Skip to main content
Open access
Articles
Published Online: 11 September 2015

Defense Mechanisms of Pregnant Mothers Predict Attachment Security, Social-Emotional Competence, and Behavior Problems in Their Toddlers

Abstract

Objective:

For at-risk (single parent, low income, low support) mothers, healthy adaptation and the ability to manage stress have clear implications for parenting and the social-emotional well-being of their young offspring. The purpose of this longitudinal study was to examine associations between defense mechanisms in pregnant women and their toddlers’ attachment security, social-emotional, and behavioral adjustment.

Method:

Participants were 84 pregnant women during their last trimester of pregnancy, recruited from community agencies primarily serving low-income families. Women were followed prospectively from pregnancy through 2 years after birth and completed several multimethod assessments during that period. Observations of mother-child interactions were also coded after the postnatal visits.

Results:

Multiple regression analyses revealed that mothers’ defense mechanisms were significantly associated with several toddler outcomes. Mature, healthy defenses were significantly associated with greater toddler attachment security and social-emotional competence and fewer behavior problems, and less mature defenses (disavowal in particular) were associated with lower levels of attachment security and social-emotional competence. Associations remained significant, or were only slightly attenuated, after controlling for demographic variables and partner abuse during pregnancy.

Conclusions:

The study findings suggest that defensive functioning in parents preparing for and parenting toddlers influences the parent-child attachment relationship and social-emotional adjustment in the earliest years of life. Possible mechanisms for these associations may include parental attunement and mentalization, as well as specific caregiving behavior toward the child. Defensive functioning during times of increased stress (such as the prenatal to postnatal period) may be especially important for understanding parental influences on the child.
Pregnancy can be a significant stressor for mothers (1), especially for low-income women (2). The ability to adaptively cope with stress before, during, and after pregnancy—both consciously and unconsciously—may affect a mother’s ability to optimally support her child’s psychological development. Coping mechanisms fall into three broad categories: seeking social support, cognitive strategies, and involuntary defense mechanisms (3). This study focused on defense mechanisms—automatic psychological processes that mediate reactions to emotional conflicts and internal or external stressors. Special attention was given to the adaptive defenses (e.g., altruism) because they promote optimal adaptation by keeping individuals aware of their thoughts and feelings and the consequences of their actions. This differs from pathological defenses, in which both thought and feeling are ignored (4). Thus, healthy defenses should allow mothers to cope with stressors and remain attuned to their child’s thoughts, feelings, behaviors, and needs.
Infants are born into a complex social world with an innate capacity for social relatedness. A major developmental task in the first year of life is to develop at least one attachment relationship, which is characterized by a preference for a specific caregiver (or more than one), who is expected to care for the infant and with whom the infant shares a close, emotional bond (5, 6). According to attachment theory, the capacity for social relatedness ensures the child’s survival. Attachment behaviors, such as crying and smiling, are present at birth to facilitate this process; these behaviors increase the likelihood that a caregiver will respond to, care for, and protect the child. Caregivers also assist considerably with the infant’s emotional and behavioral regulation through their responses to infant cues (7, 8). By the end of the first year, infants’ cognitions about the self, others, and the world (i.e., internal working models) are also beginning to develop as the result of experiences with the people most important to them (5), and these cognitions subsequently guide the child’s behaviors and future interactions. Thus, the infant’s emotional, behavioral, and social capacities are organized and firmly rooted in the relational context with primary attachment figures during the first several years of life (6, 9).
When the attachment relationship goes well, caregivers consistently respond to the child’s needs in an accurate and attuned way, and the parent-child relationship is said to be “secure” because the child can count on and expect the attachment figure to provide appropriate care in times of need (5). Secure attachment relationships are associated with better social-emotional and behavioral functioning for the child both concurrently and in their future (10). In contrast, when parents are not able to provide consistent, sensitive care (for a variety of reasons, such as their own mental health difficulties and problematic defensive processes), the parent-child relationship suffers and is considered “insecure” because the young child is forced to develop a less adaptive relationship strategy in order to maintain proximity with the parent. Attachment insecurity is in turn associated with less adaptive developmental outcomes and greater social-emotional problems for the child (9).
We hypothesized that mothers who frequently utilize mature defenses are more attuned to their child’s needs and respond more appropriately to meet those needs, helping the child to develop adequate behavioral and emotional regulatory capacities early in life. Mature defense mechanisms may be especially important during times of stress and major relational changes, as is the case during the prenatal and postpartum periods. Therefore, a greater capacity to cope with the physical and emotional demands of preparing for and parenting a new infant, particularly in the context of other psychosocial adversity, is likely to facilitate a healthy parent-child relationship and optimal child development. Thus, we hypothesized that the maturity of defense mechanisms in at-risk pregnant mothers would predict their toddlers’ attachment security, social-emotional competence, and behavior problems.

