Skip to main content
Full access
Editorial
Published Online: 1 March 1998

Decline of Dopamine: Effects of Age and Acute Neuroleptic Challenge

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry
It has been almost 30 years since the methods of nuclear medicine were first applied to the study of psychiatric disorders (1). Early studies examined regional cerebral blood flow and, subsequently, glucose metabolism as measures of neuronal activity (25). Soon thereafter, ligands that directly measure the neurobiologic substrates of neurotransmitter function were developed and were used widely in psychiatric neuroimaging (6, 7). The capacity to study neurometabolic and neurochemical activity in vivo relatively noninvasively in patients—and at different stages of their illness and lives—promised to revolutionize psychiatric research and reveal the nature of brain function and disease pathophysiology.
Positron emission tomography (PET) scanning is regarded as the Rolls Royce of neuroimaging. Interest in functional neuroimaging with PET has burgeoned over the years as the relevant technology and methodology with which it is used have become more sophisticated. The reports of Volkow et al. and Bartlett et al. in this issue may be considered in this context.
The study by Volkow and colleagues, in its simple elegance, demonstrates how the power of PET, when applied to a basic question, can yield a potentially profound result. Using PET and the competitive dopamine receptor antagonist [11C]~raclopride as the ligand for quantitative neuroreceptor (D2) imaging, these investigators found decreases in D2 receptor densities that correlated with increasing age in a group of healthy volunteers. While this finding has been previously reported (8, 9), Volkow et al. also used standard neuropsychological tests. Remarkably, they found a strong correlation between D2 receptor density and selective measures of motor and cognitive function, a correlation that could not be explained on the basis of age effects. These included motor function measured by the Finger Tapping Test, abstract thinking and mental flexibility measured by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and attention and response inhibition measured by the Stroop Color-Word Test interference score.
At first glance, it might appear that there is nothing new or noteworthy in these findings. However, this linear association between the measures of neuropsychological function and D2 receptors is in fact striking, because it was demonstrated in healthy volunteers, not symptomatic patients with neurologic disorders. These results run counter to the dogma derived largely from the literature on Parkinson's disease, in which it has been suggested that an 80% or greater loss of nigrostriatal dopamine function had to occur before clinical manifestations of functional impairment would be evident (10). It has been generally assumed that with lesser deficits, the redundancy in striatal dopamine innervation, and/or various compensatory mechanisms, would obviate the appearance of clinical signs or symptoms. Thus, the report by Volkow et al. of a linear association between decline in aspects of neuropsychological function and decrements in D2 receptors in healthy volunteers is noteworthy. Their data indicate that the resolution of PET-quantified D2 binding is sensitive enough to detect functional decrements that are subclinical and asymptomatic and also is sufficient to detect the actual linear relationship between dopamine neurotransmission (as reflected by D2 density) and specific motor and cognitive functions as well as age.
Historically, the focus of studies of dopamine and behavior has been on the D2 receptor, yet recent work has emphasized the role of D1 receptors in mediating the deficits of aging and disease (11, 12). Work by Goldman-Rakic and co-workers (e.g., reference 13) has demonstrated the role of D1 receptors in frontal lobe cognitive functions involving working memory. In addition, it has been clearly demonstrated that D1 agonists can reverse the deficits in motor and neurocognitive function associated with Parkinson's disease and MPTP-induced damage (1417). Such data not only highlight the important role for D1 receptors in normal function and nonpathologic aging, but raise interesting questions about possible changes in D1 receptors that might be revealed in the Volkow et al. paradigm.
The findings of the study by Volkow et al. also illustrate the functional consequences of brain dopamine activity. The Finger Tapping Test is the only measure for which the anatomic basis is predominantly attributable to the corpus striatum of the basal ganglia. The card sorting and Stroop tests are believed to be subserved by cognitive functions that are primarily based in the cerebral cortex. This has two important implications. First, it reaffirms the often reported but less frequently acknowledged fact that the corpus striatum, by virtue of its topographically organized and distributed projections, modulates a wide range of cognitive and motor functions (18). Second, these results demonstrate the distributed anatomic network in which dopamine plays an important functional role (19, 20).
