Skip to main content
Full access
Brief Report
Published Online: 1 February 2003

Parahippocampal Volume Deficits in Subjects With Aging-Associated Cognitive Decline

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Neuropathological evidence suggests that the earliest changes in Alzheimer’s disease selectively affect the parahippocampal regions of the brain. This study was conducted to determine if otherwise healthy elderly subjects with mild cognitive impairment had structural volume deficits affecting the parahippocampal gyrus. METHOD: Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was used to compare global and regional brain volumes in 21 subjects with mild cognitive deficits defined according to the criteria for aging-associated cognitive decline, 22 cognitively intact comparison subjects, and 12 patients with Alzheimer’s disease. RESULTS: Compared with the cognitively intact subjects, the subjects with aging-associated cognitive decline had a significantly smaller mean volume of the right parahippocampal gyrus. The subjects with aging-associated cognitive decline had a mean parahippocampal volume that was intermediate between that of the Alzheimer’s disease patients and that of the cognitively intact subjects. CONCLUSIONS: Parahippocampal atrophy underlies the observed cognitive deficits in aging-associated cognitive decline. These findings support the hypothesis that aging-associated cognitive decline represents a preclinical stage of Alzheimer’s disease.
Postmortem studies have indicated that morphological brain changes during the early, preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease almost selectively involve the parahippocampal gyrus (1, 2). However, previous studies using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) for volumetric investigations of the medial temporal lobe in elderly subjects with mild cognitive deficits, who are generally considered to have an increased risk for developing Alzheimer’s disease, have yielded inconclusive results (312). The diversity of findings may be related partly to the criteria used to define mild cognitive deficits, insofar as most previous MRI studies refer to amnestic forms, such as age-associated memory impairment (13) and mild cognitive impairment (14). An alternative approach for defining mild cognitive deficits makes use of the concept of aging-associated cognitive decline (15). In contrast to age-associated memory impairment, aging-associated cognitive decline is defined by age-adjusted norms and considers deficits in a broad range of cognitive domains.
The present study examined regional brain volumes in a group of otherwise healthy elderly subjects with mild cognitive deficits defined according to the criteria for aging-associated cognitive decline. On the basis of previous neuropathological findings, we expected these subjects to have structural volume deficits in the medial temporal lobe regions, particularly the parahippocampal gyrus.

Method

Subjects with mild cognitive deficits and cognitively intact comparison subjects were recruited among a subset of subjects from the Heidelberg area who participated in a population-based, interdisciplinary, longitudinal study of adult development (N=252 subjects born from 1930 to 1932). The health of all subjects was screened by means of medical history, physical examination, ECG, and laboratory testing. Cognitive performance was assessed with an extensive neuropsychological test battery. A detailed description of the study protocol for the longitudinal study and a description of the neuropsychological test battery have been published previously (16, 17).
Subjects with aging-associated cognitive decline were identified according to the following criteria, developed by a work group of the International Psychogeriatric Association (15):
1. Performance on a standardized test of cognition at least one standard deviation below the age-adjusted norm in at least one of the following domains: learning and memory, language, attention and cognitive speed, and visuoconstructional abilities.
2. Exclusion of any medical, neurological, or psychiatric disorder that could produce cognitive deterioration, as determined by the medical history and/or clinical examination.
3. Normal activities of daily living and exclusion of dementia identified according to the DSM-IV criteria.
Thirty-five of the 47 subjects who fulfilled the criteria for aging-associated cognitive decline at baseline as well as a corresponding number of cognitively unimpaired comparison subjects (matched for age, gender, and educational level) consented to participate in an MRI investigation that took place 30–50 months after the first visit. Thus, 71 subjects in the original interdisciplinary longitudinal study of adult development were reexamined clinically, neuropsychologically, and by MRI at follow-up.
Subjects with significant indications of cerebrovascular disease revealed by MRI, emergence of a neurological/medical condition sufficient to cause cognitive decline, or incomplete/inappropriate MRI data sets (e.g., withdrawal due to claustrophobia, motion artifacts) were excluded. There were no significant differences in neuropsychological and sociodemographic characteristics between those who were excluded and those who remained in the analysis. Of the remaining 43 subjects, 21 fulfilled the criteria for aging-associated cognitive decline at follow-up, and 22 constituted the cognitively unimpaired comparison group.
As an additional reference group, 12 patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease (diagnosed according to the criteria of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association [18]) were recruited from the section on geriatric psychiatry at the University of Heidelberg. The 12 patients were matched for age and gender with the other study groups. The Global Deterioration Scale (19) was used to compare the degree of cognitive impairment among the three study groups.
The MRI data were acquired with a 1.5-T Siemens scanner (Erlangen, Germany) by using a three-dimensional magnetized prepared rapid gradient echo sequence for the T1-weighted slices (TR=10 msec, TE=4 msec) and a three-dimensional reverse fast imaging with steady precession sequence (TR=17 msec, TE=7 msec) for the T2-weighted slices, generating 128 coronal slices for each sequence (slice thickness=1.8 mm). Volumetric measurements included the total intracranial volume, the whole brain volume, and the CSF volume, as well as volumes of the frontal and temporal lobes and the hippocampal formation bilaterally, as described previously (20, 21). In addition, volume measures of the parahippocampal gyrus were sampled on the same slices as the hippocampal measures. Although total intracranial volume and whole brain volume were determined by using semiautomatic segmentation algorithms (r=0.98 [intercorrelation coefficient], p<0.0005, N=20), volumes of the other structures were evaluated by manual tracing (0.96<r<0.99 [intercorrelation coefficient], p<0.0005, N=16). (A detailed protocol of measurements will be sent on request.) All volumetric data were corrected by dividing each raw value by the subject’s total intracranial volume. Analyses of variance with Duncan’s tests were calculated to compare the data between the diagnostic groups. Stepwise linear regression was used to assess the relationship between the extent of global cognitive impairment (Global Deterioration Scale score), level of education, and corrected morphometric values within the aging-associated cognitive decline group.