Method

Participants

Participants were 84 pregnant women recruited for a larger longitudinal study (11) of the role of psychosocial risk in women and their young children. Data were collected during the third trimester of pregnancy, at 3 months postpartum, and when children were 1 and 2 years old. For the present study, data from the prenatal and 2-year waves were chosen, since we believed that the strengths and challenges of the children would be more clearly observable at age 2, especially social-emotional competence and potential behavior problems. Participants were recruited through fliers posted at public locations and agencies serving mostly low-income families.

Procedures

Pregnant women interested in participating in the study were screened by telephone. Inclusion criteria were age ≥18 years and English fluency. The majority of assessments were conducted in the family home by two or three research assistants who had received intensive training from the second author. At each wave of data collection, self-report questionnaires were read aloud to enhance participant understanding of each question and limit random responding. Each protocol was recorded in order for the research assistant to assess for participant literacy difficulties. Financial compensation was provided in the form of gift cards.
Interviews were conducted in the third trimester and lasted approximately 2.5–3 hours. The assessments, which were administered in a standardized order, included a demographic measure, a semistructured clinical interview assessing mothers’ representations of their infant in utero, and self-report measures. Mothers were contacted approximately 2 weeks after the baby’s due date to gather birth information, and a brief telephone interview was administered when infants were approximately 3 months old. Postnatal assessments were conducted around the child’s first and second birthdays, each lasting approximately 3–3.5 hours. Demographic and self-report data were collected, and observations of parent-child interactions were made.
Because of factors associated with poverty status and other psychosocial risks, participants were relatively transient with their living arrangements. To reduce attrition, participants were contacted every 3 months to update contact information as well as that of friends and relatives who would know their location if they could not be reached. Study staff conducted home visits in person to update information if participants could not be reached by telephone or mail. At the 2-year point, 84 of 120 mother-child pairs had completed all study measures.
The study was approved by the institutional review board of Eastern Michigan University. Informed consent was obtained from each participant at every wave of data collection.

Measures

Maternal defense mechanisms.

The DSM-IV Defensive Functioning Scale was developed as a proposed axis for DSM to provide researchers and clinicians with a standard list of defense mechanisms and definitions for diagnostic and research purposes (12, 13). The scale includes 31 defenses organized into seven hierarchical levels of functioning: high adaptive, mental inhibitions, minor image-distorting, disavowal, major image-distorting, action, and defensive dysregulation. Defenses are rated each time they emerge during a clinical interview. Total scores for each level of defense and an overall defensive functioning score (a measure of maturity of defense) were calculated. The overall defensive functioning score is calculated by multiplying the score for each level (ranging from 7, “high adaptive,” to 1, “defensive dysregulation”) by the total number of defenses at each level and dividing that score by the total number of defenses. Defensive dysregulation (i.e., psychotic) defenses were not observed in this study. Interrater agreement of the scales has ranged from fair to excellent, and their validity has been supported through studies of defenses and symptoms or disorders, personality disorder, social or occupational functioning, immature defenses, childhood or adult abuse, relatedness, improvement in depressive episodes, and therapists’ use of psychodynamic-interpersonal interventions (1423). Interviews of mothers were coded by a psychologist-psychoanalyst (J.H.P.) with 24 years of experience rating defense mechanisms. Interrater agreement for the defenses was calculated from 25 randomly selected interviews rated by a doctoral candidate in clinical psychology (L.R.) who underwent extensive training. Raters were blind to all participant information. Interrater agreement (intraclass correlation) (24) for this study was as follows: overall defensive functioning, 0.81 (excellent); high adaptive, 0.76 (excellent); mental inhibition, 0.66 (good); minor image-distorting, 0.70 (good); disavowal, 0.79 (excellent); major image-distorting, 0.59 (fair); action level, 0.49 (fair).
The Defensive Functioning Scale was used to code maternal narratives resulting from the Working Model of the Child Interview (25), which was administered during the pregnancy interview. The Working Model of the Child Interview is a 1-hour (audio-recorded) semistructured interview that assesses a caregiver’s representations of the infant, the self as a caregiver, and the mother-infant relationship. The original coding system (26) includes qualitative scales (e.g., acceptance, coherence, and involvement) and categorical classifications (balanced, disengaged, and distorted); the present study utilized the narratives to evaluate mothers’ level of defenses.

