multicultural Mental Health Research, Cambridge Health Alliance, Somerville, Massachusetts. The findings of this study were presented as a poster at the annual meeting of the Academy of Psychosomatic Medicine, Las Vegas, November 11–14, 2009.

Supplementary Material

File (pss_61_08_765_01.pdf)
File (pss_61_08_765_02.pdf)

References

1.
Vega WA, Karno M, Alegría M, et al: Research issues for improving treatment of US Hispanics with persistent mental disorders. Psychiatric Services 58:385–394, 2007
2.
Weinick RM, Krauss NA: Racial/ethnic differences in children's access to care. American Journal of Public Health 90:1771–1774, 2000
3.
Pippins JR, Alegría M, Haas JS: Association between language proficiency and the quality of primary care among a national sample of insured Latinos. Medical Care 45:1020–1025, 2007
4.
DuBard CA, Gizlice Z: Language spoken and differences in health status, access to care, and receipt of preventive services among US Hispanics. American Journal of Public Health 98:2021–2028, 2008
5.
Brach C, Chevarley FM: Demographics and Health Care Access and Utilization of Limited-English-Proficient and English-Proficient Hispanics. Research Findings No 28. Rockville, Md, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, 2008
6.
Ku L, Flores G: Pay now or pay later: providing interpreter services in health care. Health Affairs 24:435–444, 2005
7.
Flores G: The impact of medical interpreter services on the quality of health care: a systematic review. Medical Care Research and Review 62:255–299, 2005
8.
Bard MR, Goettler CE, Schenarts PJ, et al: Language barrier leads to the unnecessary intubation of trauma patients. American Surgeon 70:783–786, 2004
9.
Waxman MA, Levitt MA: Are diagnostic testing and admission rates higher in non-English-speaking versus English-speaking patients in the emergency department? Annals of Emergency Medicine 36:456–461, 2000
10.
Hampers LC, Cha S, Gutglass DJ, et al: Language barriers and resource utilization in a pediatric emergency department. Pediatrics 103:1253–1256, 1999
11.
Gandhi TK, Burstin HR, Cook EF, et al: Drug complications in outpatients. Journal of General Internal Medicine 15:149–154, 2000
12.
Karliner L, Jacobs E, Chen A, et al: Do professional interpreters improve clinical care for patients with limited English proficiency? A systematic review of the literature. Health Services Research 42:727–754, 2007
13.
Sentell T, Shumway M, Snowden L: Access to mental health treatment by English language proficiency and race/ethnicity. Journal of General Internal Medicine 22:289–293, 2007
14.
Alegría M, Mulvaney-Day N, Woo M, et al: Correlates of past-year mental health service use among Latinos: results from the National Latino and Asian American Study. American Journal of Public Health 97:76–83, 2006
15.
Abe-Kim J, Takeuchi D, Hong S, et al: Use of mental health-related services among immigrant and US-born Asian Americans: results from the National Latino and Asian American Study. American Journal of Public Health 97:91–98, 2006
16.
Buxbaum E: The role of a second language in the formation of ego and superego. Psychoanalytic Quarterly 18:279–289, 1949
17.
Greenson RR: The mother tongue and the mother. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis 31:18–23, 1950
18.
Krapf EE: The choice of language in polyglot psychoanalysis. Psychoanalytic Quarterly 24:343–357, 1955
19.
Del Castillo JC: The influence of language upon symptomatology in foreign-born patients. American Journal of Psychiatry 127:242–244, 1970
20.
Sabin JE: Translating despair. American Journal of Psychiatry 132:197–199, 1975
21.
Pitta P, Marcos LR, Alpert M: Language switching as a treatment strategy with bilingual patients. American Journal of Psychoanalysis 38:255–258, 1978
22.
Javier RA: Linguistic considerations in the treatment of bilinguals. Psychoanalytic Psychology 6:87–96, 1989
23.
Vasquez C, Javier RA: The problem with interpreters: communicating with Spanish-speaking patients. Hospital and Community Psychiatry 42:163–165, 1991
24.
Baxter H, Cheng LY: Use of interpreters in individual psychotherapy. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 30:153–156, 1996
