Skip to main content
Full access
New Research
Published Online: 1 January 2014

Neuropsychological Decline in Schizophrenia From the Premorbid to the Postonset Period: Evidence From a Population-Representative Longitudinal Study

Abstract

Objective

Despite the widespread belief that neuropsychological decline is a cardinal feature of the progression from the premorbid stage to the chronic form of schizophrenia, few longitudinal studies have examined change in neuropsychological functioning from before to after illness onset. The authors examined whether neuropsychological decline is unique to schizophrenia, whether it is generalized or confined to particular mental functions, and whether individuals with schizophrenia also have cognitive problems in everyday life.

Method

Participants were members of a representative cohort of 1,037 individuals born in Dunedin, New Zealand, in 1972 and 1973 and followed prospectively to age 38, with 95% retention. Assessment of IQ and specific neuropsychological functions was conducted at ages 7, 9, 11, and 13, and again at age 38. Informants also reported on any cognitive problems at age 38.

Results

Individuals with schizophrenia exhibited declines in IQ and in a range of mental functions, particularly those tapping processing speed, learning, executive function, and motor function. There was little evidence of decline in verbal abilities or delayed memory, however, and the developmental progression of deficits in schizophrenia differed across mental functions. Processing speed deficits increased gradually from childhood to beyond the early teen years, whereas verbal deficits emerged early but remained static thereafter. Neuropsychological decline was specific to schizophrenia, as no evidence of decline was apparent among individuals with persistent depression, children with mild cognitive impairment, individuals matched on childhood risk factors for schizophrenia, and psychiatrically healthy individuals. Informants also noticed more cognitive problems in individuals with schizophrenia.

Conclusions

There is substantial neuropsychological decline in schizophrenia from the premorbid to the postonset period, but the extent and developmental progression of decline varies across mental functions. Findings suggest that different pathophysiological mechanisms may underlie deficits in different mental functions.
Neuropsychological impairment is a core feature of schizophrenia (1), and understanding the nature and course of neuropsychological functioning in schizophrenia may have important pathophysiological implications. It is widely believed that individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia experience neuropsychological decline from the premorbid to the postonset period, but relatively few studies have examined change in neuropsychological functioning from before to after the onset of schizophrenia. In this study, we provide a rigorous test of neuropsychological changes in schizophrenia using a battery of tests administered during childhood (ages 7, 9, 11, and 13) and adulthood (age 38) as part of an ongoing population-representative longitudinal study.
There is clear evidence of mild neuropsychological deficits among children who later develop schizophrenia (2). Neuropsychological deficits are even more pronounced among adults diagnosed with schizophrenia. For example, meta-analyses show an average premorbid 8-point IQ deficit (SD=0.50) among individuals who later develop schizophrenia (3) but a 14- to 21-point IQ deficit (SD=0.90–1.40) among first-episode and chronic schizophrenia patients (1, 4, 5). These findings suggest that individuals with schizophrenia experience a relative decline in neuropsychological functioning over time from before to after illness onset, with stabilization in neuropsychological functioning thereafter (6, 7), or at least until older adulthood (810).
In line with cross-sectional evidence, the few longitudinal studies that have addressed neuropsychological changes in schizophrenia from before to after illness onset have consistently shown evidence of neuropsychological decline (Table 1). However, these studies suffer from various limitations. First, the majority of the studies are based on clinical samples, which may not be representative of the full population of individuals with schizophrenia (22). Second, the age at baseline assessment varies considerably, with many studies assessing neuropsychological functioning for the first time in adolescence or adulthood, when prodromal symptoms (and altered neuropsychological functioning) tend to be present (2325). Thus, these studies may underestimate the magnitude of the decline in functioning. Third, only five of the studies included a comparison group, which is needed to provide a rigorous test of change in functioning. Fourth, many of these studies employed different neuropsychological tests across time, making it difficult to ascertain true change in functioning. Fifth, these studies focused exclusively on IQ (or IQ proxies). Since different neural systems underlie performance on different neuropsychological tests, other important mental functions, such as memory and executive function, should be examined as well. Sixth, none of the studies examined whether, in addition to poor IQ test performance, individuals with schizophrenia experience cognitive problems in their daily life.
TABLE 1. Characteristics of Studies Assessing Neuropsychological Decline From Before to After the Onset of Schizophreniaa
StudyN (Cases)Baseline Age or Age Range (Years)Follow-Up Age or Age Range (Years)Comparison GroupSame Test Across TimeNeuropsychological TestsFinding
Kremen et al. (11)105 or 9Late 30sYesYesPeabody Picture Vocabulary TestRelative decline
Seidman et al. (12)26735YesYesTwo IQ subtestsDecline
Caspi et al. (13)4416–1720sYesYesArmy Induction TestsDecline
Schwartzman and Douglas (14)5020s30sYesYesArmy Induction TestsDecline
Bilder et al. (15)391720sYesNoScholastic Aptitude Test, IQDecline
Gochman et al. (16)18ChildhoodAdolescence, adulthoodNoYesIQDecline
Russell et al. (17)348–2517–59NoYesIQNo decline
Lubin et al. (18)1591818–51NoYesArmy Induction TestsDecline
Sheitman et al. (19)279–1718–60NoNoVarious IQ testsDecline
Rappaport and Webb (20)10Adolescence15–28NoYes, nobVarious IQ testsDecline
Albee et al. (21)112ChildhoodAdulthoodNoNoVarious IQ testsNo decline
a
Studies are ordered by methodology and date, with the most methodologically rigorous and recent studies listed first. The first two studies (11, 12) used epidemiological samples; all the others used clinical samples.
b
The same test was used across time within participants, but not between participants.
In a previous report of our population-representative cohort followed prospectively from birth, we showed that children who later developed schizophrenia had IQ deficits, and we mapped changes in the specific mental functions that constitute the IQ across four measurement occasions from ages 7 to 13 years (26). Now that this cohort has been followed to age 38 and undergone additional neuropsychological testing, we examined change in IQ, as well as more specific neuropsychological functions, from before (ages 7–13) to after (age 38) the onset of schizophrenia using the same measures across time. In the present study, we tested four hypotheses. First, we tested the “IQ decline” hypothesis to determine whether individuals with schizophrenia experience a decline in IQ from before to after illness onset. We compared the group of individuals with schizophrenia to a psychiatrically healthy group to allow for an accurate interpretation of test-retest performance. Second, we tested the “generalized decline” hypothesis to determine whether decline is apparent broadly across different neuropsychological domains: verbal IQ, performance IQ, learning and memory, processing speed, executive function, and motor function. Third, we tested the “specificity” hypothesis to address whether neuropsychological decline is specific to schizophrenia. We compared neuropsychological decline in schizophrenia to decline in three other groups: a persistent depression group, a mild cognitive impairment group, and a group at risk for schizophrenia. We evaluated neuropsychological decline in individuals with persistent depression to test whether decline is common to other psychiatric disorders. Depression is characterized by neuropsychological impairment (27, 28), but it is not clear whether there is neuropsychological decline from before to after illness onset. We evaluated neuropsychological decline in children with mild cognitive impairment because they, like children with schizophrenia, exhibit cognitive difficulties early in life. However, unlike in schizophrenia, these children do not develop a psychotic condition. We also evaluated neuropsychological decline in “at-risk” individuals who did not develop schizophrenia but who matched those who did on key childhood risk factors (low IQ, family history of psychotic illness, low socioeconomic status). Fourth, we queried third-party informants to test the “everyday cognition” hypothesis that individuals with schizophrenia experience cognitive problems in daily life.