The report of the late Elsa Bartlett and her colleagues is less easy to interpret. Its results are somewhat counterintuitive and not wholly consistent with previously reported studies, though nonetheless interesting. The New York University-Brookhaven PET group had previously shown, as had others, that chronic treatment with antipsychotic drugs decreased glucose metabolic activity in extrastriatal (predominantly cerebral cortical) regions while increasing it in the striatum (2126). Moreover, it had also been shown that neither D2 receptor densities nor affinities differed between patients who were responsive and those who were not responsive to antipsychotic drug treatment (2729), although densities did correlate with plasma drug levels (27, 30). Since the onset of D2 receptor blockade is rapid (within hours of drug administration) yet therapeutic effects take weeks to occur, Bartlett et al. (21) hypothesized that the critical mechanisms mediating antipsychotic drug effects must be downstream from the receptor and might be reflected by neurometabolic activity. They tested this by measuring glucose metabolic activity not as basal levels under resting conditions or after repeated antipsychotic drug treatment but with a dynamic test 12 hours after parenteral administration of haloperidol (5 mg i.m.). They found that following drug challenge glucose metabolic activity was decreased in all brain regions including the striatum. Moreover, the decrements were greatest in the healthy volunteers and patients classified as treatment nonresponders. It is interesting that the responders to treatment showed only minimal change.
These results are puzzling for two reasons. First, the effects of haloperidol on glucose metabolic activity were widespread (essentially in all brain regions measured) and greatly exceeded the known distribution of D2 receptors. Presumably, this reflects the transduction of D2-mediated effects across the distributed striatal-thalamic-cortical circuits through which striatal neurons project (18). Second, the direction of the changes in glucose metabolic activity is unusual—haloperidol produced decreased activity, including in the striatum, except in treatment-responsive schizophrenic patients. How are we to understand this? Some prior studies indicate that repeated antipsychotic drug treatment increases striatal metabolic activity and decreases cerebral cortical metabolic activity, while others have found increases in cerebral perfusion with drug treatment (2225, 31). It is notable that brain metabolic studies using 2-deoxyglucose in rodents have shown that acute antipsychotic drug administration generally depresses metabolic activity (32, 33). Although it is possible that acute single-dose administration produces a different pattern of effects on brain glucose utilization than repeated dosing (34), these disparate findings are not readily reconcilable.
The fact that the greatest changes were seen in the nonresponding patients and normal subjects is also puzzling. Buchsbaum et al. (22, 23) previously reported that low basal levels and greater increases in glucose metabolic activity after repeated antipsychotic drug treatment were associated with better response. Critical data missing from the Bartlett et al. study are the baseline levels of the healthy subjects. Without these values, it is difficult to conclude whether the greater decreases in glucose activity that occurred in the nonresponders were to levels commensurate with those of the normal subjects (i.e., the treatment normalized their aberrant levels). Wolkin et al. (35) reported blunted prefrontal decreases and relative striatal increases in glucose metabolism in response to chronic haloperidol administration in patients with prominent negative symptoms. Poor response to antipsychotic treatment is characteristic of this subgroup of patients (3638). The Brookhaven group also previously reported that amphetamine challenge decreased cerebral cortical glucose metabolism but that this effect was also blunted in the patients with negative symptoms (39). These results from the same group suggest that the smallest change would be in the patients who were not responsive to treatment—that the treatment-responsive patients would exhibit the greater plasticity and capacity for antipsychotic response (40).
In general, selective or nonselective dopamine agonists produce decreases in cortical glucose metabolism (33, 4143), although Volkow et al. (44) subsequently found, using methylphenidate, that brain metabolic changes varied by region, possibly because of differences in D2 receptor availability and/or function. Preclinical studies have shown that this effect is reversed in animals with pharmacological or surgical lesions of the striatum or prefrontal cortex (45). A recent study by Weinberger's group (manuscript by R. Saunders et al. submitted for publication) may help to bring these apparently inconsistent findings into a cogent context. They examined the dopamine response in the caudate nuclei, following pharmacological stimulation with amphetamine of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, in adult rhesus monkeys that had had either neonatal or adult lesions of the medial temporal lobe and in normal animals. The normal animals, as well as those with lesions of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex that were made when they were adults, showed a reduction in dopamine overflow. In contrast, the monkeys with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex lesions made during neonatal periods became hyperdopaminergic (i.e., had decreased dopamine overflow). These data illustrate that early injury to the primate medial temporal lobe predisposes an adult brain to respond to prefrontal cortical stimulation with abnormal striatal dopamine release. Consequently, it could be that pathologies of different types or timing in the course of development result in distinct patterns of neural organization in the striatal-thalamic-cortical architecture that produce varying responses to dopaminergic stimulation.
There is a sense of satisfaction in having an article definitively settle a question in such a way that further research would be only confirmational. However, these two very interesting reports raise as many issues as they settle, and it is clear that further research on these important questions is necessary.