Child attachment security.

The Attachment Q-Set (27) was used to measure toddler attachment security at the 2-year visit. It includes 90 items of infant behavior indicative of attachment security and insecurity, along a continuum; these items are later sorted by the researchers into nine equal piles of 10 statements ranging from “least characteristic of the child” to “most characteristic of the child” and are based on naturalistic observations of infant behavior toward the caregiver/mother during the 3-hour home visit. Research assistants were extensively trained for 6 months prior to attending home visits and sorting. Training included extensive readings and discussions about attachment theory, infant attachment behaviors, and attachment assessments. Two to three research assistants from the team (advanced undergraduates and clinical psychology graduate students) attended every home visit. After each visit, all research assistants who attended the home visit completed the Q-sort together, using consensus methodology, usually within 24–48 hours. Because home observations were made while research assistants were attending to all that was occurring in the home (e.g., the lead interviewer was focused on appropriate administration of measures, others were focused on monitoring the baby’s activities, etc.), the decision was made early on to have all assistants complete the Q-sort together, rather than independently, so that all observations were taken into account in the final Q-sort. Research assistants discussed every item together, consulted their interview notes, shared their observations, and decided on card placement. Every home visit was also discussed with the study’s principal investigator (A.H.B.) during weekly lab meetings with all research assistants present. Disagreements (which were rare) were conferenced. Each participant’s sort was then correlated with a criterion sort developed by experts in the field that represents the “ideal secure” infant. The resulting correlations were then used as the variable in analyses, with higher scores representing greater attachment security.

Child social-emotional competence and behavior problems.

The Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (28) was administered to evaluate the toddler’s social, emotional, and behavioral problems, as well as competencies, according to maternal report. The 42-item scale assesses child difficulties and competencies within the past month on a 3-point scale from 0 (not true/rarely) to 2 (very true/often). Items are summed to form the problem scale (31 items) and the competence scale (11 items). The measure has been demonstrated to have strong psychometric properties (28). In the present study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.83 for the problem scale and 0.85 for the competence scale.

Demographic characteristics.

A questionnaire was administered to mothers to gather data on characteristics such as age, education, income, marital status, and ethnicity (Table 1). The Conflict Tactics Scale (29) was used to assess partner abuse during pregnancy with items covering minor and severe forms of abuse. Participants rated items on the subscales for physical abuse (12 items), sexual coercion (seven items), and psychological abuse (eight items) in reference to their partner’s behavior during pregnancy. Physical and sexual abuse items were combined into a total score, and the final physical and psychological abuse scores were dichotomized (absent or present). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.63 for the physical abuse scale and 0.79 for the psychological abuse scale.
TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of Mothers in a Study of Maternal Defense Mechanisms During Pregnancy and Subsequent Toddler Outcomes (N=84)
Characteristic  
 MeanSD
Age (years)26.875.81
 N%
Ethnicity  
 Caucasian3238
 African American3946
 Biracial911
 Other45
Marital status  
 Married or living with partner2732
 Single5363
 Separated or divorced45
Education  
 Less than high school45
 High school graduate5667
 College graduate2428
Yearly income  
 Less than 20,0004351
 20,000–39,9992024
 40,000–59,999911
 60,000 or more1214
Partner abuse during pregnancy  
 Psychological6274
 Physical1821