Method

Participants

Participants are members of the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study, a longitudinal investigation of the health and behavior of a complete birth cohort of consecutive births between April 1, 1972, and March 31, 1973, in Dunedin, New Zealand. The cohort of 1,037 children (91% of eligible births; 52% boys) was constituted at age 3 years. Cohort families represent the full range of socioeconomic status in the general population of New Zealand’s South Island and are primarily of white European ancestry. Follow-up assessments were conducted at ages 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, 26, 32, and, most recently, 38 years, when 95% of the 1,007 living study members underwent assessment in the period 2010–2012.
The study protocol was approved by the institutional ethical review boards of the participating universities, and study members gave informed consent before participating.

Schizophrenia

Schizophrenia was assessed at ages 21, 26, 32, and 38. We previously described the schizophrenia cases up to age 32 (26, 29, 30), and here we update this information with data from age 38. DSM criteria for schizophrenia were assessed at each age with the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) (31, 32). We took several steps to enhance the validity of our research diagnosis. First, we required the presence of hallucinations (not substance use-related) in addition to at least two other positive symptoms. This requirement is stricter than that of DSM-IV, which does not require hallucinations, although requiring them has been shown to reduce overdiagnosis (33). Second, because self-reports can be compromised by poor insight in schizophrenia, we required objective evidence of impairment resulting from psychosis, as reported by informants and as recorded in the study’s life-history calendars, which document continuous histories of employment and relationships. Third, in our research, the DIS is administered by experienced clinicians, not lay interviewers. These clinicians record detailed case notes. Our staff also rate observable symptoms manifested in affect, grooming, and speech during the full day that participants spend at our research unit. Fourth, participants bring their medications, which are then classified by a pharmacist. Fifth, informants report study members’ positive and negative psychotic symptoms via postal questionnaires. Finally, study members’ parents were interviewed about their adult child’s psychotic symptoms and treatment as part of the Dunedin Family Health History Study (2003–2005). These data, accumulated in the Dunedin study at ages 21, 26, 32, and 38, were compiled into dossiers reviewed by four clinicians to achieve best-estimate diagnoses with 100% consensus. By age 38, 2% of the cohort (N=20) met criteria for schizophrenia and had, according to the multisource information collected in the dossiers, been hospitalized for schizophrenia (totaling 1,396 days of psychiatric hospitalization, according to official New Zealand administrative record searches) or received prescriptions for antipsychotic medications. An additional 1.7% (N=17) met all criteria for schizophrenia, had hallucinations, and suffered significant life impairment but had not, to our knowledge, been treated specifically for psychotic illness. Together, these two groups constituted a total of 37 cases of diagnosed schizophrenia in the cohort. Of these 37 individuals, four died before the age-38 neuropsychological assessment and two declined to participate, leaving an effective group size of 31 for this study.
Of the 31 individuals in the schizophrenia group, the majority (N=17; 55%) had received treatment specifically for psychotic illness. Of those who, to our knowledge, had not received treatment specifically for psychotic illness, nearly all reported receiving treatment for another mental health problem (Table 2). The two groups appeared similar on a variety of correlates, including adult IQ, personality functioning, substance dependence, and even receipt of government benefits, suggesting that the groups are comparably impaired. The notable exception was that those who had not received treatment for psychotic illness were from families with lower socioeconomic status.
TABLE 2. Comparison of Cohort Members Diagnosed With Schizophrenia Who Had or Had Not Received Treatment for Psychotic Illness
Measure (Age at Assessment)Treated for Psychosis (N=17)Not Treated for Psychosis (N=14)Cohort Norm (Na=942–1,031)
 %95% CI%95% CI%
Mental health treatment (20–38 years)     
 Received treatment for a mental health problem100.00100.00, 100.0085.71b57.19, 98.2246.29
 Hospitalized for a mental health problem58.8232.92, 81.5621.434.66, 50.807.33
Received government benefits (26–38 years)88.2463.56, 98.5492.8666.13, 99.8242.01
Persistent substance dependence (18–38 years)     
 Tobacco dependence52.9427.81, 77.0264.2935.14, 87.2424.13
 Alcohol dependence29.4110.31, 55.9628.578.39, 58.1014.96
 Cannabis dependence23.536.81, 49.9028.578.39, 58.108.87
 Hard drug dependence (26–38 years)0.000.00, 0.007.140.18, 33.872.58
 Mean95% CIMean95% CIMean
Full-scale IQ (38 years)87.1678.03, 96.2988.8579.12, 98.57100.00
Childhood socioeconomic statusc (birth–15 years)–0.02–0.54, 0.50–0.57–1.12, –0.010.00
Informant-reported personalityc (26–38 years)     
 Agreeableness–0.78–1.50, –0.06–1.07–1.51, –0.630.00
 Conscientiousness–0.64–1.08, –0.20–1.00–1.75, –0.260.00
 Extraversion–0.44–1.04, 0.15–0.49–1.29, 0.320.00
 Neuroticism0.930.46, 1.410.960.39, 1.520.00
 Openness0.06–0.57, 0.68–0.20–0.96, 0.570.00
a
Ns varied because of different amounts of missing data across study measures.
b
Based on self-report.
c
Standardized to cohort (mean=0.00, SD=1.00).
The cohort’s 3.7% prevalence rate of schizophrenia is high and should be understood in the context of three methodological aspects of our study. First, our birth cohort, with a 95% participation rate, allows us to count psychotic individuals overlooked by previous surveys because individuals with psychotic disorders often decline to participate in surveys or die prematurely (34), and surveys often exclude homeless or institutionalized individuals with psychosis. Second, our cohort members are all from one city in the South Island of New Zealand. It is possible, given geographical variation in rates of schizophrenia (3537), that the prevalence is somewhat elevated there. No data exist to compare prevalence rates of schizophrenia in New Zealand to rates in other countries, but the high prevalence of suicide in New Zealand could be consistent with an elevated prevalence of severe mental health conditions (38). Third, our research diagnoses did not make fine-grained distinctions among psychotic disorders (e.g., schizophrenia versus schizoaffective disorder). Thus, the cohort members diagnosed with schizophrenia here might not be considered by all clinicians to have schizophrenia. We note, however, that over half of those we diagnosed were confirmed by receipt of treatment. Moreover, etiological mechanisms appear to be similar across the continuum of psychosis (39).

