Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 June 2006

Task-Specific Hypoactivation in Prefrontal and Temporoparietal Brain Regions During Motor Inhibition and Task Switching in Medication-Naive Children and Adolescents With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

Objective: A relatively small number of functional imaging studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have shown abnormal prefrontal and striatal brain activation during tasks of motor response inhibition. However, the potential confound of previous medication exposure has not yet been addressed, and no functional imaging study exists to date on medication-naive children and adolescents with ADHD. The aim of this study was to investigate the neural substrates of a range of motor and cognitive inhibitory functions in a relatively large group of children and adolescents with ADHD who had never previously been exposed to medication. Method: Nineteen boys with ADHD and 27 healthy age- and IQ-matched boys underwent functional MRI to compare brain activation during performance of tasks that assessed motor response inhibition (go/no go task), cognitive interference inhibition (motor Stroop task), and cognitive flexibility (switch task). Results: Boys with ADHD showed decreased activation in the left rostral mesial frontal cortex during the go/no go task and decreased activation in the bilateral prefrontal and temporal lobes and right parietal lobe during the switch task. No significant group differences were observed during motor Stroop task performance. Conclusion: Abnormal brain activation was observed in medication-naive children and adolescents with ADHD during tasks involving motor inhibition and task switching, suggesting that hypoactivation in this patient group is unrelated to long-term stimulant exposure. Furthermore, functional abnormalities are task-specific and extend from frontostriatal to parietal and temporal cortices.
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) affects 2% of the U.K. population (1) and is characterized behaviorally by impulsiveness, poor attention, and hyperactivity. The cognitive deficits in children with ADHD include functions of inhibitory, attentional, and motivational control and timing (2) .
Deficits in ADHD may be due to the impairment of fronto-striatal-cerebellar networks, found to be abnormal in children with ADHD (3) . Functional imaging can provide information about neural function during cognitive processing and may be useful in elucidating the dynamic nature of ADHD (4) .
Positron emission tomography (PET) studies of adults with ADHD have revealed underactivation of anterior cingulate cortices associated with working memory (5) and underactivation of the hippocampal gyrus and insula during decision making (6) . Since tasks of motor response inhibition reveal performance deficits in ADHD (7), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of children with ADHD have focused upon associated frontostriatal networks (8) and have shown prefrontal activation to be either reduced (911) or increased (1214) as well as hypoactivation in the anterior cingulate and supplementary motor area (9) and increased activation in temporal cortices (9) . While reduced caudate activation in ADHD patients has been consistently found during motor inhibition (1013), more inconsistent outcomes may be related to small study group sizes or the use of block designs, which are more sensitive to performance differences than event-related designs.
A possible confound in existing imaging studies is the inclusion of patients with ADHD with long-term medication history (15) . Methylphenidate may increase frontostriatal activation (12) and alter frontostriatal dopamine transporter binding sites in patients with ADHD (16) . Long-term changes of methylphenidate on dopamine function have been observed in frontal and striatal regions in animals (17) and in humans as a consequence of chronic stimulant abuse (18) .
This study investigates whether neurofunctional abnormalities during inhibitory control can be observed in medication-naive children and adolescents with ADHD. We extended the probes from motor-inhibitory to more cognitive-inhibitory and attentional domains by using 1) a go/no go task to measure selective motor response inhibition, 2) a motor Stroop task to measure interference inhibition, and 3) a visual-spatial switch task to measure the ability to inhibit previously valid stimulus-response associations during switch events.
Children with ADHD have shown impairments in go/no go, Stroop, and switch tasks (19), which are mediated by specific frontostriatal pathways thought to be impaired in ADHD children: mesial, orbitofrontal, and caudate regions for the go/no go task (20) ; left inferior prefrontal and putamen areas for the motor Stroop task (20) ; and right inferior prefrontal, caudate, and parietal regions for the switch task (21) . However, no fMRI study has yet investigated interference inhibition in a Stroop-like task or cognitive switching in children and adolescents with ADHD, particularly with medication-naive patients.
We hypothesized that medication-naive boys with ADHD would show a reduction in task-specific prefrontal brain activation relative to healthy comparison subjects during frontal lobe-mediated tasks of motor and cognitive inhibition.