Data Analysis

Means, standard deviations, and ranges were computed for each variable (Table 2). Pearson correlations (two-tailed) are reported in Table 3. Significant correlations between predictor (mothers’ defenses) and criterion variables (toddler variables) determined inclusion in regression analyses. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were performed with demographic and partner abuse variables entered into the first block and defense levels entered into the second block in order to determine whether defenses predict attachment security, social-emotional competence, and behavior problems beyond what is accounted for by the demographic and partner abuse variables. To avoid the issue of collinearity, the overall defensive functioning scores were run in separate analyses from defense levels.
TABLE 2. Descriptive Statistics for Child Outcome Variables and Maternal Defense Levels
VariableMeanSD
Criterion variables/toddler outcomes  
 Attachment security0.300.27
 Social-emotional competence18.362.42
 Behavior problems11.376.80
Predictor variables/defenses  
 Overall defensive functioning5.510.66
 High adaptive level2.572.53
 Mental inhibition level3.922.59
 Minor image-distorting level1.832.06
 Disavowal level3.232.64
 Major image-distorting0.230.61
 Action level0.170.51
TABLE 3. Correlations Between Maternal Demographic and Partner Abuse Characteristics, Maternal Defenses, and Toddler Outcomes (N=84)
 Maternal Demographic VariablesPartner Abuse
MeasureAgeMarital StatusaEducationEthnicityaIncomePsychologicalPhysical
Mother’s defenses       
 Overall defensive functioning0.30**–0.080.37***–0.080.34**–0.24*–0.08
 High adaptive0.28*–0.120.20–0.110.21–0.27*–0.11
 Mental inhibition0.010.010.010.080.05–0.090.03
 Minor image-distorting–0.24*0.02–0.14–0.21–0.01–0.040.27*
 Disavowal–0.200.03–0.29**0.18–0.34**–0.22*0.17
 Major image-distorting–0.05–0.03–0.010.09–0.01–0.05–0.05
 Action–0.07–0.07–0.140.11–0.02–0.08–0.08
Toddler outcomes       
 Attachment security0.22*–0.210.41***–0.26*0.35***–0.20–0.26*
 Social-emotional competence0.17–0.010.10–0.150.070.030.01
 Behavior problems–0.19–0.09–0.140.14–0.150.160.04
a
Dummy variables were created for marital status (single=1, other=0) and ethnicity (African American=1, other=0).
*
p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

Results

Correlations Between Mothers’ Defenses, Demographic Variables, and Abuse During Pregnancy

Correlations between demographic or partner abuse variables and defense mechanisms are reported in Table 3. Only marital status failed to correlate significantly with any defense or toddler outcome variables, and it was not included in any subsequent analyses.

Correlations Between Maternal Defenses and Toddler Outcomes

Mothers’ overall defensive functioning and high adaptive level defense scores were significantly and positively correlated with child attachment security and social-emotional competence (Table 4). Mothers’ overall defensive functioning and high adaptive level defense scores were also significantly and negatively correlated with child behavior problems. The correlation between disavowal and behavior problems approached but fell short of significance (p=0.07). Disavowal was the only other defense level that significantly correlated with toddler outcomes. Therefore, high adaptive and disavowal level defenses were included in the regression analyses predicting attachment security and social-emotional competence. Separate regressions were run with overall defensive functioning predicting toddler outcomes.
TABLE 4. Correlations Between Maternal Defense Characteristics and Toddler Outcomes (N=84)
 Maternal Defense VariablesToddler Outcomes
Toddler OutcomesOverall Defensive FunctioningHigh AdaptiveMental InhibitionMinor Image-DistortingDisavowalMajor Image-DistortingActionSocial-Emotional CompetenceBehavior Problems
Attachment security0.40***0.47***–0.03–0.19–0.22*0.05–0.090.32**–0.19
Social-emotional competence0.23*0.24*–0.07–0.07–0.30**0.060.03 –0.21
Behavior problems–0.26*–0.29**–0.070.140.200.120.05  
*
p<0.05. **p<0.01. ***p<0.001.

Predicting Toddler Outcomes With Overall Defensive Functioning and Defense Levels

For regression analyses, demographic and partner abuse variables were entered into the first block and overall defensive functioning, high adaptive, and disavowal defense variables were entered as the second block. Results of the six analyses are reported in Table 5.
TABLE 5. Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Toddler Outcomes With Overall Maternal Defensive Functioning Score and Defense Levels
MeasureBR2Adjusted R2R2 ChangeF Changedfp for F Change
Predicting attachment security
Block 1       
 Age       
 Ethnicity       
 Education       
 Income       
 Partner psychological abuse       
 Partner physical abuse       
  0.250.190.254.276, 770.001
Block 2       
 Overall defensive functioning score0.270.310.240.066.211, 760.015
Block 2       
 High adaptive0.39      
 Disavowal–0.04      
  0.380.310.137.762, 750.001
Predicting social-emotional competence
Block 1       
 Age       
 Ethnicity       
 Education       
 Income       
 Partner psychological abuse       
 Partner physical abuse       
  0.040.030.040.586, 770.74
Block 2       
 Overall defensive functioning0.240.090.0040.053.751, 760.06
Block 2       
 High adaptive0.24      
 Disavowal–0.31
  0.160.070.125.302, 750.007
Predicting behavior problems
Block 1       
 Age       
 Ethnicity       
 Education       
 Income       
 Partner psychological abuse       
 Partner physical abuse       
  0.07–0.0040.070.956, 770.47
Block 2       
 Overall defensive functioning score–0.200.100.020.032.461, 760.12
Block 2       
 High adaptive–0.230.110.030.063.881, 760.05