Persistent Depression

Depression was assessed by DSM criteria at ages 18, 21, 26, 32, and 38 using the DIS. Cohort members who were diagnosed with depression on two or more occasions between ages 18 and 38 were classified into the persistent depression group. We focused on persistent depression in an effort to make this group more comparable to the schizophrenia group in terms of chronicity and severity of illness. Neuropsychological data were incomplete for six of the 191 cohort members in the persistent depression group, leaving an effective group size of 185.

Mild Cognitive Impairment

Individuals with a childhood IQ (averaged across ages 7–13) in the range of 80–89 were considered to have mild cognitive impairment (N=120).

Neuropsychological Functioning

We assessed neuropsychological functioning using tests of IQ, learning and memory, processing speed, executive function, and motor function. Full-scale IQ can be thought of as an omnibus measure of general intellectual ability, because it captures overall ability across differentiable components of intellectual functioning (i.e., verbal IQ and performance IQ). Verbal and performance IQ can be further “unpacked” to make finer-grained distinctions in ability. Learning and memory, processing speed, executive function, and motor function represent even more basic mental functions.
IQ was assessed in childhood at ages 7, 9, 11 and 13 (before the onset of schizophrenia) and again in adulthood at age 38. We report a comparison of scores on the WISC-R (40) and the WAIS-IV (41). Full-scale, verbal, and performance IQ were standardized to population norms with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15; subtest scaled scores were standardized to population norms with a mean of 10 and a standard deviation of 3. Learning and memory, processing speed, executive function, and motor function were each assessed at ages 13 and 38 using, respectively, the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (42), the Trail Making Test (43), and the Grooved Pegboard Test (42). (For details about each test, see Table S1 in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article.)

Informant-Reported Cognitive Problems

Informant reports of study members’ cognitive function were obtained at age 38. Study members nominated people “who knew them well.” These informants were mailed questionnaires and asked to complete a checklist, including whether the study member had problems with his or her attention and memory over the past year. The informant-reported attention problems scale consisted of four items: “Is easily distracted, gets sidetracked easily,” “Can’t concentrate, mind wanders,” “Tunes out instead of focusing,” and “Has difficulty organizing tasks that have many steps” (internal consistency reliability=0.79). The informant-reported memory problems scale consisted of three items: “Has problems with memory,” “Misplaces wallet, keys, eyeglasses, paperwork,” and “Forgets to do errands, return calls, pay bills” (internal consistency reliability=0.64). Informant-reported cognitive problems (attention and memory problems combined) were correlated with adult full-scale IQ (r=−0.22, p<0.0001).

Control Variables

DSM cannabis and alcohol dependence were assessed at ages 18, 21, 26, 32, and 38, and DSM hard drug (e.g., heroin, cocaine, amphetamines) dependence was assessed at ages 26, 32, and 38. Study members who were diagnosed with dependence at two or more assessments were considered persistently dependent on these substances.

Statistical Analysis

We compared the schizophrenia and persistent depression groups to a healthy group (a group of individuals in the cohort who had no current psychiatric disorder; N=518) on change in neuropsychological functioning from childhood to adulthood. Change scores were created by subtracting the childhood test score (averaged across ages 7–13 for the IQ tests and subtests) from the adulthood test score. Negative scores indicate neuropsychological decline. In Tables 4 and 5, childhood and adulthood test scores as well as change scores are presented in standard deviation units (mean=0.00, SD=1.00). Standardized scores reflect effect sizes for how different each group is from the cohort norm. Differences between pairs of groups can also be interpreted as effect sizes. Effect sizes of 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80 reflect small, medium, and large effects, respectively (44). Statistical tests involved planned orthogonal comparisons of each psychiatric group to the healthy group and were adjusted for sex, although results were unchanged when sex was excluded from the model.

Results

IQ Over Time in Schizophrenia

Figure 1A shows that, consistent with findings from meta-analyses, the schizophrenia group evidenced a 9-point IQ deficit in childhood relative to the healthy group, and a 15-point deficit in adulthood. The greater relative deficit in adulthood was due to an average 6-point IQ decline in the schizophrenia group from childhood to adulthood (from a mean of 93.63 in childhood to 87.92 in adulthood). There was no evidence of IQ decline for the healthy group (from 102.71 in childhood to 102.44 in adulthood), and IQ decline was significantly greater in the schizophrenia group than in the healthy group (p=0.0009). IQ decline in the schizophrenia group was not attributable to current antipsychotic medication use, as an almost 6-point IQ decline (from 95.29 in childhood to 89.88 in adulthood; paired t=2.83, p=0.0094) was still apparent among the 24 individuals who did not use antipsychotic medication during the year before adult neuropsychological testing.
FIGURE 1. IQ in Childhood and Adulthood in Individuals With Schizophrenia, Persistent Depression, or Mild Cognitive Impairment and Healthy Comparison Subjectsa
a Panel A shows full-scale IQ in childhood (averaged across ages 7–13) and adulthood (age 38) for the healthy, schizophrenia, and persistent-depression groups. There was a statistically significant difference (paired t=3.29, p=0.003) between childhood and adulthood IQ for members of the schizophrenia group. Panel B shows full-scale IQ in childhood (ages 7, 9, 11, and 13) and adulthood (age 38) for the healthy, schizophrenia, and persistent-depression groups as well as a group of children with mild cognitive impairment. IQ was stable across childhood for all groups but declined for the schizophrenia group between ages 13 and 38. Error bars indicate standard error.

