Skip to main content
Full access
Perspectives
Published Online: 1 July 2011

The Search for Improved Antidepressant Strategies: Is Bigger Better?

Despite a proliferation of pharmaceutical options for the treatment of major depression over the past 20 years, reported remission rates among patients given antidepressants have remained stubbornly low. Clinicians confronted with an inadequate antidepressant response have four options open to them—a dose adjustment, a switch to an alternative antidepressant, the introduction of another drug not considered itself an antidepressant (augmentation), or the addition of another antidepressant. Unfortunately, controlled trials have yielded very limited support for the first, second, and last of these options, and evidence for augmentation strategies concerns chiefly the use of lithium or T3 with tricyclic antidepressants, rather than with the antidepressants now in wide use (1). Indeed, the Sequenced Treatment Alternatives to Relieve Depression (STAR*D) project revealed discouraging response rates when either of these was used in the face of inadequate response to a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) (2). There do now exist a number of large, industry-sponsored trials that have shown the addition of atypical antipsychotics to be helpful in the face of inadequate responses to SSRIs or to serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (SNRIs) (1). Their use, though, often entails adverse metabolic consequences and/or considerable added expense.
Given the low overall response rates seen among patients whose depression does not remit after one or more antidepressant trials (3, 4), might better outcomes be achieved by treatment that is more aggressive from the outset? Encouraging evidence exists for the treatment of other common and often refractory conditions in medicine, and in two recent trials by Blier et al., depressed patients whose treatment began with a combination of mirtazapine and paroxetine, fluoxetine, or bupropion experienced remission rates roughly twice those of patients who received fluoxetine or paroxetine monotherapy (5, 6). These findings set the stage for the Combining Medications to Enhance Depression Outcomes (CO-MED) comparison of escitalopram with placebo, escitalopram with bupropion, and venlafaxine with mirtazapine (7). Unfortunately, the results are satisfying chiefly in their clarity. No trend emerged to favor either combination therapy, and only the presence and severity of side effects significantly separated combination from monotherapy groups.
These findings complete an all too familiar sequence in which the encouraging results of small studies prompt a much larger and carefully designed effort, one that then ends with surprisingly negative conclusions.
Among the possible explanations considered for such disparities are variances in drug dosing and in trial duration, but these have, I believe, been credibly dismissed here. This leaves subject differences as the chief suspect. Did the CO-MED study group include far fewer individuals likely to respond preferentially to antidepressant regimens that affect multiple receptors?
At first glance, those who participated in the CO-MED study seemed as responsive to antidepressant treatment generally as did those described in the two reports by Blier et al.; pooled remission rates were 38.5% for CO-MED and 39.2% for Blier et al. The remission rate with monotherapy, however, was significantly higher in the CO-MED study than in the combined studies by Blier et al.: 38.8% versus 23.5%, respectively (χ2=5.4, df=1, p<0.03). The reverse was true for remission rates among those given combination treatments; these were 38.3% for the CO-MED group and 50.0% for the Blier et al. combined groups (χ2=4.6, df=1, p<0.05). To the extent that response differences between combination treatment and monotherapy are analogous to those between active drug and placebo, the reconciliation of findings hinges on characteristics of the CO-MED study group that are associated with smaller drug effects.
What might these characteristics be? Sizable literatures exist concerning the correlates both of placebo response and of antidepressant response in major depression. The latter, of course, is confounded by the fact that many perceived “drug responses” are, in reality, placebo responses. A more direct approach to the question of who is likely to benefit from the drug component of an administered antidepressant seeks to identify attributes associated with larger drug-placebo differences. Such studies are quite scarce, though, because they require the entry of large numbers of subjects into placebo-controlled trials, and such efforts have been largely consigned to industry. There, marketing considerations have discouraged designs that would identify subgroups for which the proprietary drug being tested would not be indicated.
The clinical trial database of the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) has permitted the clear establishment of one feature predictive of greater antidepressant-placebo differences: mean severity as measured by the scale common to nearly all antidepressant trials, the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D). It is notable, then, that of 10 placebo-controlled trials with a mean baseline HAM-D score resembling that of the CO-MED cohort, between 23 and 24, only one showed a significant treatment effect (8). The Blier et al. groups, though, had similar mean HAM-D scores at baseline, so if differences in illness severity played a role in the contrasting results, it is not a straightforward one.
Perhaps the HAM-D as administered across the CO-MED sites did not adequately capture the element of symptom severity most relevant to the emergence of clear drug effects. The CO-MED study group contained far fewer subjects with melancholia than did either of the groups of Blier et al., and depressed patients with melancholic features have been shown to have higher severity ratings on global and other symptom-based measures in comparison to those without melancholia, despite having nearly identical HAM-D scores (9). Group differences were only somewhat larger in the CO-MED subset with melancholic features than in the patients without them. The performance of melancholia as a response predictor, though, varies considerably by how its components are defined and applied (10), and the CO-MED report does not state how melancholic symptoms were assessed. The relative scarcity of melancholia in the CO-MED patients nevertheless indicates a correspondingly lower severity level on some dimension.
A particularly striking feature of the CO-MED study group is the coexistence of the absence of treatment resistance, as specified by the entry criteria, and a high rate of chronicity. Although none of the participants had had an adequate monotherapy trial of an FDA-approved antidepressant within the current episode, over one-half of the group had a depressive episode that had been fully syndromal for at least the preceding 2 years. Why had they not undergone even one adequate antidepressant trial before the CO-MED effort came to pass? Whatever the answer, it seems likely that the average interval between episode onset and the receipt of first treatment was quite long. A number of prospective studies of major depressive disorder have shown the no-treatment interval to be as robust a predictor of poor outcome with treatment as neuroticism (11). Why this is so is a matter of speculation, but the measure's association with poorer treatment response generally may well have narrowed differences between the regimens applied here.
Finally, there is the issue of study size. Difficulties in maintaining adherence to entry criteria and to standards with which diagnostic criteria and outcome measurements are applied necessarily grow with the number of sites involved in a trial. Such problems are likely to be compounded by competition among sites to meet recruitment goals. It is not surprising then that a review of multicenter antidepressant randomized controlled trials showed the number of sites to be the strongest determinant of placebo response rates and, therefore, of small effect sizes (12).
The truth, of course, may lie with the CO-MED trial, and the conclusions of Blier et al. may comprise false leads. The trade-offs necessary in the pursuit of statistical power through multiple sites are widely recognized, though, and several remedies should come into play. One is the use of centralized ratings to ensure standard applications of entry criteria and outcome measures. This would limit measurement error from intersite differences and rater drift and thus improve the odds of revealing true treatment effects. The other is an increasing focus on tailored treatment. It may indeed prove much more efficient, particularly in the case of such highly heterogeneous disorders as major depressive disorder, to identify subgroups that are expected, from mechanistic considerations, to show a preferential response to a new intervention and to then test this with small study groups before a targeted population is put to a larger and more definitive trial. This is not a new idea but is one that appears to be finally gathering momentum.