Mothers’ Defense Mechanisms Predict Attachment Security, Social-Emotional Competence, and Behavior Problems in Toddlers

Demographic and partner abuse variables significantly predicted attachment security (p=0.001), and together they explained 25% of the variance in attachment security. Overall defensive functioning significantly predicted attachment security after variance from the first block was accounted for (p=0.015), explaining an additional 6% of the variance. In a separate analysis, high adaptive and disavowal level defenses, entered as the second block, significantly predicted attachment security (p=0.001), explaining an additional 13% of the variance.
Demographic and partner abuse variables in the first block failed to predict toddler social-emotional competence. Overall defensive functioning, entered as the second block, approached significance (p=0.06) in predicting social-emotional competence after variance from the first block was accounted for (p=0.015), explaining an additional 5% of the variance. In a separate analysis, high adaptive and disavowal level defenses significantly predicted social-emotional competence after variance from the first block was accounted for (p=0.007), explaining an additional 12% of the variance.
Finally, demographic and partner abuse variables in the first block failed to predict toddler behavior problems. Overall defensive functioning, entered as the second block, also failed to predict behavior problems. In a separate analysis, high adaptive level defenses, entered as the second block, significantly predicted behavior problems, explaining an additional 6% of the variance.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first prospective study evaluating maternal defense mechanisms as predictors of toddler attachment security, social-emotional competence, and behavior problems. Our findings demonstrate that greater use of healthy, adaptive defenses by pregnant mothers, assessed through their representations of their child in utero, predicted greater attachment security, better social-emotional competence, and fewer behavior problems in their toddlers after accounting for the significant relationships between demographic or partner abuse variables and child outcomes. Similarly, the findings demonstrate that less frequent use of maladaptive defenses (disavowal level defenses) predicted greater attachment security, better social-emotional competence, and fewer behavior problems. Thus, it appears that, as predicted, a mother’s ability to use healthy defense mechanisms during stressful times allows her to support the child’s psychological development and regulatory capacities in the earliest years of life.
These findings raise questions regarding the mechanisms by which certain maternal defenses contribute to toddlers’ social-emotional adjustment. We propose several such mechanisms, including maternal caregiving behavior and the mother’s capacity to mentalize about the child during pregnancy and after birth. Mentalization is the process by which an individual is able to consider and anticipate what is in the mind of another and use mental states to understand behavior (3032). For a parent, mentalization, also known as parental reflective functioning (33, 34), involves entering into the inner world of the child and helping the child make sense of and organize his or her experiences.
In our sample, the most frequently occurring high adaptive defense mechanisms were humor, anticipation, altruism, suppression, and self-observation, all of which may promote more sensitive caregiving and greater parental mentalizing. For instance, the appropriate use of humor likely increases shared positive affect and attentive playfulness with the child and may help diffuse tension or negative affect under stress. Anticipation promotes the mother’s ability to store up and/or measure her own resources, considering ahead of time when the child is likely to need her support; planning and anticipation may also reduce the occurrence of stressful experiences in general. Altruism allows the mother to set her own needs aside to care for the child, placing the child’s attachment needs at the forefront, while also continuing to gain gratification from doing so. The use of suppression in response to daily stress, anger, and fatigue while parenting a toddler diminishes the chances of any direct aggression or hostility toward the child and instead allows for a “cooling down” period before responding. Finally, self-observation refers to the mother examining her feelings, wishes, fears, and maladaptive ways of potentially responding to the child. Through the use of self-observation, the mother can choose not to follow her immediate impulses in responding to a difficult situation with the toddler (e.g., blaming [projection or devaluation] or hitting the child [acting out]). Self-observation most overlaps with mentalizing, although all of the healthy defenses probably overlap somewhat with a mother’s capacity for reflective functioning. Therefore, overall, the findings in our study are consistent with past research demonstrating that higher levels of maternal reflective functioning are associated with more sensitive caregiving behavior, as well as greater infant attachment security (35, 36).
In contrast, the use of disavowal level defenses (denial, projection, rationalization) may distort perceptions of a child’s mental states and behavior. For example, denying a toddler’s need for emotional refueling after separations may interfere with the child’s sense of security because the mother may not offer support or soothing at such times. Anecdotally, we have also heard mothers in our sample describing the toddler as “not needing me” because of the child’s (pseudo) independence or because of the child serving in some role-reversed way in order to meet the needs of the mother (another form of denial of the child’s needs). Additionally, projecting malevolent intentions to a child’s behaviors (e.g., “He won’t go to bed because he wants me to be miserable,” or as seen in the first of the two vignettes presented in the shaded box) is likely to result in negative maternal responses that do not match the child’s internal experience. As a result, repeated experiences with a mother who displays such projections likely results in the toddler developing an incoherent sense of self, having difficulty understanding and organizing interpersonal experiences, and exhibiting poor emotion regulation and insecure, if not disorganized, attachment behavior. Indeed, a few reports (37, 38) indicate that maternal negative projections toward their infant or toddler are associated with problematic caregiving behavior, as well as child internalizing and externalizing problems and disorganized attachment. It is important to note, however, that no existing studies to our knowledge have measured projection per se by trained coders. Furthermore, the importance of negative maternal projections is highlighted by the fact that they are the focus of some forms of infant-parent psychotherapy (39).
This study had several strengths. One was its prospective, multimethod design, following women and their children from pregnancy through age 2. Another strength was that the ratings of the mothers’ defenses were based on a semistructured interview that elicited representations of their unborn child; to our knowledge, defense mechanisms have not previously been studied in pregnant parents to predict later child outcomes. Furthermore, ratings of defense mechanisms were not based on mothers’ defense use in general. The Defensive Functioning Scale allows for a comprehensive assessment of defenses ranging from normal to pathological functioning. A third strength was the success in maintaining contact with 84 at-risk mothers over a 2-year period. As for limitations, mentalization capacities and observed maternal behavior among participants were not directly assessed. Consequently, statements about causality cannot be made with certainty, and interpretations of the findings need to be examined in future studies. Whether or not mothers’ defense mechanisms directly influence attachment security and other aspects of child development, or are mediated in part by maternal mentalization and maternal behavior, remains an empirical question.
In summary, findings from this study suggest that maternal defense mechanisms are important in understanding the nature of the earliest parent-child relationship and subsequent child adjustment in the early years of life. Examining maternal defenses and other forms of coping may be particularly important during the prenatal and postnatal periods because this is typically a physically and emotionally demanding time, especially for women experiencing adversity and risk such as single parenthood, economic disadvantage, and difficulties with partners and social support. The findings underscore the importance of assessing defense mechanisms as part of a comprehensive evaluation of pregnant mothers and mothers with young children.

