Skip to main content
Full access
Letters to the Editor
Published Online: 1 August 2019

Large Sample Sizes Cannot Compensate for Mismeasured Environments in Gene-by-Environment Research

To the Editor: We respect Border et al.’s ambitious undertaking (1), which was published in the May 2019 issue of the Journal. We endorse greater scrutiny of novel effects within the literature on gene-by-environment (G×E) interactions, including replication using highly powered methods. We argue, however, that the approach used here to measure the environment—several dichotomized questionnaire items—is insufficient to draw conclusions, despite probing measurement error. Evidence for the importance of the environment for depression is well established (i.e., approximately 63% of liability; [2]), and vulnerability-stress models of depression are widely endorsed and long precede G×E research. However, G×E research has failed to invest in measuring the environment (3); we urge adoption of “E” assessment that matches the rigor of “G” assessment.
Before G×E research, stress interview measures emerged as gold-standard measures among researchers studying stress and depression. Indeed, one 1998 review indicated, “today, use of life event checklists designed to rate the presence or absence of a finite number of events has largely been abandoned” (4, p. 301). With the advent of G×E research, a need for quick measures resurrected questionnaire measurement. Although a full rationale is beyond this letter (5), appropriate measures maximally disambiguate stress exposure from response, account for investigator-rated severity, distinguish interpersonal from noninterpersonal stress, attend to events’ depressogenic time-frame (<3 months), and establish temporal precedence.
Why is it insufficient to use a large N and estimate the impact of measurement error? First, even in their “catastrophic” simulations, the authors vastly underestimate the amount of random error introduced by inadequate stress measures. One estimate suggests that questionnaires accounted for only 16% of variance of interview measures (6), even without artificially dichotomizing questionnaires as done here. Would the field tolerate such poor validity measurement of genotypes? Second, large samples address random error but not systematic error. Specifically, the authors’ approach does not account for findings that different types of stress confer significantly different unique variance for depression (7), are poor indicators of each other (major interpersonal compared with noninterpersonal events, r=0.04 and r=0.32, respectively; [7, 8]), and can produce G×E effect sizes in opposite directions. For example, in a simultaneous model, a significant interpersonal major event G×E effect was the opposite direction from the trending noninterpersonal major event G×E effect (9). Similarly, a serotonergic multilocus score produced significantly stronger G×E effects for major interpersonal events than other event categories (full results available from the authors).
Further, differential susceptibility theory (10) suggests that some genetic variants—including many in the present study—confer sensitivity to the environment for better and worse, meaning the same genetic variant may have opposite effects depending on environmental conditions. Differential susceptibility renders the effects of many variants uninterpretable without the accurate capture of environmental effects. The field has attempted to address inconsistent findings with ever-growing sample sizes. To the extent that they create the need to rely on inadequate “envirotyping,” dazzling sample sizes not only fail to increase the likelihood of the detection of real G×E effects for differential susceptibility variants—they may completely obscure them.

References

1.
Border R, Johnson EC, Evans LM, et al: No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples. Am J Psychiatry 2019; 176:376–387
2.
Sullivan PF, Neale MC, Kendler KS: Genetic epidemiology of major depression: review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:1552–1562
3.
Monroe SM, Reid MW: Gene-environment interactions in depression research: genetic polymorphisms and life-stress polyprocedures. Psychol Sci 2008; 19:947–956
4.
Mazure CM: Life stressors as risk factors in depression. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 1998; 5:291–313
5.
Harkness KL, Monroe SM: The assessment and measurement of adult life stress: basic premises, operational principles, and design requirements. J Abnorm Psychol 2016; 125:727–745
6.
McQuaid JR, Monroe SM, Roberts JR, et al: Toward the standardization of life stress assessment: definitional discrepancies and inconsistencies in methods. Stress Med 1992; 8:47–56
7.
Vrshek-Schallhorn S, Stroud CB, Mineka S, et al: Chronic and episodic interpersonal stress as statistically unique predictors of depression in two samples of emerging adults. J Abnorm Psychol 2015; 124:918–932
8.
Starr LR, Huang M: HPA-axis multilocus genetic variation moderates associations between environmental stress and depressive symptoms among adolescents. Dev Psychopathol (Epub ahead of print, Nov 5, 2018)
9.
Vrshek-Schallhorn S, Mineka S, Zinbarg RE, et al: Refining the candidate environment: interpersonal stress, the serotonin transporter polymorphism, and gene-environment interactions in major depression. Clin Psychol Sci 2014; 2:235–248
10.
Belsky J, Pluess M: Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull 2009; 135:885–908

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 667 - 668
PubMed: 31366222

History

Accepted: 6 May 2019
Published online: 1 August 2019
Published in print: August 01, 2019

Keywords

  1. Stress
  2. Genetics
  3. Biological Markers
  4. Unipolar Depression

Authors

Affiliations

Suzanne Vrshek-Schallhorn, Ph.D. [email protected]
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro (Vrshek-Schallhorn, Corneau); Department of Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, N.Y. (Starr).
Gail M. Corneau, M.A.
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro (Vrshek-Schallhorn, Corneau); Department of Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, N.Y. (Starr).
Lisa R. Starr, Ph.D. [email protected]
Department of Psychology, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, Greensboro (Vrshek-Schallhorn, Corneau); Department of Psychology, University of Rochester, Rochester, N.Y. (Starr).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Vrshek-Schallhorn ([email protected]) and Dr. Starr ([email protected]).

Funding Information

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share