Specificity of IQ Decline to Schizophrenia

Relative to the healthy group, the persistent depression group showed a statistically significant 3-point IQ deficit in both childhood and adulthood (Figure 1A). There was no evidence of IQ decline for the persistent depression group (from 99.30 in childhood to 99.05 in adulthood), and estimates of IQ change for the persistent depression and healthy groups did not differ.
Figure 1B expands the analysis to show that full-scale IQ was relatively stable for the schizophrenia, persistent depression, and healthy groups from ages 7 to 13 but dropped substantially for the schizophrenia group between ages 13 and 38. As a further comparison, we also evaluated IQ decline from childhood to adulthood among children with mild cognitive impairment and children at risk for developing schizophrenia. Children with mild cognitive impairment are of interest because, like children with schizophrenia, they exhibit cognitive difficulties early in life, but unlike those with schizophrenia, they do not develop a psychotic condition. Figure 1B shows that, in contrast to children who develop schizophrenia, children with mild cognitive impairment did not show evidence of IQ decline.
Next, we used data from our cohort to match schizophrenia cases to “at-risk” individuals who did not develop schizophrenia but who shared key childhood risk factors with schizophrenia cases. Low IQ, low socioeconomic status, and a family history of schizophrenia are well-established risk factors for developing schizophrenia (4547). We used propensity score matching to identify individuals who matched our schizophrenia cases on these risk factors but did not develop psychotic illness (Table 3). IQ decline was not apparent among individuals at risk for schizophrenia. The at-risk group, with a childhood liability to develop schizophrenia similar to that of members of the schizophrenia group, showed an IQ decline of 0.01 standard deviations, whereas the schizophrenia group showed a decline of 0.39 standard deviations. Thus, at-risk children who did not develop schizophrenia showed less IQ decline than their matched counterparts who did (F=8.57, df=1, 91, p=0.0043). In sum, IQ decline was unique to those diagnosed with schizophrenia.
TABLE 3. Mean Scores on Childhood Risk Factors for Cohort Members Who Did (Case) or Did Not (Control) Develop Schizophrenia, Before and After Propensity Score Matchinga
 Before MatchingAfter Matching
Childhood Risk Factor (Age at Assessment)Case Subjects (N=31)Control Subjects (N=875)Standardized Bias (%)Case Subjects (N=31)Control Subjects (N=62)Standardized Bias (%)
Full-scale IQ (7–13 years)93.63101.054693.6393.890
Performance IQ (7–13 years)94.64100.973994.6494.800
Verbal IQ (7–13 years)93.64101.044093.6494.032
Family history of psychotic illnessb0.25–0.01230.250.29–3
Socioeconomic statusb (birth–15 years)–0.270.0431–0.27–0.31–5
Average standardized bias  36  –1
a
We used propensity score matching to perform the match using SAS (SAS, Inc., Cary, N.C.). First, we obtained propensity scores via a logistic regression predicting likelihood of developing schizophrenia based on the childhood correlates listed here. Next, we performed an optimal 2-to-1 (control-to-case) match on these propensity scores, whereby matches were made on the basis of the absolute difference in propensity score between case and control subjects. The maximum absolute difference was set to 0.10. Results were similar across models with different match parameters (i.e., varied number of case subjects matched to control subjects, varied maximum absolute difference). A total of 906 study members (31 case subjects and 875 control subjects) were available for matching, as analyses required nonmissing values for all childhood risk factors. The table shows that matching resulted in a high degree of similarity in the distributions of the childhood risk factors across case and control subjects, as standardized bias after matching was below 10 for each risk factor. Negative standardized bias values indicate higher risk in the control group.
b
Standardized to cohort (mean=0.00, SD=1.00).