Vignettes From the Working Model of the Child Interview

A Mother With an Immature Level of Defensive Functioning (4.5 on a Scale of 1 to 7)

Interviewer: How have you felt physically and emotionally throughout your pregnancy?
Mother: Umm … Probably in between, I think. In between, ’cause she’s [fetus] been pushing me around! [Level 4: projection] I don’t know the words to describe it.
Interviewer: What have you been doing during your pregnancy?
Mother: Resting because they … they somehow rest on you … I basically been resting. Trying to stay off of my feet.
Interviewer: What have been your impressions about the baby while you’re pregnant? What do you sense the baby might be like?
Mother: Hoping he ain’t nothing like his daddy! [Level 5: devaluation] I don’t know. I hope he come out alright and healthy.
Interviewer: How do you think you’ll react to labor and delivery?
Mother: I don’t have no feelings because I’m having a C-section! [Level 4: denial/rationalization] Just ready to be over with it all.
Toddler’s profile: During the home visit, the child was observed to display a number of signs of attachment insecurity. For example, researchers coded the child on the Q-sort as often showing feelings of ambivalence toward the mother. The child seemed to need to stay in close contact with her (beyond simply keeping track of her whereabouts) and indicated wanting contact, but also fussing and wanting to be put back down again at the same time. The child also showed wariness, along with disinterest, in the research assistants who came along to provide care, and avoided them throughout the visit. Furthermore, the toddler was observed to fall to the floor several times, face down, whimpering and crying, in an apparent helpless state. Reports from the mother also indicated that the child exhibited low levels of social-emotional competence and a high number of behavior problems.

A Mother With a Mature Level of Defensive Functioning (6.3 on a Scale of 1 to 7)