25.
Oquendo MA: Psychiatric evaluation and psychotherapy in the patient's second language. Psychiatric Services 47:614–618, 1996
26.
Rueda-Lara MA, Buchert S, Skotzko C, et al: Psychiatric symptoms masking pituitary adenoma in Spanish speaking immigrants. General Hospital Psychiatry 25:367–371, 2003
27.
Björn G: Ethics and interpreting in psychotherapy with refugee children and families. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry 59:516–521, 2005
28.
Marcos LR, Alpert M, Urcuyo L, et al: The effect of interview language on the evaluation of psychopathology in Spanish-American schizophrenic patients. American Journal of Psychiatry 130:549–553, 1973
29.
Marcos LR, Uruyo L, Kesselman M, et al: The language barrier in evaluating Spanish-American patients. Archives of General Psychiatry 29:655–659, 1973
30.
Price CS, Cuellar I: Effects of language and related variables on the expression of psychopathology in Mexican American psychiatric patients. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 3:145–160, 1981
31.
Drennan G, Swartz L: The paradoxical use of interpreting in psychiatry. Social Science and Medicine 54:1853–1866, 2002
32.
Haasen C, Yagdiran O, Mass R, et al: Potential for misdiagnosis among Turkish migrants with psychotic disorders: a clinical controlled study in Germany. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 101:125–129, 2000
33.
Marcos LR: Effects of interpreters on the evaluation of psychopathology in non-English-speaking patients. American Journal of Psychiatry 136:171–174, 1979
34.
Price J: Foreign language interpreting in psychiatric practice. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 9:263–267, 1975
35.
Farooq S, Fear C, Oyebode F: An investigation of the adequacy of psychiatric interviews conducted through an interpreter. Psychiatric Bulletin 21:209–213, 1997
36.
Eytan A, Bischoff A, Rrustemi I, et al: Screening of mental disorders in asylum-seekers from Kosovo. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 36:499–503, 2002
37.
Bischoff A, Bovier PA, Rrustemi I, et al: Language barriers between nurses and asylum seekers: their impact on symptom reporting and referral. Social Science and Medicine 57:503–512, 2003
38.
Dodd W: Do interpreters affect consultations? Family Practice 1:42–47, 1984
39.
Gilmer TP, Ojeda VD, Barrio C, et al: Adherence to antipsychotics among Latinos and Asians with schizophrenia and limited English proficiency. Psychiatric Services 60:175–182, 2009
40.
Zayas L, Cabassa L, Perez MC, et al: Using interpreters in diagnostic research and practice: pilot results and recommendations. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 68:924–928, 2007
41.
Kline F, Acosta FX, Austin W, et al: The misunderstood Spanish-speaking patient. American Journal of Psychiatry 137:1530–1533, 1980
42.
Miranda J, McGuire TG, Williams DR, et al: Mental health in the context of health disparities. American Journal of Psychiatry 165:1102–1108, 2008
43.
Bradford DT, Munoz A: Translation in bilingual psychotherapy. Professional Psychology 24:52–61, 1993
44.
Berwick DM: A user's manual for the IOM's "Quality Chasm" report. Health Affairs 21(3):80–90, 2002
45.
Institute of Medicine Committee on Crossing the Quality Chasm: Adaptation to Mental Health and Addictive Disorders: Improving the Quality of Health Care for Mental and Substance-Use Conditions. Washington DC, National Academies Press, 2006
46.
Karliner L, Napoles-Springer A, Schillinger D, et al: Identification of limited English proficient patients in clinical care. Journal of General Internal Medicine 23:1555–1560, 2008

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to Psychiatric Services
Go to Psychiatric Services
Psychiatric Services
Pages: 765 - 773
PubMed: 20675834

History

Published online: 1 August 2010
Published in print: August, 2010

Authors

Details

Amy M. Bauer, M.D., M.S.
Margarita Alegría, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

PPV Articles - Psychiatric Services

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share