Decline Across Mental Functions

Table 4 lists test scores (in standard deviation units) in childhood and adulthood for the healthy, schizophrenia, and persistent depression groups on a range of mental functions: IQ (and the subtests of different cognitive abilities that constitute the IQ), learning (total recall on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test), delayed memory (delayed recall on the Rey test), processing speed (Trails A), executive function (Trails B), and motor function (Grooved Pegboard Test). Table 4 also indicates the change in test performance (in standard deviation units) from childhood to adulthood.
TABLE 4. Neuropsychological Decline Across Different Mental Functions in Individuals With Schizophrenia or Persistent Depression and Healthy Comparison Subjectsa
 Healthy Group (N=518)Schizophrenia Group (N=31)Persistent Depression Group (N=185)
Neuropsychological TestChildAdultΔChildAdultΔChildAdultΔ
Full-scale IQ0.180.16–0.02–0.42b–0.81b–0.39b–0.04b–0.06b–0.02
Performance IQ0.150.150.00–0.36b–0.90b–0.54b–0.02b–0.06b–0.04
Digit symbol coding subtest0.090.100.01–0.22–0.98b–0.76b0.090.02b–0.07
Block design subtest0.140.09–0.05–0.25b–0.63b–0.38b–0.02–0.010.01
Picture completion subtest0.120.10–0.02–0.44b–0.63b–0.19–0.04–0.030.01
Verbal IQ0.170.14–0.03–0.42b–0.67b–0.25–0.05b–0.06–0.01
Information subtest0.170.14–0.03–0.28b–0.33b–0.05–0.05–0.11–0.06
Similarities subtest0.140.07–0.07–0.42b–0.45b–0.03–0.010.120.13b
Vocabulary subtest0.160.11–0.05–0.36b–0.48b–0.12–0.04b0.010.05
Arithmetic subtest0.180.15–0.03–0.45b–0.66b–0.21–0.08b–0.16b–0.08
Rey total recall0.080.05–0.03–0.41b–0.88b–0.47b0.060.100.04
Rey delayed recall0.080.06–0.02–0.37b–0.38b–0.010.060.100.04
Trails A (time in seconds)c–0.01–0.07–0.060.240.73b0.49b0.060.01–0.05
Trails B (time in seconds)c–0.10–0.11–0.010.69b1.21b0.52b0.08b–0.05–0.13
Grooved Pegboard Test (time in seconds)c–0.07–0.11–0.040.37b1.04b0.67b0.01–0.04b–0.05
a
The table lists mean childhood and adulthood neuropsychological test scores (in standard deviation units) for each group. The change column (Δ) indicates the change in test scores (in standard deviation units) from childhood to adulthood. Statistical tests are adjusted for sex. Ns for the IQ subtests range from 514 to 518 for the healthy group, 30 to 31 for the schizophrenia group, and 184 to 185 for the persistent depression group. A subset of cohort members completed IQ testing in childhood but did not complete the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, the Trail Making Test (parts A and B), or the Grooved Pegboard Test in childhood; for these tests, the Ns are 384–393 for the healthy group, 21–22 for the schizophrenia group, and 138–140 for the persistent depression group. There were no significant differences in IQ decline between those who completed these tests and those who did not, either for the cohort as a whole or for the healthy, schizophrenia, or persistent depression groups.
b
Significant difference (p<0.05) compared with the healthy group.
c
Higher scores on these tests indicate worse performance. Positive change scores on these tests reflect decline in performance from childhood to adulthood.
Relative to the healthy group, the schizophrenia group showed significantly greater decline on all mental functions except verbal IQ and delayed memory (Rey delayed recall). Inspection of the means suggests that the greatest declines occurred for processing speed (digit symbol coding, Trails A), learning (Rey total recall), executive function (Trails B), and motor function (Grooved Pegboard Test). By comparison, the persistent depression group generally did not show neuropsychological decline on any mental function.
Figure 2 expands the analysis to show that the progression of neuropsychological deficits from ages 7 to 38 varies across mental functions. Given our earlier report that children who later develop schizophrenia show developmental lags from ages 7 to 13 in processing speed, working memory, and attention but static deficits in verbal abilities (26), we elected to show results for the two IQ subtests most representative of these processes: the digit symbol coding test and the similarities test. Figure 2A shows that deficits on the digit symbol coding test were not apparent at age 7 but emerged gradually from ages 7 to 38. Figure 2B shows that deficits on the similarities test emerged by age 7 but remained relatively stable from ages 7 to 38.
FIGURE 2. Scaled Scores on Subtests of the WISC-R and WAIS-IV IQ in Individuals With Schizophrenia, Persistent Depression, or Mild Cognitive Impairment and Healthy Comparison Subjectsa
a Panel A shows scaled scores (population mean=10.00, SD=3.00) on the Wechsler digit symbol coding subtest from age 7 to age 38. The deficit among individuals with schizophrenia increased from ages 7 to 38. Panel B shows scaled scores (population mean=10.00, SD = 3.00) on the Wechsler similarities subtest from age 7 to age 38. The deficit among individuals with schizophrenia was apparent as early as age 7 and remained relatively stable from age 7 to age 38. Error bars indicate standard error.

Possible Alternative Explanations for the Neuropsychological Decline

We ruled out three alternative explanations for the observed association between schizophrenia and neuropsychological decline, namely, that these effects could be explained by cannabis dependence, alcohol dependence, or hard drug dependence. We recalculated the mean change in full-scale IQ for the healthy, schizophrenia, and persistent depression groups, excluding individuals with each form of substance dependence. We elected to show results only for full-scale IQ for this analysis because this measure captures overall intellectual functioning. Table 5 shows that excluding individuals with each form of substance dependence did not alter the initial finding; effect sizes, representing within-person IQ change, remained virtually the same and remained statistically significant. Moreover, even after statistically adjusting IQ change scores for cannabis, alcohol, and hard drug dependence conjointly, members of the schizophrenia group still showed a significantly greater IQ decline than members of the healthy group (mean decline, 0.34 standard deviations and 0.03 standard deviations, respectively; p=0.005).
TABLE 5. Ruling Out Alternative Explanations for the Observed Neuropsychological Decline in Schizophreniaa
 Healthy GroupSchizophrenia GroupPersistent Depression Group
TestΔ IQNΔ IQNpΔ IQNp
Full-scale IQ–0.02517–0.3931<0.001–0.021850.98
Excluding those with:        
 Persistent cannabis dependence–0.01497–0.41230.0020.011620.72
 Persistent alcohol dependence–0.01476–0.32220.0110.001410.86
 Persistent hard drug dependence–0.01512–0.37300.001–0.011780.89
a
Mean change in IQ from childhood to adulthood is listed in standard deviation units. Statistical tests compare each psychiatric group to the healthy group and are adjusted for sex.

Impairment in Everyday Cognition

Table 6 summarizes ratings of informant-reported attention and memory problems (in standardized units) for each group at age 38. Members of the schizophrenia group were rated 1.00 and 0.95 standard deviations above the cohort mean on attention and memory problems, respectively, whereas members of the healthy group were rated 0.18 and 0.16 standard deviations below the mean. By comparison, informants reported less pronounced attention and memory problems for the persistent depression group.
TABLE 6. Cognitive Impairment in Everyday Life in Individuals With Schizophrenia or Persistent Depression and Healthy Comparison Subjectsa
 Healthy Group (N=508)Schizophrenia Group (N=30)Persistent Depression Group (N=182)
Informant-Reported Cognitive ProblemMeanMeanpMeanp
Attention problems–0.181.00<0.0010.11<0.001
Memory problems–0.160.95<0.0010.09<0.001
a
Age-38 informant-reported attention and memory problem ratings are listed in standard deviation units. Statistical tests compare each psychiatric group to the healthy group and are adjusted for sex.

