Skip to main content
Full access
Letters to the Editor
Published Online: 1 August 2019

Measurement Error Cannot Account for Failed Replications of Historic Candidate Gene-by-Environment Hypotheses: Response to Vrshek-Schallhorn et al.

To the Editor: Vrshek-Schallhorn et al. dispute our conclusion that historic candidate gene-by-environment (G×E) hypotheses were incorrect, criticizing the measures of environmental stressors we employed. We appreciate the opportunity to respond to their points.
We agree with their point that measures of environment are important, as we emphasized in our article (1). Our results demonstrate that both interpersonal and noninterpersonal environmental measures influence depression liability (see Table S6 in the online supplement to our article). The additive effects of these environmental measures replicate previous findings that a variety of stressors affect depression liability. However, Vrshek-Schallhorn et al. suggest that the measures we used are completely obscuring real candidate G×E effects. Below, we argue that this cannot be the case and that several of their comments are inaccurate.
They refer to the use of artificially dichotomized questionnaires, implying that we chose arbitrary cutoff points for continuous measures. In actuality, the binary stress measures we examined were inherently dichotomous, indicating whether or not participants endorsed one of a handful of events. For example, exposure to trauma in childhood was coded affirmatively if participants stated that they had been subject to sexual or physical abuse in childhood. On the other hand, we did not dichotomize the Townsend deprivation index, a continuous measure of socioeconomic hardship. In addition, Vrshek-Schallhorn et al. state that our worst-case-scenario measurement error simulations “vastly underestimate the amount of random error introduced by inadequate stress measures,” suggesting that a noisy stress questionnaire measure might account for as little as 16% of the variance of its adequately measured analogue. In response, we introduced this degree of error variance in both the depression and stress measures in our simulations. Even with this extreme degree of error, we still observed greater than 90% power to detect even modest interaction effects by candidate gene standards (odds ratio≥1.26).
Systematic error can influence results, as Vrshek-Schallhorn et al. assert. However, the several severe systematic measurement error regimes we examined (see section S4.3.3 in the online supplement to our article) demonstrate that misclassification of environment measures cannot account for the lack of candidate gene or candidate G×E replication. In truth, we are unable to construct a plausible measurement error model that reconciles the validity of previously reported candidate gene findings with our observations that every stressor we examined evidenced substantial, highly significant effects on every depression measure (see section S6 in the online supplement to our article) but that no candidate gene polymorphism or stressor-by-polymorphism interaction had detectable effects, despite ∼100% power across a broad array of measurement error scenarios. This is not to say that measurement error is unimportant. However, with respect to the large effects reported in the candidate gene literature in small samples, measurement error cannot account for the lack of support for historical candidate gene hypotheses in our study (1) or in other large, collaborative studies (2) that have investigated the genetic underpinnings of depression, even in carefully phenotyped studies specifically testing genome-wide G×E hypotheses (3). Instead, the most plausible explanation for these failures to replicate is that the original candidate gene findings were false positives.
Vrshek-Schallhorn et al. also state that we failed to distinguish between weakly correlated types of stress that may interact in different ways with genetic variants. The great majority of scenarios wherein either or both of these types of stress interact with a candidate genetic variant should still produce detectable genetic variant main effects—as noted, none were detected (see section S7 in the online supplement to our article). In the unlikely case of a complete crossover interaction where the variant has no main effect, any interactions detectable in candidate G×E samples as small as those cited by our critics would in turn induce detectable differences in variance across genotypes in large samples, even allowing for extreme measurement error. We found no evidence for such heteroscedasticity (results available upon request).
The trajectory of increasing complexity in candidate gene research follows a pattern of reactions to repeated replication failures. In the 1990s, it was hypothesized that specific, common polymorphisms within serotonergic and other neurotransmitter genes would explain substantial variation in depression liability. In the 2000s, it was hypothesized that moderation of genetic effects by environmental stressors would explain inconsistent main effect findings. Now, Vrshek-Schallhorn et al. suggest that our null findings can be explained by different types of stressors that produce interaction effects in opposite directions, or by catastrophic measurement error, and that their hypotheses cannot be adequately tested in existing well-powered samples. We instead suggest that these lines of inquiry are fundamentally flawed; neither the notion that common variants have large effects on complex traits nor the relevance of these specific candidate polymorphisms to depression is supported by well-powered studies (15).
The genetic architectures of traits such as depression have revealed themselves to be hyperpolygenic—thousands of common variants influence liability, each exerting an effect far too small to be detected in conventional samples, even with precise phenotypic measurement. The era of using small samples to investigate specific common genetic variants based on historical biological hypotheses that are unsupported by genome-wide data is drawing to a close.

References

1.
Border R, Johnson EC, Evans LM, et al: No support for historical candidate gene or candidate gene-by-interaction hypotheses for major depression across multiple large samples. Am J Psychiatry 2019; 176:376–387
2.
Culverhouse RC, Saccone NL, Horton AC, et al: Collaborative meta-analysis finds no evidence of a strong interaction between stress and 5-HTTLPR genotype contributing to the development of depression. Mol Psychiatry 2018; 23:133–142
3.
Peterson RE, Cai N, Dahl AW, et al: Molecular genetic analysis subdivided by adversity exposure suggests etiologic heterogeneity in major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175:545–554
4.
CONVERGE consortium: Sparse whole-genome sequencing identifies two loci for major depressive disorder. Nature 2015; 523:588–591
5.
Wray NR, Ripke S, Mattheisen M, et al: Genome-wide association analyses identify 44 risk variants and refine the genetic architecture of major depression. Nat Genet 2018; 50:668–681

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 668 - 669
PubMed: 31366224

History

Accepted: 6 May 2019
Published online: 1 August 2019
Published in print: August 01, 2019

Keywords

  1. Stress
  2. Genetics
  3. Biological Markers

Authors

Affiliations

Richard Border, M.A., M.S. [email protected]
Institute for Behavioral Genetics (Border, Evans, Keller), Department of Psychology and Neuroscience (Border, Keller), and Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology (Evans), University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder; Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis (Johnson).
Emma C. Johnson, Ph.D.
Institute for Behavioral Genetics (Border, Evans, Keller), Department of Psychology and Neuroscience (Border, Keller), and Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology (Evans), University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder; Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis (Johnson).
Luke M. Evans, Ph.D.
Institute for Behavioral Genetics (Border, Evans, Keller), Department of Psychology and Neuroscience (Border, Keller), and Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology (Evans), University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder; Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis (Johnson).
Matthew C. Keller, Ph.D. [email protected]
Institute for Behavioral Genetics (Border, Evans, Keller), Department of Psychology and Neuroscience (Border, Keller), and Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology (Evans), University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder; Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis (Johnson).

Notes

Send correspondence to Mr. Border ([email protected]) and Dr. Keller ([email protected]).

Funding Information

The authors’ disclosures accompany the original article.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

There are no citations for this item

View Options

View options

PDF/ePub

View PDF/ePub

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share