Method

Subjects

Male right-handed boys were recruited (18–27 healthy subjects and 14–19 subjects with ADHD), although data were not available for all subjects for all tasks because of technical problems, time constraints, movement artifacts, and scanner intolerability. Female subjects were excluded because the male/female ratio for ADHD ranges from 3:1 to 9:1 (22) . Subjects were recruited through advertisements, parent groups, and clinics and were paid £22 for their participation. Parental consent and approval from the local ethical committee was obtained. Clinical diagnosis of ADHD (combined subtype) was established by a trained psychiatrist using the standardized Maudsley diagnostic interview (23), which includes a systematic symptom checklist that excludes autism and other pervasive developmental disorders, bipolar disorder, affective disorders, and anxiety state. Subjects with ADHD inattentive subtype were not recruited for this study. Five patients with comorbid conduct disorder, secondary to ADHD, were included. Subjects with ADHD scored above the cutoff for hyperactive symptoms on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (24) and had significantly higher scores on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire than comparison subjects for all three task comparisons (t>–9.2, df>33, p<0.001). The two groups also had significantly different scores on the five items within the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (restlessness, fidgeting, distractibility, thinking things out, and attention span) (t>–4, df>33, p<0.001). Patients were medication-naive through personal choice or were scanned prior to initial medication. All subjects scored above the 10th percentile on the Standard Progressive Matrices (25) and there were no significant group differences in Raven scores or age ( Table 1 ), although differences in age approached significance when groups were compared by motor Stroop task performance (z=2.0, df=41, p=0.051).

Paradigms

The tasks were explained to the subjects, and each task was completed for practice on a laptop. Instructions were repeated again before each task during the scanning session.
Tasks were presented on a mirror within the scanner. A keypad with a four-button, diamond configuration was used, and responses were recorded throughout image acquisition on a PC. All tasks lasted approximately 6 minutes, and activation associated only with successful trials was included within all three analyses.
A rapid, randomized, mixed-trial, event-related fMRI design was used for all tasks with jittered interstimulus intervals providing optimal statistical efficiency (26) . For the go/no go and motor Stroop tasks, the interstimulus intervals were jittered between 1.6 seconds and 2 seconds (mean 1.8 seconds), and for the switch task between 2.2 seconds and 2.6 seconds (mean 2.4 seconds).
The go/no go and motor Stroop tasks both consisted of 208 trials that included 24 no go/Stroop trials and 24 oddball trials (to control for low frequency events and providing increased control). In the go/no go task, arrows in the center of the screen pointed either left or right for 600 msec and the subject had to press the left or right button respectively. No go arrows pointed upward for 600 msec, and the subject was told not to respond to these. The oddball arrows pointed at a 45° angle either left or right, and subjects were told to respond as they would to a “go” prompt ( Figure 1 ).
Figure 1. The Go/No Go Tasks and Motor Stroop Tasks Performed During fMRI Acquisition by Children and Adolescents With ADHD and Healthy Comparison Subjects
In the motor Stroop task, congruent stimuli consisted of arrows either on the left of the screen pointing left or the right of the screen pointing right, appearing for 600 msec. The subject was told to press the left or right button, respectively. The incongruent (Stroop) stimuli consisted of arrows pointing left but appearing on the right of the screen, or arrows pointing right and appearing on the left of the screen for 600 msec. The subject was told to press according to the direction of the arrow, not the side of the screen upon which it appeared. Oddball arrows pointed at a 45° angle either left or right, and subjects were told to respond as they would to a congruent stimulus ( Figure 1 ).
The event-related analysis for the go/no go and Stroop tasks contrasted activation associated with no go and incongruent trials with that of oddball trials after baseline go and congruent trials had been subtracted from both.
In the event-related fMRI adaptation (21) of the Meiran Switch task (27), a target appeared in a corner of a grid with a central arrow ( Figure 2 ). If the central arrow was horizontal, the subject indicated whether the target was on the left or right side of the grid (left or right button). If the central arrow was vertical, the subject indicated whether the target was in the lower or upper half of the grid (up or down button). Every 4–6 repeat trials were followed by a switch trial where the arrow changed position. The event-related analysis contrasted switch trial activation with repeat trial activation.
Figure 2. The Switch Task Performed During fMRI Acquisition by Children and Adolescents With ADHD and Healthy Comparison Subjects