Results

Age, gender distribution, level of education, and total intracranial volume did not differ significantly between the groups. Subjects with aging-associated cognitive decline had a significantly lower mean volume of the right parahippocampal gyrus (–12%) (F=13.8, df=2, 52, p=0.0001) than the comparison group (Table 1). The mean volume of the left parahippocampal gyrus was 7% smaller in the aging-associated cognitive decline group than in the comparison group, although this difference did not reach statistical significance. By contrast, Alzheimer’s disease patients had a significantly lower volume of the bilateral parahippocampal gyrus (right: F=13.8, df=2, 52, p=0.0001; left: F=14.2, df=2, 52, p=0.0001), relative to both the aging-associated cognitive decline group (right: 13.6% lower in the Alzheimer’s disease patients; left: 24.0% lower) and the comparison group (right: 24.0% lower in the Alzheimer’s disease patients; left: 29.6% lower).
The whole brain volume, the CSF volumes, and the frontal, temporal, and hippocampal volumes of the aging-associated cognitive decline subjects were not significantly different from those of the comparison subjects. In contrast, the Alzheimer’s disease patients were characterized by significantly smaller frontal as well as hippocampal and temporal lobe volumes bilaterally, relative to both the group with aging-associated cognitive decline and the comparison group (3.8≤F≤25.4, df=2, 52, 0.0001≤p≤0.03).
In a stepwise linear regression analysis, only the right parahippocampal gyrus volume remained in the regression model for the extent of global cognitive impairment of the aging-associated cognitive decline subjects as indicated by the Global Deterioration Scale (R=0.53, F=7.4, df=1, 20, p<0.05).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first MRI study demonstrating a significant deficit in the volume of the parahippocampal gyrus in subjects with aging-associated cognitive decline. Consistent, although much more pronounced, changes were found in the Alzheimer’s disease patients, indicating that, with respect to parahippocampal gyrus volume, the subjects with aging-associated cognitive decline took an intermediate position between the Alzheimer’s disease patients and the cognitively intact comparison subjects. Our results suggest that cerebral volume deficits in subjects with aging-associated cognitive decline preferably, and possibly selectively, involve the parahippocampal gyrus, leaving other regions of the brain relatively unaffected.
In view of the neuropathological evidence, it is conceivable that the observed parahippocampal gyrus volume deficit reflects degenerative atrophic changes, as described for the early preclinical (transentorhinal) stages of Alzheimer’s disease (1, 2). This assumption is supported by the significant association between parahippocampal gyrus volumes and severity of cognitive deficits in the aging-associated cognitive decline group, a finding in line with the view that pathological alterations in the parahippocampal gyrus leading to a disconnection of the hippocampus from neocortical association areas represent the neuroanatomical correlate of cognitive dysfunction in early dementia (2, 22).
Hitherto, structural MRI studies have revealed only inconclusive evidence linking medial temporal lobe atrophy with mild cognitive deficits. The divergent results may be related to aspects of study subject selection. In the majority of previous studies—in particular those yielding negative results—subjects were selected on the basis of the concept of age-associated memory impairment (46, 9), which does not consider age-adjusted norm values when defining cognitive deficits. In addition, age-associated memory impairment refers solely to mnestic functions and might exclude at-risk subjects for whom the development of dementia is preceded by a disturbance of other cognitive domains. In contrast to age-associated memory impairment, aging-associated cognitive decline includes not only memory problems but also performance deficits in any other principal domain of cognition. Accordingly, the concept of aging-associated cognitive decline takes into account the heterogeneity of neuropsychological deficits potentially preceding the development of Alzheimer’s disease. Indeed, impairment defined according to the criteria for aging-associated cognitive decline could be associated with a higher risk for subsequent dementia than impairment defined according to previously used criteria (23).
In summary, we conclude that quantitative MRI has the potential to detect the earliest morphological alterations of the medial temporal lobe in individuals who are at risk for developing the full clinical picture of Alzheimer’s disease subsequently. These alterations appear to affect selectively the parahippocampal gyrus. Such findings could be used to facilitate the preclinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease.
TABLE 1