Footnote

Address reprint requests to Dr. Lieberman at the Department of Psychiatry, 7025 Neuro~sciences Hospital, University of North Carolina School of Medicine, Chapel Hill, NC 27599-7160. Supported in part by NIMH grants MH-00537 and MH-33127 to Dr. Lieberman and MH-40537 to Dr. Mailman and by the Mental Health and Neuroscience Clinical Research Center, University of North Carolina School of Medicine.

References

1.
Ingvar D, Franzen G: Abnormalities of cerebral blood flow distribution in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1974; 15:425–462
2.
Huang SC, Phelps ME, Hoffman EJ, Sideris K, Selin CJ, Kuhi DE: Non-invasive determination of local cerebral metabolic rate for glucose in man. Am J Physiol 1980; 238:E69–E82
3.
Frackowiak RSJ, Lenzi CL, Jones T, Heather JD: Quantitative measurement of regional cerebral blood flow and oxygen metabolism in man using 15 O and positron emission tomography. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1980; 4:727–736
4.
Buchsbaum M, Ingvar D, Kessler R: Cerebral glucography with positron tomography. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39:251–259
5.
Gur R, Skolnick B, Gur R: Brain function in psychiatric disorders, 1: regional cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:1250–1254
6.
Sedvall G, Blomquist G, DePaulis T: PET Studies on Brain Energy Metabolism and Dopamine Receptors in Schizophrenic Patients and Monkeys. New York, Plenum Press, 1985
7.
Andreasen NC: Brain imaging: applications in psychiatry. Science 1988; 239:1381–1388
8.
Kemper T: Neuroanatomical and neuropathological changes in normal aging and dementia, in Clinical Neurology of Aging. Edited by Albert M. New York, Oxford University Press, 1984, pp 9–52
9.
Joseph JA, Roth GS, Strong R: The striatum, a microcosm for the examination of age-related alterations in the CNS: a selected review. Rev Biol Res Aging 1990; 4:181–199
10.
Bernheimer H, Birmayer W, Hornykiewicz O, Jellinger K, Seitelberger F: Brain dopamine and the syndromes of Parkinson and Huntington: clinical, morphological and neurochemical correlations. J Neurol Sci 1973; 20:415–455
11.
Self DW, Barnhart WJ, Lehman DA, Nestler EJ: Opposite modulation of cocaine-seeking behavior by D1- and D2-like dopamine receptor agonists. Science 1996; 271:1586–1589
12.
Okubo Y, Suhara T, Suzuki K, Kobayashi K, Inoue O, Terasaki O, Someya Y, Sassa T, Sudo Y, Matsushima E, Iyo M, Tateno Y, Toru M: Decreased prefrontal dopamine D1 receptors in schizophrenia revealed by PET. Nature 1997; 385:634–636
13.
Williams GV, Goldman-Rakic PS: Modulation of memory fields by dopamine D1 receptors in prefrontal cortex. Nature 1995; 376:572–575
14.
Taylor JR, Lawrence MS, Redmond DE Jr, Elsworth JD, Roth RH, Nichols DE, Mailman RB: Dihydrexidine, a full dopamine D1 agonist, reduces MPTP-induced parkinsonism in African green monkeys. Eur J Pharmacol 1991; 199:387–388
15.
Shiosaki K, Jenner P, Asin KE, Britton DR, Lin CW, Michaelides M, Smith L, Bianchi B, Didomenico S, Hodges L, Hong Y, Mahan L, Mikusa J, Miller T, Nikkel A, Stashko M, Witte D, Williams M: ABT-431: the diacetyl prodrug of A-86929, a potent and selective dopamine D1 receptor agonist: in vitro characterization and effects in animal models of Parkinson's disease. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 1996; 276:150–160
16.