Image Acquisition and Analysis

Gradient-echo echoplanar MR imaging data were acquired on a GE Signa 1.5T Horizon LX System (General Electric, Milwaukee) retrofitted magnet with Advanced NMR hardware and software (ANMR, Woburn, Mass.) at the Maudsley Hospital, London. Consistent image quality was ensured. A quadrature birdcage headcoil was used for radiofrequency transmission and reception. In each of 16 noncontiguous planes parallel to the anterior-posterior commissure, 208 (go/no go, Stroop task) or 152 (switch task) T2*-weighted MR images depicting BOLD contrast covering the whole brain were acquired (TE=40 msec; TR=1.8 seconds [go/no go, Stroop task] or 2.4 seconds [switch task]; flip angle: 90˚; in-plane resolution: 3.1 mm; slice thickness: 7 mm; slice-skip: 0.7 mm). Simultaneously, a high-resolution inversion recovery echo-planar image of the whole brain was acquired in the intercommissural plane. This echo planar imaging dataset provided almost complete brain coverage.

Individual Analysis

The data were realigned (28) to minimize motion-related artifacts and smoothed using a Gaussian filter (7.2 mm, full width at half maximum). Responses to the experimental paradigms were detected by time-series analysis using gamma-variate functions (peak responses at 4 and 8 seconds) to model the BOLD response. Each experimental condition was then convolved separately with the 4-second and 8-second Poisson functions to yield two models of the expected hemodynamic response to that condition. The weighted sum of these two convolutions that gave the best fit (least squares) to the time series at each voxel was then computed to allow voxel-wise variability in time to peak hemodynamic response. A goodness-of-fit statistic (sum of squares ratio, i.e., the sum of squares of deviations from the mean intensity value due to the model divided by the sum of squares due to the residuals) was computed at each voxel. The time series was permuted 10 times at each voxel using a wavelet-based resampling method (29) and the data combined over all voxels, resulting in 10 permuted parametric sum of squares ratio maps at each plane for each subject. This strategy was applied at each voxel to preserve spatial correlational structure in the data during randomization. Combining the randomized data over all voxels yields the null hypothesis distribution of sum of squares ratios. Voxels activated at any desired level of type I error can then be determined.

Group Mapping

The observed and randomized sum of squares ratio maps were first transformed into standard space (30) . By applying the two spatial transformations computed for each subject to the statistic maps obtained by analyzing the observed and wavelet-randomized data, a generic brain activation map could be produced for each experimental condition by taking the median observed sum of squares ratio over all subjects at each voxel (using median values to minimize outlier effects) at each intracerebral voxel in standard space (31) and comparing it against a critical value of the permutation distribution for median sum of squares ratio ascertained from the spatially transformed wavelet-permuted data (30) . Hypothesis testing was carried out at the cluster level using a method developed initially for structural image analysis (28) and subsequently shown to give excellent cluster-wise type I error control in both structural and functional MRI analysis. When applied to fMRI data, this method estimates the probability of occurrence of clusters under the null hypothesis using the distribution of median sum of squares ratios. Zero false positive activated clusters were expected at a p value of <0.05 (voxel level) and 0.01 (cluster level).

Group Comparisons

The differences between the mean sum of squares ratio values in each group were calculated at each voxel. The mean ratio was then recalculated many times at each voxel following random permutation of group membership, yielding the distribution of mean differences under the null hypothesis of no effect of group membership. Voxel-wise maps of significant group differences at any desired level of type I error are then available. If identical permutations are carried out at each voxel (preserving spatial correlations), this method can then be extended to yield cluster-level differences (28) . Zero false positive activated clusters were expected at a p value of <0.05 (voxel level) and 0.01 (cluster level).

Results

Performance Data

Each group’s performance data are shown in Table 2 . Patients with ADHD had significantly slower reaction times to repeat trials in the switch task. The ADHD subjects also had significantly greater within-subject variability of reaction time to “go” prompts in the go/no go task, congruent stimuli in the motor Stroop task, and repeat trials in the switch task. No other group differences were observed.