Interviewer: How have you felt physically and emotionally throughout your pregnancy?
Mother: I’ve actually felt really good, physically and emotionally. I’ve just been excited because I’m getting so close. I feel tired because it’s a lot to carry.
Interviewer: What have you been doing during your pregnancy?
Mother: I’m pretty much doing everything the same … cleaning, being social, and seeing friends. Now I’m just so big. [Laughs] I’m too big to do yard work! [Level 7: humor]
Interviewer: What have been your impressions about the baby while you’re pregnant? What do you sense the baby might be like?
Mother: She’s been really active. She’s been great … She seems to react a lot when I touch my stomach, and, umm, and that’s kinda fun.
Interviewer: How do you think you’ll react to labor and delivery?
Mother: Well, I’m nervous about it. It’s scary ’cause you don’t know what to expect, it’s the unknown. I mean of course I have in head all of the different ways that I want it to be, and the ways it could be, and how I’m planning for things. [Level 7: anticipation] But whether it will actually be like that, it’s hard to say. I’m just trying to be realistic.
Toddler’s profile: In contrast to the previous example, this toddler demonstrated a number of signs of attachment security during the home visit. Researchers observed the child to use the mother as a secure base from which to explore and play, alternating between checking in with her and exploring toys and others in the home. When being picked up by the mother, the child readily accepted her contact with comfort and ease. The toddler also showed considerable positive affect, appearing lighthearted, playful, happy, and friendly while still showing a preference for the mother over the research assistants. Reports from the mother indicated that child exhibited high levels of social-emotional competence and a low-moderate number of behavior problems.

Supplementary Material

File (feb2016_defense.mp3)

References

1.
Woods SM, Melville JL, Guo Y, et al: Psychosocial stress during pregnancy. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2010; 202:e1–e7
2.
Bradley RH, Corwyn RF: Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu Rev Psychol 2002; 53:371–399
3.
Vaillant GE: Adaptive mental mechanisms: their role in a positive psychology. Am Psychol 2000; 55:89–98
4.
Vaillant GE: Ego mechanisms of defense and personality psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 103:44–50
5.
Bowlby J: Attachment and Loss, vol 1, Attachment. New York, Basic Books, 1969/1982
6.
Sroufe LA: Emotional Development: The Organization of Emotional Life in the Early Years. New York, Cambridge University Press, 1995
7.
Fox NA, Hane AA: Studying the biology of human attachment, in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd ed. Edited by Cassidy J, Shaver PR. New York, Guilford, 2008, pp 217–240
8.
Tronick EZ: The Neurobehavioral and Social-Emotional Development of Infants and Children. New York, WW Norton, 2007
9.
Rosenblum KL, Dayton CJ, Muzik M: Infant social and emotional development: emerging competence in a relational context, in Handbook of Infant Mental Health, 3rd ed. Edited by Zeanah CH. New York, Guilford, 2009, pp 80–103
10.
DeKlyen M, Greenberg MT: Attachment and psychopathology in childhood, in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd ed. Edited by Cassidy J, Shaver PR. New York, Guilford, 2008, pp 637–665
11.
Huth-Bocks AC, Krause K, Ahlfs-Dunn S, et al: Relational trauma and posttraumatic stress symptoms among pregnant women. Psychodyn Psychiatry 2013; 41:277–301
12.
Bond MP, Vaillant JS: An empirical study of the relationship between diagnosis and defense style. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1986; 43:285–288
13.
Perry JC, Hoglend P, Shear K, et al: Field trial of a diagnostic axis for defense mechanisms for DSM-IV. J Pers Disord 1998; 12:56–68
14.
Perry JC, Høglend P: Convergent and discriminant validity of overall defensive functioning. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186:529–535
15.
Porcerelli JH, Cogan R, Markova T, et al: The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Defensive Functioning Scale: a validity study. Compr Psychiatry 2010; 52:225–230
16.
DeFife JA, Hilsenroth MJ: Clinical utility of the Defensive Functioning Scale in the assessment of depression. J Nerv Ment Dis 2005; 193:176–182
17.
Lingiardi V, Lonati C, Delucchi F, et al: Defense mechanisms and personality disorders. J Nerv Ment Dis 1999; 187:224–228
18.
Perry JC: A pilot study of defenses in adults with personality disorders entering psychotherapy. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189:651–660
19.
Blais MA, Conboy CA, Wilcox N, et al: An empirical study of the DSM-IV Defensive Functioning Scale in personality disordered patients. Compr Psychiatry 1996; 37:435–440
20.
Hilsenroth MJ, Callahan KL, Eudell EM: Further reliability, convergent and discriminant validity of overall defensive functioning. J Nerv Ment Dis 2003; 191:730–737
21.
Porcerelli JH, Cogan R, Kamoo R, et al: Convergent validity of the Defense Mechanisms Manual and the Defensive Functioning Scale. J Pers Assess 2010; 92:432–438
22.
Høglend P, Perry JC: Defensive functioning predicts improvement in major depressive episodes. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186:238–243
23.
Siefert CJ, Hilsenroth MJ, Weinberger J, et al: The relationship of patient defensive functioning and alliance with therapist technique during short-term psychodynamic psychotherapy. Clin Psychol Psychother 2006; 13:20–33
24.
Shrout PE, Fleiss JL: Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull 1979; 86:420–428
25.
Zeanah CH, Benoit D, Hirshberg L, et al: Mothers’ representations of their infants are concordant with infant attachment classifications. Developmental Issues in Psychiatry and Psychology 1994; 1:9–18
26.
Zeanah CH, Benoit D: Clinical applications of a parent perception interview in infant mental health. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin North Am 1996; 4:539–554
27.
Waters E, Deane KE: Defining and assessing individual differences in attachment relationships: Q-methodology and the organization of behavior in infancy and early childhood, in Growing Points of Attachment Theory and Research. Edited by Bretherton I, Waters E. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 1985; 50(serial no 209):41–65
28.
Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS: Manual for the Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA), Version 2. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp and Harcourt Press, 2006
29.
Straus MA, Hamby SL, Boney-McCoy S, et al: The Revised Conflict Tactics Scales (CTS2): development and preliminary psychometric data. J Fam Issues 1996; 17:283–316
30.
Fonagy P, Steele M, Steele H, et al: Attachment, the reflective self, and borderline states: the predictive specificity of the Adult Attachment Interview and pathological emotional development, in Attachment Theory: Social, Developmental, and Clinical Perspectives. Edited by Goldberg R, Muir R, Kerr J. Hillsdale, NJ, Analytic Press, 1995, pp 233–278
31.
Fonagy P, Target M: Attachment and reflective function: their role in self-organization. Dev Psychopathol 1997; 9:679–700
32.
Fonagy P, Gergely G, Jurist E, et al: Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of the Self. New York, Other Press, 2005
33.
Slade A: Parental reflective functioning: an introduction. Attach Hum Dev 2005; 7:269–281
34.
Sharp C, Fonagy P: The parents’ capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: constructs, measures, and implications for developmental psychopathology. Soc Dev 2008; 7:737–754
35.
Stacks AM, Muzik M, Wong K, et al: Maternal reflective functioning among mothers with childhood maltreatment histories: links to sensitive parenting and infant attachment security. Attach Hum Dev 2014; 16:515–533
36.
Grienenberger JF, Kelly K, Slade A: Maternal reflective functioning, mother-infant affective communication, and infant attachment: exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attach Hum Dev 2005; 7:299–311
37.
Levendosky A, Bogat G, Huth-Bocks A: The influence of domestic violence on the development of the attachment relationship between mother and young child. Psychoanal Psychol 2011; 28:512–527
38.
de Castro F: Psychoanalytic research using longitudinal studies: an inquiry on the developmental impact of early maternal projections. J Am Psychoanal Assoc 2007; 55:243–253
39.
Stern D: The Motherhood Constellation: A Unified View of Parent-Infant Psychotherapy. New York, Basic Books, 1995