Footnote

Editorial accepted for publication March 2011.

References

1.
Connolly KR, Thase ME: If at first you don't succeed: a review of the evidence for antidepressant augmentation, combination and switching strategies. Drugs 2011; 71:43–64
2.
Nierenberg AA, Fava M, Trivedi MH, Wisniewski SR, Thase ME, McGrath PJ, Alpert JE, Warden D, Luther JF, Niederehe G, Lebowitz B, Shores-Wilson K, Rush AJ STAR*D Study Team: A comparison of lithium and T3 augmentation following two failed medication treatments for depression: a STAR*D report. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:1519–1530
3.
Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Wisniewski SR, Stewart JW, Nierenberg AA, Thase ME, Ritz L, Biggs MM, Warden D, Luther JF, Shores-Wilson K, Niederehe G, Fava M: Bupropion-SR, sertraline, or venlafaxine-XR after failure of SSRIs for depression. N Engl J Med 2006; 354:1231–1242
4.
Trivedi MH, Fava M, Wisniewski SR, Thase ME, Quitkin F, Warden D, Ritz L, Nierenberg AA, Lebowitz BD, Biggs MM, Luther JF, Shores-Wilson K, Rush AJ: Medication augmentation after the failure of SSRIs for depression. N Engl J Med 2006; 354:1243–1252
5.
Blier P, Gobbi G, Turcotte JE, de Montigny C, Boucher N, Hebert C, Debonnel G: Mirtazapine and paroxetine in major depression: a comparison of monotherapy versus their combination from treatment initiation. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2009; 19:457–465
6.
Blier P, Ward HE, Tremblay P, Laberge L, Hébert C, Bergeron R: Combination of antidepressant medications from treatment initiation for major depressive disorder: a double-blind randomized study. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:281–288
7.
Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Stewart JW, Nierenberg AA, Fava M, Kurian BT, Warden D, Morris DW, Luther JF, Husain MM, Cook IA, Shelton RC, Lesser IM, Kornstein SG, Wisniewski SR: Combining Medications to Enhance Depression Outcomes (CO-MED): acute and long-term outcomes of a single-blind randomized study. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168:689–701
8.
Khan A, Leventhal RM, Khan SR, Brown WA: Severity of depression and response to antidepressants and placebo: an analysis of the Food and Drug Administration database. J Clin Psychopharmacol 2002; 22:40–45
9.
Zimmerman M, Black DW, Coryell W: Diagnostic criteria for melancholia: the comparative validity of DSM-III and DSM-III-R. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989; 46:361–368
10.
Parker G, Roy K, Wilhelm K, Mitchell P: Assessing the comparative effectiveness of antidepressant therapies: a prospective clinical practice study. J Clin Psychiatry 2001; 62:117–125
11.
Gormley N, O'Leary D, Costello F: First admissions for depression: is the 'no-treatment interval' a critical predictor of time to remission? J Affect Disord 1999; 54:49–54
12.
Bridge JA, Birmaher B, Iyengar S, Barbe RP, Brent DA: Placebo response in randomized controlled trials of antidepressants for pediatric major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:42–49

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 664 - 666
PubMed: 21724669

History

Accepted: March 2011
Published online: 1 July 2011
Published in print: July 2011

Authors

Details

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Coryell ([email protected]).

Funding Information

The author reports no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share