Imaging Data

No significant group differences were observed in the extent of three-dimensional motion during any of the tasks.
Regions of activation for each group for each task as well as significant differences in activation between groups for each task are presented in a data supplement that accompanies the online version of this article. Significant activation associated with the no go/oddball contrast for healthy subjects included the mesial frontal and right inferior prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, right caudate, left temporal cortex, precuneus, bilateral insula, and right sensorimotor cortex. Boys with ADHD activated right superior and left inferior prefrontal cortices, left temporal cortex, and cerebellum. An ANCOVA revealed significantly greater activation in healthy subjects of left rostral mesial frontal cortex during successful no go versus oddball trials ( Figure 3 ).
Figure 3. Region of Significantly Greater Activation in Healthy Comparison Subjects During Successful “No Go” Inhibition Relative to Oddball Stimulus Reaction
The Stroop/oddball contrast in the healthy subjects revealed activation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, left inferior frontal cortices, right sensorimotor cortex, left temporal cortex, and bilateral cerebellum. Boys with ADHD activated the right inferior frontal and middle temporal cortices, left sensorimotor cortex, caudate, thalamus, and cerebellum. An ANCOVA revealed no significant group differences in activation for this contrast.
During switch trials, healthy subjects activated the right anterior cingulate, bilateral basal ganglia, and temporal and inferior parietal cortices. Patients with ADHD activated the right superior prefrontal cortex, bilateral anterior cingulate, and left inferior parietal lobe. Group comparison revealed two clusters of increased activation in healthy subjects compared with patients, one extending from the left inferior prefrontal to superior temporal gyrus and insula ( Figure 4 ) and another more right-sided cluster that extended from the inferior prefrontal gyrus through superior and middle temporal to inferior parietal cortex ( Figure 5 ).
Figure 4. Left-Sided Regions of Significantly Greater Activation in Healthy Comparison Subjects Relative to Children and Adolescents With ADHD During a Switch Task
Figure 5. Right-Sided Regions of Significantly Greater Activation in Healthy Comparison Subjects Relative to Children and Adolescents With ADHD During a Switch Task

Group Differences in Heterogeneity of Activation

Greater numbers of healthy subjects versus patients (27 versus 14) in the switch comparison may have influenced group differences in activation: if the variance in brain activation is comparable, then the difference in N is not problematic. If the variance is greater in the smaller group, then tests of differences may be too liberal; conversely, if variance is greater in the larger group, then tests of difference may be conservative. Coordinates (x, y, z) were extracted for both clusters of differences in activation for each subject. A principal component analysis was performed on the standardized residuals (to control for group membership bias) using an unrotated factor solution based on the correlation matrix. One principal component for each cluster of difference in brain activation emerged, explaining between 55% and 59% of the variances. Increased variance was observed for the first principal component of the residuals of the peak coordinates of the left hemisphere cluster of activation difference in the larger group of healthy subjects (F=4.299, df=39, p=0.045), suggesting that this comparison is conservative. No group differences in variance were observed in the right hemisphere cluster.

Covarying for Age

Because the age difference between the two groups approached significance, we computed correlations between the strength of significant clusters in the ANOVA analysis and age, although none were significant: go/no go task (r=0.003, p=0.988); switch task (left hemisphere cluster: r=0.18, p=0.27; right hemisphere cluster: r=0.30, p=0.06). However, given the potential impact of age and its borderline significant relationship with the right hemisphere cluster of increased activation in healthy subjects during the switch task, age was included as a covariate in ANCOVAs on imaging and performance data. All clusters showing between-group differences of activation within the go/no go and the switch tasks remained significant. For performance data, entering age as a covariate resulted in the between-group differences becoming nonsignificant for variability of reaction time for the go/no go (F=2.2, df=1, 32, p=0.15), motor Stroop (F=3.4, df=1, 40, p=0.07), and switch (F=3.9, df=1, 38, p=0.06) tasks. The significant difference in mean reaction time to repeat trials during the switch task also disappeared (F=2.4, df=1, 38, p=0.13).

Controlling for Conduct Disorder

To control for the potentially confounding effect of comorbid conduct disorder, we reanalyzed the group comparison removing those subjects with comorbid conduct disorder (go/no go: N=13; switch: N=11). All original significant clusters of between-group differences remained significant.