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 138 - 146
PubMed: 26357872

History

Received: 6 February 2015
Revision received: 11 May 2015
Revision received: 9 June 2015
Accepted: 15 June 2015
Published online: 11 September 2015
Published in print: February 01, 2016

Authors

Details

John H. Porcerelli, Ph.D.
From the Department of Family Medicine and Public Health Sciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit; the Department of Psychology, Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti; the Department of Psychology, Wichita State University, Wichita, Kan.; and the Department of Psychology, University of Detroit Mercy, Detroit.
Alissa Huth-Bocks, Ph.D.
From the Department of Family Medicine and Public Health Sciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit; the Department of Psychology, Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti; the Department of Psychology, Wichita State University, Wichita, Kan.; and the Department of Psychology, University of Detroit Mercy, Detroit.
Steven K. Huprich, Ph.D.
From the Department of Family Medicine and Public Health Sciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit; the Department of Psychology, Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti; the Department of Psychology, Wichita State University, Wichita, Kan.; and the Department of Psychology, University of Detroit Mercy, Detroit.
Laura Richardson, Ph.D.
From the Department of Family Medicine and Public Health Sciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit; the Department of Psychology, Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti; the Department of Psychology, Wichita State University, Wichita, Kan.; and the Department of Psychology, University of Detroit Mercy, Detroit.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Porcerelli ([email protected]).
Presented in part at the 2014 National Meeting of the American Psychoanalytic Association, New York, Jan. 14–19, 2014.

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

American Psychoanalytic Fund for Psychoanalytic Research:
Supported by grants from the American Psychoanalytic Fund for Psychoanalytic Research and the International Psychoanalytic Association to Dr. Huth-Bocks.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share