Discussion

This study provides evidence of neuropsychological decline in schizophrenia from before to after illness onset in a population-based birth cohort of individuals followed prospectively from birth to age 38. This finding is consistent with previous longitudinal studies showing evidence of neuropsychological decline in schizophrenia (1116, 1820).
This study advances knowledge in several ways. First, previous longitudinal studies have focused almost exclusively on IQ. We showed that individuals with schizophrenia experienced declines in IQ as well as in a range of different mental functions, particularly those tapping processing speed, learning, executive function, and motor function. Decline was greatest on the digit symbol coding test, which is consistent with research suggesting that this test, more so than other neuropsychological tests, taps a core impairment in schizophrenia and may reflect network integration problems (48). Decline was not ubiquitous across all mental functions, however. There was little evidence of decline in verbal IQ or delayed memory. Impaired verbal IQ and delayed memory among cohort adults diagnosed with schizophrenia could be traced back to childhood deficits that remained relatively stable across development. These findings highlight the importance of “unpacking” measures of generalized intellectual functioning, such as IQ, into more specific mental functions.
Second, we showed that neuropsychological decline was relatively specific to schizophrenia, as there was no evidence of decline among individuals in key comparison groups: children with mild cognitive impairment, at-risk children who did not develop schizophrenia, and individuals diagnosed with persistent depression. Research on the association between depression and neuropsychological decline has focused mainly on older adults and has yielded inconsistent findings, with some studies finding no association between depression and accelerated neuropsychological decline (49, 50) and others finding a positive association (51). Ours is the first study, to our knowledge, to examine depression-associated changes in neuropsychological functioning from before to after illness onset in a relatively young cohort. Notably, in our study, individuals with persistent depression performed worse than healthy individuals on a handful of neuropsychological tests in adulthood and were rated by informants as having more cognitive problems as adults than healthy individuals. Neuropsychological test deficits, however, were apparent from childhood, consistent with the interpretation that lower IQ constitutes a risk for depression (52, 53).
Third, our findings suggest that neuropsychological decline in individuals with schizophrenia is nontrivial. Estimates of decline ranged from 1/3 to 3/4 of a standard deviation unit more than average for the healthy group on tests tapping processing speed, learning, attention, working memory, and motor function. Moreover, cognitive impairment in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia was apparent in everyday life, as third-party informants noticed substantially more attention and memory problems in adults diagnosed with schizophrenia.
The results of this study should be viewed in the context of its limitations. First, although we found evidence of neuropsychological decline in schizophrenia from before to after illness onset, we could not fully map the developmental progression of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia from childhood to adulthood (funding agencies were unwilling to support repeated intellectual testing between ages 13 and 38). Nonetheless, we examined how deficits progressed from ages 7 to 13 for different mental functions and linked these deficits to data obtained at age 38. Deficits on the digit symbol coding test were not apparent at age 7 but increased gradually from ages 7 to 13, and by age 38, individuals with schizophrenia scored 1.08 standard deviations below the healthy group on this test. Notably, in an earlier report of the ages 7–13 neuropsychological test data (26), we showed that children who would later develop schizophrenia exhibited slowed growth in performance on the digit symbol coding test, and based on this trajectory of slowed growth, we predicted the adulthood deficit of >1 standard deviation that we report here. This suggests that the “decline” in processing speed that we observed reflects a gradual, progressive process of slowed growth in this mental function that begins in childhood and continues beyond the early teen years. Exactly when in childhood the deficit in processing speed becomes evident is difficult to pinpoint, as at least two other studies have reported statistically significant deficits on the digit symbol coding test at approximately age 7 (12, 54). Conversely, we showed that deficits on the similarities test emerged early but remained relatively static from age 7 through midlife. These findings imply that different pathophysiological mechanisms underlie the various neuropsychological deficits observed in schizophrenia patients.
A second limitation is that our findings are based on a relatively small group of individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia. The small group size prevented us from conducting an in-depth exploration of heterogeneity in neuropsychological decline. However, given reports of schizophrenia patients with IQs in the normal range (55, 56) and the presumption that these patients have escaped neuropsychological decline, we asked if any of the individuals with schizophrenia in our cohort fit this profile. Of the 31 individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia, three individuals had both a childhood IQ of >100 and an IQ decline of <3.19 IQ points (the standard error of measurement of the WISC-R). While one of these three individuals performed in the normal range on all adult neuropsychological tests, each showed decline on the digit symbol coding test (mean=−0.88 standard deviations), suggesting that decline in processing speed is a core feature of schizophrenia. These findings further suggest that average to above-average neuropsychological test performance in a subset of adults diagnosed with schizophrenia cannot be used to infer that neuropsychological decline has not occurred. Rather, prospective baseline tests of neuropsychological functioning are necessary to document decline.
A third limitation concerns three unusual aspects of our sample that may limit the generalizability of our findings. First, the prevalence of schizophrenia is high. As we discussed earlier, this may be explained in part by a combination of our comprehensive repeated-measurement ascertainment strategy, high retention rates, and geographical variation. Second, a portion of individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia had not, to our knowledge, received treatment specifically for psychotic illness. These individuals had, however, come into contact with the mental health care system and were virtually indistinguishable on a variety of correlates from those who had been treated for psychotic illness. The one exception was that those who had not received treatment specifically for psychotic illness were from families of lower socioeconomic status, which might reflect an association between socioeconomic status and quality of care. Third, most individuals with schizophrenia were not taking antipsychotic medication in the year before adult testing. While this increases confidence that neuropsychological decline between ages 7–13 and age 38 is not due to recent antipsychotic medication use, it raises the question of whether our results are generalizable to patients currently taking antipsychotic medication. We noted very little difference in IQ decline between those who were (N=7; IQ decline, ∼7 IQ points) and those who were not (N=24; IQ decline, ∼6 points) taking antipsychotic medication in the year before testing. Bolstering the generalizability of our findings is the fact that our estimates of the IQ deficit in both childhood (9 points) and adulthood (15 points) precisely match estimates from meta-analyses of the premorbid IQ deficit in schizophrenia (3) and the IQ deficit in first-episode schizophrenia (1, 4).
This study has a number of implications. First, the results suggest that individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia experience neuropsychological decline from before to after illness onset. Second, however, the extent of decline and the developmental progression of decline varies considerably across mental functions that can be generally organized as fluid and crystallized abilities. Fluid abilities (e.g., processing speed, learning, executive function) showed the most substantial decline, with deficits in processing speed, for example, increasing gradually from childhood to beyond the early teen years. In contrast, crystallized abilities (e.g., verbal IQ) did not decline. Rather, these deficits were already apparent in childhood and remained static through midlife. This suggests that different pathophysiological mechanisms underlie the deficits in fluid and crystallized abilities seen in adult schizophrenia patients. Moreover, the findings highlight the fact that a substantial proportion of the neuropsychological deficits seen in adult schizophrenia patients is apparent before the onset of puberty, and future research on the emergence of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia should target early childhood to ascertain deficits in crystallized abilities and later childhood to ascertain deficits in fluid abilities. Finally, pharmacological and cognitive remediation therapies should target neuropsychological functioning as well as cognitive impairment in everyday life (57), as treatment strategies that target both outcomes may have the greatest chances of success (58).

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the Dunedin Study members, their families, the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit staff, and study founder Phil Silva.