Discussion

Despite similar task performance, medication-naive boys with ADHD showed hypoactivation in task-specific brain regions compared with healthy subjects in two tasks: reduced activation in rostral mesial prefrontal cortex during the go/no go task and in right hemispheric inferior prefrontal, temporal, and parietal brain regions during the switch task. No differences in brain activation were observed during the motor Stroop task.
Patients were not impaired on the specific performance measures in this study: we wanted to equalize performance across the two groups, and so tasks were easier and slower versions of the same tasks in which, except for the Stroop task, we have found performance deficits in children and adolescents with ADHD (32) .
Deficits in the rostral mesial frontal cortex in ADHD subjects during motor response inhibition support previous functional imaging findings reporting mesial prefrontal underactivation in ADHD during a stop task (10), a go/no go task (9), and a motor timing task (10) . The reduced rostral prefrontal activation during intact inhibitory performance may be related to comeasured processes of selective attention and decision making (8, 20), since more lateral prefrontal brain regions such as right dorsolateral and inferior prefrontal cortices are thought to mediate inhibitory control (20) . Rostral mesial prefrontal cortex activation in adults during go/no go task performance has been related to decision making/response selection aspects of the inhibitory processes measured in this task (20, 33) . Reduced activation in the mesial prefrontal cortex has been observed in ADHD during go/no go (9), stop (10), Stroop (34), and timing tasks (10) . Our study shows for the first time that this region is also underactivated in medication-naive children and adolescents with the disorder, suggesting that difficulties in recruitment of the mesial prefrontal cortex are inherent to ADHD pathology and unconfounded by medication history.
Performance of healthy adults on the same switching task used in this study has been shown to be mediated by prefrontal, temporal, and parietal cortices, regions presumably concerned with inhibition of irrelevant stimulus-response associations (prefrontal lobe), preparation of potential action (temporal lobe), and facilitating sustained visual attention (parietal lobe) (21) . Right inferior prefrontal activation has been strongly associated with inhibitory processes in stop (8, 33), go/no go (20, 33), and switch (20, 21) tasks and has emerged in adults as the common substrate for a shared inhibitory process in a conjunction analysis across tasks of switching and motor inhibition (35) . We thus speculate that underactivation of the right inferior prefrontal cortex in ADHD during the switch task may be attributable to inhibitory processes. This is supported by the finding of underactivation of the right inferior prefrontal cortex during a stop task in this same group of medication-naive children and adolescents with ADHD (11) and in an earlier sample of boys with ADHD (10) . Underactivation of the right inferior prefrontal cortex in the same patient group during both successful stop and switch task performance suggests an underfunctioning neural substrate for an inhibitory mechanism required for both of these tasks.
The finding of right fronto-temporo-parietal hypoactivation in ADHD extends the deficit beyond hypothesized impairment of frontostriatal networks. While predominantly frontal lobe tasks elicit frontal abnormalities, the switch task, with its relatively greater load on parieto-temporal activation (21), gives rise to additional deficits in these areas in patients with ADHD. This study supports structural and metabolic findings of abnormalities in temporal and parietal as well as prefrontal cortices: structural MRI studies found reduced bilateral parietal lobe volumes (36) and reduced lateral-temporal and parietal cortex surfaces in children with ADHD (37) . Functional rest studies using SPECT found reduced blood flow in right middle temporal and prefrontal cortex (38) and left frontal and parietal regions (39) in children with ADHD. Blood flow in temporal cortex is shown to be inversely correlated to the degree of motor impairment in children with ADHD (40) . Medial temporal (41) and parietal lobes (42) do not achieve maturity until at least age 10, which may be further delayed in children and adolescents with ADHD (43) .
In this study, underactivation in mesial and fronto-parieto-temporal brain regions during go/no go and switch task performance, respectively, did not lead to impaired performance. Brain activation measures may be more sensitive to abnormalities than performance. Alternatively, ADHD patients might have used idiosyncratic, alternative compensatory brain activation patterns that did not survive the critical thresholds for the group activation contrasts.
The integrity of neural networks during motor Stroop task performance in boys with ADHD reflects controversial behavioral findings on the sensitivity of the Stroop task in ADHD (19) and our findings of no impairment in performance scores in children with ADHD using an offline version of this task (32) . However, an fMRI study of adults with ADHD found hypoactivation of the anterior cingulate during a counting Stroop task (34), which may have been more challenging than our motor Stroop task. Alternatively, the neural activation of adult patients may differ from that of children and adolescents with ADHD or may also have been altered by their long-term exposure to stimulant medication.
A few ADHD cases could not be scanned, which resulted in varying Ns for each task. This may have introduced a bias and limits the generalizability of findings and also the generalizability concerning medication-naivety across tasks. However, this study is a first attempt to investigate neurofunctional abnormalities in ADHD independent of potential confounds of chronic stimulant medication. Comparisons of ADHD patients with and without medication history within the same study design are needed to further clarify the extent and nature of the effects of long-term medication.
In conclusion, this study shows that medication-naive children and adolescents with ADHD demonstrate task-specific underactivation during performance of motor response inhibition and task switching that is not limited to the prefrontal cortex but also includes parietal and temporal cortices.