Footnote

Presented in part at the 7th World Congress of Biological Psychiatry in Berlin, July 1–6, 2001. Received March 11, 2002; revision received Aug. 20, 2002; accepted Sept. 6, 2002. From the Section on Geriatric Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany; the German Center for Research on Aging, Heidelberg; and the German Cancer Research Center, Heidelberg. Send reprint requests to Prof. Dr. Schröder, Psychiatrische Universitätsklinik, Voss-Strasse 4, 69115 Heidelberg, Germany; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by a grant from the Ministry of Science and Research (Baden-Württemberg) and the German Federal Ministry for Families, Senior Citizens, Women, and Youth. The authors thank Dr. Daniel Hüger for assistance with the morphometric measures and Prof. Dr. Ursula Lehr and Prof. Dr. Peter Martin for helpful criticism. Johannes Pantel, M.D., Ph.D., and Johannes Schröder, M.D., Ph.D., contributed equally to this study.

References

1.
Braak H, Braak E, Bohl J: Staging of Alzheimer-related cortical destruction. Eur Neurol 1993; 33:403-408
2.
Van Hoesen GW, Augustinack JC, Dierking J, Redman SJ, Thangavel R: The parahippocampal gyrus in Alzheimer’s disease: clinical and preclinical neuroanatomical correlates. Ann NY Acad Sci 2000; 911:254-274
3.
Jack CR Jr, Petersen RC, Xu YC, O’Brien PC, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Waring SC, Tangelos EG, Kokmen E: Prediction of AD with MRI-based hippocampal volume in mild cognitive impairment. Neurology 1999; 52:1397-1403
4.
Soininen HS, Partanen K, Pitkänen A, Vainio P, Hänninen T, Hallikainen M, Koivisto K, Riekkinen PJ: Volumetric MRI analysis of the amygdala and the hippocampus in subjects with age-associated memory impairment: correlation to visual and verbal memory. Neurology 1994; 44:1660-1668
5.
Laakso MP, Partanen K, Lehtovirta M, Hallikainen P, Hänninen T, Vainio P, Riekkinen P, Soininen H: MRI of amygdala fails to diagnose early Alzheimer’s disease. Neuroreport 1995; 6:2414-2418
6.
Parnetti L, Lowenthal DT, Presciutti O, Pelliccioli GP, Palumbo R, Gobbi G, Chiarini P, Palumbo B, Tarducci R, Senin U: 1H-MRS, MRI-based hippocampal volumetry, and 99Tc-HMPAO-SPECT in normal aging, age-associated memory impairment, and probable Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 1996; 44:133-138
7.
Convit A, de Leon MJ, Tarshish C, de Santi S, Tsui W, Rusinek H, George A: Specific hippocampal volume reductions in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 1997; 18:131-138
8.
Kaye JA, Swihart T, Howieson D, Dame A, Moore MM, Karnos T, Camicioli R, Ball M, Oken B, Sexton G: Volume loss of the hippocampus and temporal lobe in healthy elderly persons destined to develop dementia. Neurology 1997; 48:1297-1304
9.
Laakso MP, Soininen H, Partanen K, Lehtovirta M, Hallikainen M, Hänninen T, Helkala EL, Vainio P, Riekkinen PJ Sr: MRI of the hippocampus in Alzheimer’s disease: sensitivity, specificity, and analysis of the incorrectly classified subjects. Neurobiol Aging 1998; 19:23-31
10.
Visser PJ, Scheltens P, Verhey FRJ, Schmand B, Launer LJ, Jolles J, Jonker C: Medial temporal lobe atrophy and memory dysfunction as predictors for dementia in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. J Neurol 1999; 246:477-485