Supplementary Material

Supplementary Material (91_ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Reichenberg A, Harvey PD: Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings. Psychol Bull 2007; 133:833–858
2.
Dickson H, Laurens KR, Cullen AE, Hodgins S: Meta-analyses of cognitive and motor function in youth aged 16 years and younger who subsequently develop schizophrenia. Psychol Med 2012; 42:743–755
3.
Woodberry KA, Giuliano AJ, Seidman LJ: Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:579–587
4.
Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis KK: Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 1998; 12:426–445
5.
Mesholam-Gately RI, Giuliano AJ, Goff KP, Faraone SV, Seidman LJ: Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 2009; 23:315–336
6.
Heaton RK, Gladsjo JA, Palmer BW, Kuck J, Marcotte TD, Jeste DV: Stability and course of neuropsychological deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58:24–32
7.
Hoff AL, Svetina C, Shields G, Stewart J, DeLisi LE: Ten year longitudinal study of neuropsychological functioning subsequent to a first episode of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005; 78:27–34
8.
Fucetola R, Seidman LJ, Kremen WS, Faraone SV, Goldstein JM, Tsuang MT: Age and neuropsychologic function in schizophrenia: a decline in executive abilities beyond that observed in healthy volunteers. Biol Psychiatry 2000; 48:137–146
9.
Kurtz MM: Neurocognitive impairment across the lifespan in schizophrenia: an update. Schizophr Res 2005; 74:15–26
10.
Harvey PD, Silverman JM, Mohs RC, Parrella M, White L, Powchik P, Davidson M, Davis KL: Cognitive decline in late-life schizophrenia: a longitudinal study of geriatric chronically hospitalized patients. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:32–40
11.
Kremen WS, Vinogradov S, Poole JH, Schaefer CA, Deicken RF, Factor-Litvak P, Brown AS: Cognitive decline in schizophrenia from childhood to midlife: a 33-year longitudinal birth cohort study. Schizophr Res 2010; 118:1–5
12.
Seidman LJ, Buka SL, Goldstein JM, Tsuang MT: Intellectual decline in schizophrenia: evidence from a prospective birth cohort 28 year follow-up study. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006; 28:225–242
13.
Caspi A, Reichenberg A, Weiser M, Rabinowitz J, Kaplan Z, Knobler H, Davidson-Sagi N, Davidson M: Cognitive performance in schizophrenia patients assessed before and following the first psychotic episode. Schizophr Res 2003; 65:87–94
14.
Schwartzman AE, Douglas VI: Intellectual loss in schizophrenia, I. Can J Psychol 1962; 16:1–10
15.
Bilder RM, Reiter G, Bates J, Lencz T, Szeszko P, Goldman RS, Robinson D, Lieberman JA, Kane JM: Cognitive development in schizophrenia: follow-back from the first episode. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2006; 28:270–282
16.
Gochman PA, Greenstein D, Sporn A, Gogtay N, Keller B, Shaw P, Rapoport JL: IQ stabilization in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005; 77:271–277
17.
Russell AJ, Munro JC, Jones PB, Hemsley DR, Murray RM: Schizophrenia and the myth of intellectual decline. Am J Psychiatry 1997; 154:635–639
18.
Lubin A, Gieseking CF, Williams HL: Direct measurement of cognitive deficit in schizophrenia. J Consult Psychol 1962; 26:139–143
19.
Sheitman BB, Murray MG, Snyder JA, Silva S, Goldman R, Chakos M, Volavka J, Lieberman JA: IQ scores of treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients before and after the onset of the illness. Schizophr Res 2000; 46:203–207
20.
Rappaport SR, Webb WB: An attempt to study intellectual deterioration by premorbid and psychotic testing. J Consult Psychol 1950; 14:95–98
21.
Albee GW, Lane EA, Corcoran C, Werneke A: Childhood and intercurrent intellectual performance of adult schizophrenics. J Consult Psychol 1963; 27:364–366
22.
Cohen P, Cohen J: The clinician’s illusion. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1984; 41:1178–1182
23.
Keefe RSE, Perkins DO, Gu H, Zipursky RB, Christensen BK, Lieberman JA: A longitudinal study of neurocognitive function in individuals at-risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res 2006; 88:26–35
24.
Hawkins KA, Keefe RSE, Christensen BK, Addington J, Woods SW, Callahan J, Zipursky RB, Perkins DO, Tohen M, Breier A, McGlashan TH: Neuropsychological course in the prodrome and first episode of psychosis: findings from the PRIME North America double blind treatment study. Schizophr Res 2008; 105:1–9
25.
Seidman LJ, Giuliano AJ, Meyer EC, Addington J, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, Tsuang MT, Walker EF, Woods SW, Bearden CE, Christensen BK, Hawkins K, Heaton R, Keefe RSE, Heinssen R, Cornblatt BANorth American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS) Group: Neuropsychology of the prodrome to psychosis in the NAPLS consortium: relationship to family history and conversion to psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:578–588
26.
Reichenberg A, Caspi A, Harrington H, Houts R, Keefe RS, Murray RM, Poulton R, Moffitt TE: Static and dynamic cognitive deficits in childhood preceding adult schizophrenia: a 30-year study. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:160–169
27.
Burt DB, Zembar MJ, Niederehe G: Depression and memory impairment: a meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and specificity. Psychol Bull 1995; 117:285–305
28.
Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brüne M, Bullmore ET, Carter CS, Clayton NS, Connor R, Davis S, Deakin B, DeRubeis RJ, Dubois B, Geyer MA, Goodwin GM, Gorwood P, Jay TM, Joëls M, Mansuy IM, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Murphy D, Rolls E, Saletu B, Spedding M, Sweeney J, Whittington M, Young LJ: Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes, and the quest for improved therapy. Nat Rev Drug Discov 2012; 11:141–168
29.
Poulton R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, Murray R, Harrington H: Children’s self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:1053–1058
30.
Cannon M, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Taylor A, Murray RM, Poulton R: Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:449–456
31.
Robins LN, Helzer JE, Croughan J, Ratcliff KS: National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its history, characteristics, and validity. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981; 38:381–389
32.
Robins LN, Cottler L, Bucholz KK, Compton W: Diagnostic Interview Schedule for DSM-IV. St Louis, Mo, Washington University School of Medicine, 1995
33.
Kendler KS, Gallagher TJ, Abelson JM, Kessler RC: Lifetime prevalence, demographic risk factors, and diagnostic validity of nonaffective psychosis as assessed in a US community sample: the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1996; 53:1022–1031
34.
Dutta R, Murray RM, Allardyce J, Jones PB, Boydell JE: Mortality in first-contact psychosis patients in the UK: a cohort study. Psychol Med 2012; 42:1649–1661
35.
Youssef HA, Kinsella A, Waddington JL: Evidence for geographical variations in the prevalence of schizophrenia in rural Ireland. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:254–258
36.
Arajärvi R, Suvisaari J, Suokas J, Schreck M, Haukka J, Hintikka J, Partonen T, Lönnqvist J: Prevalence and diagnosis of schizophrenia based on register, case record, and interview data in an isolated Finnish birth cohort born 1940–1969. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2005; 40:808–816
37.
Torrey EF, McGuire M, O’Hare A, Walsh D, Spellman MP: Endemic psychosis in western Ireland. Am J Psychiatry 1984; 141:966–970
38.
Ferguson S, Blakely T, Allan B, Colling S: Suicide Rates in New Zealand: Exploring Associations With Social and Economic Factors. Wellington, New Zealand, Ministry of Health, 2005
39.
van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L: A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness-persistence-impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med 2009; 39:179–195
40.
Wechsler D: Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Revised. New York, Psychological Corp, 1974
41.
Wechsler D: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th ed. San Antonio, Tex, Pearson Assessment, 2008
42.
Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th ed. New York, Oxford University Press, 2004
43.
Army Individual Battery: Manual and Directions for Scoring. Washington, DC, War Department, Adjutant General’s Office, 1944
44.
Cohen J: A power primer. Psychol Bull 1992; 112:155–159
45.
Davidson M, Reichenberg A, Rabinowitz J, Weiser M, Kaplan Z, Mark M: Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1328–1335
46.
Werner S, Malaspina D, Rabinowitz J: Socioeconomic status at birth is associated with risk of schizophrenia: population-based multilevel study. Schizophr Bull 2007; 33:1373–1378
47.
Mortensen PB, Pedersen CB, Westergaard T, Wohlfahrt J, Ewald H, Mors O, Andersen PK, Melbye M: Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. N Engl J Med 1999; 340:603–608
48.
Dickinson D, Ramsey M, Gold JM: Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2007; 64:532–542
49.
Gale CR, Allerhand M, Deary IJHALCyon Study Team: Is there a bidirectional relationship between depressive symptoms and cognitive ability in older people? A prospective study using the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Psychol Med 2012; 42:2057–2069
50.
Ganguli M, Du YC, Dodge HH, Ratcliff GG, Chang CCH: Depressive symptoms and cognitive decline in late life: a prospective epidemiological study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:153–160
51.
Dufouil C, Fuhrer R, Dartigues JF, Alpérovitch A: Longitudinal analysis of the association between depressive symptomatology and cognitive deterioration. Am J Epidemiol 1996; 144:634–641
52.
Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, Poulton R, Caspi A: Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:50–57
53.
Zammit S, Allebeck P, David AS, Dalman C, Hemmingsson T, Lundberg I, Lewis G: A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:354–360
54.
Niendam TA, Bearden CE, Rosso IM, Sanchez LE, Hadley T, Nuechterlein KH, Cannon TD: A prospective study of childhood neurocognitive functioning in schizophrenic patients and their siblings. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160:2060–2062
55.
Weickert TW, Goldberg TE, Gold JM, Bigelow LB, Egan MF, Weinberger DR: Cognitive impairments in patients with schizophrenia displaying preserved and compromised intellect. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57:907–913
56.
MacCabe JH, Brébion G, Reichenberg A, Ganguly T, McKenna PJ, Murray RM, David AS: Superior intellectual ability in schizophrenia: neuropsychological characteristics. Neuropsychology 2012; 26:181–190
57.
Ventura J, Reise SP, Keefe RSE, Baade LE, Gold JM, Green MF, Kern RS, Mesholam-Gately R, Nuechterlein KH, Seidman LJ, Bilder RM: The Cognitive Assessment Interview (CAI): development and validation of an empirically derived, brief interview-based measure of cognition. Schizophr Res 2010; 121:24–31
58.
Bowie CR, McGurk SR, Mausbach B, Patterson TL, Harvey PD: Combined cognitive remediation and functional skills training for schizophrenia: effects on cognition, functional competence, and real-world behavior. Am J Psychiatry 2012; 169:710–718