Footnote

Received Jan. 31, 2005; revisions received Aug. 15, Oct. 11, and Nov. 21, 2005; accepted Dec. 2, 2005. From the Department of Child Psychiatry, Department of Biostatistics and Computing, and Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College, London, U.K. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Smith, Department of Child Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, PO47, London, SE5 8AF, U.K.Supported by grants from the Medical Research Council (G9900839) and the Wellcome Trust (053272/Z/98/Z/JRS/JP/JAT).

Supplementary Material

File (ajp_163_6_1044_01.pdf)

References

1.
Taylor E: Clinical foundations of hyperactivity research. Behavioural Brain Res 1998; 94:11–24
2.
Barkley RA: Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 1997; 121:65–94
3.
Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Molloy E, Castellanos FX: Brain imaging of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2001; 931:33–49
4.
Rubia K: The dynamic approach to neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders: Use of fMRI combined with neuropsychology to elucidate the dynamics of psychiatric disorders, exemplified in ADHD and schizophrenia. Behavioural Brain Res 2002; 130(1–2):47–56
5.
Schweitzer JB, Faber TL, Grafton ST, Tune LE, Hoffman JM, Kilts CD: Alterations in the functional anatomy of working memory in adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:278–280
6.
Ernst M, Kimes AS, London ED, Matochik JA, Eldreth D, Tata S, Contoreggi C, Leff M, Bolla K: Neural substrates of decision making in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1061–1070
7.
Rubia K, Taylor E, Smith A, Oksannen H, Overmeyer S, Newman S: Neurophysiological analyses of impulsiveness in childhood hyperactivity. Br J Psychiatry 2001; 179:138–143
8.
Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Taylor E: Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. NeuroImage 2003; 20:351–358
9.
Tamm L, Menon V, Ringel J, Reiss AL: Event-related fMRI evidence of frontotemporal involvement in aberrant response inhibition and task switching in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004; 43:1430–1440
10.
Rubia K, Overmeyer S, Taylor E, Brammer M, Williams SCR, Simmons A, Bullmore ET: Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-order motor control: a study using functional MRI. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:891–896
11.
Rubia K, Smith A, Brammer M, Toone B, Taylor E: Medication-naive adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder show abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:1067–1075
12.
Vaidya CJ, Austin G, Kirkorian G, Ridlehuber HW, Desmond JE, Glover GH, et al: Selective effects of methylphenidate in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance study. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 1998; 95:14494–14499
13.
Durston S, Tottenham NT, Thomas KM, Davidson MC, Eigsti IM, Yang Y, et al: Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without ADHD. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53:871–878
14.
Schulz KP, Fan J, Tang CY, Newcorn JH, Buchsbaum MS, Cheung AM, Halperin JM: Response inhibition in adolescents diagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during childhood: an event-related fMRI study. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1650–1657
15.
Leo J, Cohen D: Broken brains or flawed studies? A critical review of ADHD neuroimaging research. J Mind Behav 2003; 24:29–56
16.
Dresel S, Krause J, Krause KH, LaFougere C, Brinkbaumer K, Kung HF, et al: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: binding of [99mTc]TRODAT-1 to the dopamine transporter before and after methylphenidate treatment. Eur J Nucleic Med 2000; 27:1518–1524
17.
Sproson EJ, Chantrey J, Hollis C, Marsden CA, Fonel KC: Effect of repeated methylphenidate administration on presynaptic dopamine and behaviour in young adult rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001; 15:67–75
18.
Sekine Y, Minabe Y, Ouchi Y, Takei N, Iyo M, Nakamura K, et al: Association of dopamine transporter loss in the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices with methamphetamine-related psychiatric symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:1699–1701
19.
Sergeant JA, Geurts H, Oosterlaan J: How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behavioural Brain Res 2002; 130(1–2):3–28
20.
Rubia K, Smith AB, Woolley J, Nosarti C, Heyman I, Brammer M, et al: Progressive age-related increase of task-specific prefrontal and parietal brain activation during cognitive control: a developmental study using fast, event-related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp (in press)
21.