Schneider JS, Sun Z-Q, Roeltgen DP: Effects of dihydrexidine, a full dopamine D-1 receptor agonist, on delayed response performance in chronic low-dose MPTP-treated monkeys. Brain Res 1994; 663:140–144
17.
Arnsten AF, Cai JX, Murphy BL, Goldman-Rakic PS: Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994; 116:143–151
18.
Alexander G, DeLong M, Srick P: Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 1990; 9:357–381
19.
Carlsson A: The current status of the dopamine hypotheses of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 1988; 1:179–186
20.
Swerdlow NR, Koob GF: Dopamine, schizophrenia, mania, and depression: toward a unified hypothesis of cortico-striate-pallido-thalmic function. Behav Brain Sci 1987; 10:197–245
21.
Bartlett EJ, Wolkin A, Brodie JD, Laska EM, Wolf AP, Sanfilipo M: Importance of pharmacologic control in PET studies: effects of thiothixene and haloperidol on cerebral glucose utilization in chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res: Neuroimaging 1991; 40:115–124
22.
Buchsbaum MS, Potkin SG, Siegel BV Jr, Lohr J, Katz M, Gottschalk LA, Gulasekaram B, Marshall JF, Lottenberg S, Teng CY, Abel L, Plon L, Bunney WE Jr: Striatal metabolic rate and clinical response to neuroleptics in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:966–974
23.
Buchsbaum MS, Haier RJ, Potlan SG, Nuechterlein K, Bracha HS, Katz M, Lohr J, Wu H, Lottenberg S, Jerabek PA, Trenary M, Tufalla R, Reynolds C, Bunney WE Jr: Frontostriatal disorder of cerebral metabolism in never-medicated schizophrenics. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:935–942
24.
DeLisi L, Holcomb H, Cohen R, Pickar D, Carpenter W, Morihisa J: Positron emission tomography in schizophrenic patients with and without neuroleptic medication. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1985; 5:201–206
25.
Szechtman H, Nahmias C, Garnett ES, Firnau G, Brown GM, Kaplan RD, Cleghorn JM: Effect of neuroleptics on altered cerebral glucose metabolism in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988; 45:523–532
26.
Volkow N, Brodie J, Wolf A, Angrist B, Russell J, Cancro R: Brain metabolism in patients with schizophrenia before and after acute neuroleptic administration. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1986; 49:1199–1202
27.
Wolkin A, Barouche F, Wolf AP, Rotrosen J, Fowler JS, Shiue C-Y, Cooper TB, Brodie JD: Dopamine blockade and clinical response: evidence for two biological subgroups of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1989; 146:905–908
28.
Coppens H, Slooff C, Paans A, Wiegman T, Vaalburg W, Korf J: High central D2-dopamine receptor occupancy as assessed with positron emission tomography in medicated but therapy-resistant schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry 1991; 29:629–634
29.
Pilowsky LS, Costa DC, Ell PJ, Murray RM, Verhoeff NP, Kerwin RW: Antipsychotic medication, D2 dopamine receptor blockade and clinical response: a 123I IBZM SPET (single photon emission tomography) study. Psychol Med 1993; 23:791–797
30.
Nyberg S, Nordström AL, Halldin C, Farde L: Positron emission tomography studies on D2 dopamine receptor occupancy and plasma antipsychotic drug levels in man. Int Clin Psychopharmacol 1995; 10(suppl 3):81–85
31.
Miller DD, Rezai K, Alliger R, Andreasen NC: The effect of antipsychotic medication on relative cerebral blood perfusion in schizophrenia: assessment with technetium-99m hexamethyl-propylene~amine oxime single photon emission computed tomography. Biol Psychiatry 1997; 41:550–559