11.
Xu Y, Jack CR, O’Brien PC, Kokmen E, Smith GE, Ivnik RJ, Boeve BF, Tangalos RG, Petersen RC: Usefulness of MRI measures of entorhinal cortex versus hippocampus in AD. Neurology 2000; 54:1760-1767
12.
Du AT, Schuff N, Amend D, Laakso MP, Hsu YY, Jagust WJ, Yaffe K, Kramer JH, Reed B, Norman D, Chui HC, Weiner MW: Magnetic resonance imaging of the entorhinal cortex and hippocampus in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001; 71:441-447
13.
Crook T, Bartus RT, Ferris SH, Whitehouse P, Cohen GD, Gershon S: Age-associated memory impairment: proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change—report of a National Institute of Mental Health work group. Dev Neuropsychol 1986; 2:261-276
14.
Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, Ritchie K, Rossor M, Thal L, Winblad B: Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 2001; 58:1985-1992
15.
Levy R (Working Party of the International Psychogeriatric Association in collaboration with the World Health Organization): Aging-associated cognitive decline. Int Psychogeriatr 1994; 6:63-68
16.
Schröder J, Kratz B, Pantel J, Minnemann E, Lehr U, Sauer H: Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in an elderly community sample. J Neural Transm Suppl 1998; 54:51-59
17.
Martin P, Martin M: Design und Methodik der Interdisziplinären Längsschnittstudie des Erwachsenenalters, in Aspekte der Entwicklung im mittleren und höheren Lebensalter. Ergebnisse der Interdisziplinären Längsschnittstudie des Erwachsenenalters (ILSE). Edited by Martin P, Ettrich KU, Lehr U, Roether D, Martin M, Fischer-Cyrulies A. Darmstadt, Germany, Steinkopff, 2000, pp 17-27
18.
McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, Katzman R, Price D, Stadlan EM: Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of the Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984; 34:939-944
19.
Reisberg B, Ferris SH, De Leon MJ, Crook T: The Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. Am J Psychiatry 1982; 139:1136-1139
20.
Pantel J, Schröder J, Schad LR, Friedlinger M, Knopp MV, Schmitt R, Geissler M, Blüml S, Essig M, Sauer H: Quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological functions in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Psychol Med 1997; 27:221-229
21.
Pantel J, Schönknecht P, Essig M, Amann M, Eysenbach K, Schad LR, Schröder J: Progressive medial temporal lobe changes in Alzheimer’s disease revealed by quantitative MRI: potential use for monitoring of drug-related changes. Drug Dev Res 2002; 56:51-56
22.
Mitchell TW, Mufson EJ, Schneider JA, Cochran EJ, Nissanow J, Han LY, Bienias JL, Lee VMY, Trojanowski JQ, Bennett DA, Arnold SE: Parahippocampal tau pathology in healthy aging, mild cognitive impairment, and early Alzheimer’s disease: Ann Neurol 2002; 51:182-189
23.
Ritchie K, Aartero S, Touchon J: Classification criteria for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based validation study. Neurology 2001; 56:37-42

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 379 - 382
PubMed: 12562591

History

Published online: 1 February 2003
Published in print: February 2003

Authors

Details

Johannes Pantel, M.D., Ph.D.
Johannes Schröder, M.D., Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share