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 91 - 101
PubMed: 24030246

History

Received: 19 November 2012
Revision received: 14 April 2013
Revision received: 23 May 2013
Accepted: 3 June 2013
Published online: 1 January 2014
Published in print: January 2014

Authors

Details

Madeline H. Meier, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.
Avshalom Caspi, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.
Abraham Reichenberg, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.
Richard S.E. Keefe, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.
Helen L. Fisher, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.
HonaLee Harrington, B.S.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.
Renate Houts, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.
Richie Poulton, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.
Terrie E. Moffitt, Ph.D.
From the Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Duke University, Durham, N.C.; the Duke Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center, Center for Child and Family Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Institute for Genome Sciences and Policy, Duke University, Durham; the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Duke University Medical Center, Durham; the Department of Psychosis Studies and the Social, Genetic, and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London; and the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit, Department of Preventive and Social Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Moffitt ([email protected]).

Funding Information

Dr. Keefe has received investigator-initiated research funding support from the Department of Veterans Affairs, Feinstein Institute for Medical Research, GlaxoSmithKline, NIMH, Novartis, Psychogenics, Research Foundation for Mental Hygiene, and Singapore National Medical Research Council; he has received honoraria from or served as a consultant or advisory board member for Abbott, Akebia, Amgen, Astellas, Asubio, BiolineRx, Biomarin, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, EnVivo, Helicon, Lundbeck, Merck, Mitsubishi, Novartis, Otsuka, Pfizer, Roche, Shire, Sunovion, Takeda, and Targacept; he receives royalties for the BACS testing battery and the MATRICS Battery (BACS Symbol Coding) and is a shareholder in NeuroCog Trials. The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.
Supplementary Material
Supported in part by the National Institute on Aging (grant AG032282) and the U.K. Medical Research Council (grant MR/K00381X). The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit is supported by the New Zealand Health Research Council. Dr. Meier was supported by National Institute on Drug Abuse grant P30 DA023026 and by the Jacobs Foundation.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share