32.
Cascella NG, Tarazi FI, Shirakawa O, Tamminga CA: Savoxepine fails to selectively influence glucose metabolism in the rat limbic system. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1994; 114:275–280
33.
McCulloch J, Kelly PAT, Ford J: Effects of apomorphine on the relationship between local cerebral utilization and local blood flow. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1982; 2:487–499
34.
Jibiki I, Matsuda H, Yamaguchi N, Kurokawa K, Hisada K: Acutely administered haloperidol-induced widespread reduction of regional cerebral blood flow observed from subtraction of brain imaging with single photon emission computed tomography using technetium-99m hexamethyl-pro~pyleneamine oxime. Neuropsychobiology 1990; 24:125–128
35.
Wolkin A, Sanfilipo M, Duncan E, Angrist B, Wolf AP, Cooper TB, Brodie JD, Laska E, Rotrosen JP: Blunted change in cerebral glucose utilization after haloperidol treatment in schizophrenic patients with prominent negative symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 1996; 153:346–354
36.
Andreasen NC, Olsen S: Negative v positive schizophrenia: definition and validation. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39:789–794
37.
Carpenter WT Jr, Hienrichs DW, Wagman AMI: Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept. Am J Psychiatry 1988; 145:578–583
38.
Mayerhoff DI, Loebel AD, Alvir JMJ, Szymanski SR, Geisler SH, Borenstein M, Lieberman JA: The deficit state in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151:1417–1422
39.
Wolkin A, Sanfilipo M, Angrist B, Duncan E, Wieland S, Wolf AP, Brodie JD, Cooper TB, Laska E, Rotrosen JP: Acute d-amphetamine challenge in schizophrenia: effects on cerebral glucose utilization and clinical symptomatology. Biol Psychiatry 1994; 36:317–325
40.
Lieberman JA, Sheitman B, Kinon BJ: Neurochemical sensitization in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia: deficits in neuronal regulation and plasticity. Neuropsychopharmacology 1997; 17:205–229
41.
Wechsler LR, Savaki HE, Sokoloff L: Effects of d- and l-amphetamine on local cerebral glucose utilization in the conscious rat. J Neurochem 1979; 32:15–22
42.
Mathew RJ, Wilson WH: Dextroamphetamine-induced changes in regional cerebral blood flow. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1985; 87:298–302
43.
Lyons D, Friedman DP, Nader MA, Porrino LJ: Cocaine alters cerebral metabolism within the ventral striatum and limbic cortex of monkeys. J Neurosci 1996; 15:1230–1236
44.
Volkow ND, Wang G-J, Fowler JS, Logan J, Angrist B, Hitzemann R, Lieberman J, Pappas N: Effects of methylphenidate on regional brain glucose metabolism in humans: relationship to dopamine D2 receptors. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:50–55
45.
Porrino LJ, Burns RS, Crane AM, Palombo E, Kopin IJ, Sokoloff L: Local cerebral metabolic effects of L-dopa therapy in 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine-induced parkinsonism in monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1987; 84:5995–5999

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 319 - 323
PubMed: 9501739

History

Published online: 1 March 1998
Published in print: March 1998

Authors

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share