Smith AB, Taylor E, Brammer M, Rubia K: The neural correlates of switching set as measured in fast, event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Hum Brain Mapp 2004; 21:247–256
22.
Horn WF, Wagner AE, Ialongo N: Sex differences in school-aged children with pervasive attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1989; 17:109–125
23.
Goldberg D, Murrey R (eds): Maudsley Handbook of Practical Psychiatry. Oxford, UK, Oxford University Press, 2002
24.
Goodman R: The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997; 38:581–586
25.
Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH: Manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales, Section 3: The Standard Progressive Matrices. San Antonio, The Psychological Corporation, 1998
26.
Dale AM: Optimal design for event-related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 1999; 8:109–114
27.
Meiran N: Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to task performance. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 1996; 22:1423–1442
28.
Bullmore ET, Suckling J, Overmeyer S, Rabe-Hesketh S, Taylor E, Brammer MJ: Global, voxel, and cluster tests, by theory and permutation, for a difference between two groups of structural MR images of the brain. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 1999; 18:32–42
29.
Bullmore E, Long C, Suckling J, Fadili J, Calvert G, Zelaya F, et al: Colored noise and computational inference in neurophysiological (fMRI) time series analysis: resampling methods in time and wavelet domains. Hum Brain Mapp 2001; 12:61–78
30.
Brammer MJ, Bullmore ET, Simmons A, Williams SC, Grasby PM, Howard RJ, et al: Generic brain activation mapping in functional magnetic resonance imaging: a nonparametric approach. Magn Res Imaging 1997; 15:763–770
31.
Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the brain. New York, Thieme, 1988
32.
Rubia K, Smith A, Taylor E: The Maudsley Attention and Response Suppression Task (MARS) battery: a test battery to measure impulsiveness applied to children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychol (in press)
33.
Rubia K, Russell T, Overmeyer S, Brammer M, Bullmore E, Sharma T, et al: Mapping motor inhibition: conjunctive brain activations across different versions of go/no-go and stop tasks. NeuroImage 2001; 13:250–261
34.
Bush G, Frazier JA, Rauch SL, Seidman LJ, Whalen PJ, Jenike MA, et al: Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the counting Stroop. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:1542–1552
35.
Konishi S, Nakajima K, Uchida I, Kikyo H, Kameyama M, Miyashita Y: Common inhibitory mechanism in human inferior prefrontal cortex revealed by event-related functional MRI. Brain 1999; 122:981–991
36.
Filipek PA, Semrud-Clikeman M, Steingard RJ, Renshaw PF, Kennedy DN, Biederman J: Volumetric MRI analysis comparing subjects having attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder with normal controls. Neurol 1997; 48:589–601
37.
Sowell ER, Thompson PM, Welcome SE, Henkenius AL, Toga AW, Peterson BS: Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet 2003; 362:1699–1707
38.
Kim BN, Lee JS, Shin MS, Cho SC, Lee DS: Regional cerebral perfusion abnormalities in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: statistical parametric mapping analysis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002; 252:219–225
39.
Sieg KG, Gaffney GR, Preston DF, Hellings JA: SPECT brain imaging abnormalities in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin Nuclear Med 1995; 20:55–60
40.
Gustafsson P, Thernlund G, Ryding E, Rosen I, Cederblad M: Associations between cerebral blood-flow measured by single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), electro-encephalogram (EEG), behaviour symptoms, cognition and neurological soft signs in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Acta Paediatrica 2000; 89:830–835
41.
Oades RD: Frontal, temporal and lateralized brain function in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a psychophysiological and neuropsychological viewpoint on development. Behavioural Brain Res 1998; 94:83–95
42.
Giedd JN, Snell JW, Lange N, Rajapakse JC, Casey BJ, Kozuch PL, et al: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging of human brain development: ages 4–18. Cerebral Cortex 1996; 6:551–560
43.
Castellanos FX, Lee PP, Sharp W, Jeffries NO, Greenstein DK, Clasen LS, et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA 2002; 288:1740–1748

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 1044 - 1051
PubMed: 16741205

History

Published online: 1 June 2006
Published in print: June, 2006

Authors

Details

Eric Taylor, M.D., Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share