Skip to main content
Full access
Editorials
Published Online: 1 October 2023

Maternal Perinatal Stress Associated With Offspring Negative Emotionality, But the Underlying Mechanisms Remain Elusive

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry
Anxiety and depression—the internalizing disorders—typically emerge in the first three decades of life (1, 2). They are among the most prevalent and burdensome mental illnesses, affecting ∼580 million individuals worldwide (3, 4). In the United States, nearly one in three individuals will experience an anxiety disorder in their lifetime (5), and one in five will experience a depressive disorder (6). Direct spending on anxiety and depression treatment exceeds $100 billion annually (7). Existing treatments are far from curative for many, fueling the search for new etiological insights and new targets for intervention (814).
The roots of internalizing illness extend into the earliest chapters of human development, when the brain and behavior are likely most plastic. There is ample evidence that caregiver behavior in the first half decade of life can have a profound and enduring impact on both neurobiological and psychological development and, ultimately, on lifelong health and well-being (1517). The consistent provision of supportive, sensitive, and engaged care is essential for healthy emotional development and provides a buffer against the potentially toxic effects of childhood challenges, setbacks, and adversities (15, 18). Not surprisingly, clinicians and researchers have shown a long-standing interest in understanding the role of parents and other caregivers in the development, treatment, and prevention of internalizing disorders (16). Recent work highlights the potentially important role of perinatal caregiver stress in promoting child psychopathology, but the exact nature of this association remains unclear (15, 19).
From this perspective, the new report from Marr and colleagues in this issue of the Journal is a welcome addition to the literature (20). Leveraging a composite index of maternal stress that was repeatedly assessed across pregnancy and the early postnatal period in a small but demographically diverse sample of American mothers (N=115), the authors used an innovative machine-learning approach and identified—in a completely data-driven way—six distinct patterns of perinatal maternal stress (21). Two of the patterns predominantly reflected overall differences in stress severity, effectively clustering the mothers into groups with comparatively high or low levels of stress. The other four patterns reflected differences in the temporal dynamics of perinatal stress. Here, the mothers were split into groups characterized by distinct stress trajectories, including a middle-pregnancy peak (trajectory 2), a late-pregnancy peak (trajectory 1), a middle- and late-pregnancy trough (trajectory 3), and an inverted-U-shaped pattern (trajectory 4). Follow-up analyses demonstrated that the four stress-trajectory groups were demographically indistinguishable, suggesting that this novel classification might provide a useful additional source of information about children’s early-life experience. But are the trajectories a reproducible feature of maternal experience or simply an idiosyncratic feature of the data at hand? A key strength of Marr and colleagues’ study was the use of a second, much larger sample of Finnish mothers (N=2,156) to address this key question. Of the four stress trajectories identified in the U.S. sample, trajectories 1, 2, and 3 were replicated in the Finnish sample.
To clarify the consequences of mothers’ perinatal stress on their infant’s emotional development, Marr and colleagues modeled prospective associations between the replicable stress patterns and longitudinal measures of infant negative emotionality that were acquired over the first 2 years of life (22). Negative emotionality is a particularly interesting phenotype because children who are prone to feelings of fear, irritability, and sadness are more likely to develop internalizing disorders and other forms of psychopathology as they age (16, 2325). Latent growth curve analyses indicated that the average level of infant negative emotionality showed a developmentally appropriate increase across the first year of life, before declining in the second (26). Among the stress patterns examined, trajectory 3 (a trough in maternal stress during middle and late pregnancy) was uniquely associated with longitudinal changes in negative emotionality of the offspring. Follow-up analyses demonstrated that the infants of mothers in the trajectory 3 group showed significantly lower levels of negative emotionality than those in the other groups by the end of their first year. Consistent with the American results, the offspring of mothers in Finland with the same longitudinal pattern of stress (trajectory 3) showed consistently dampened levels of negative emotionality across their first 2 years, with the differences reaching significance at ages 6, 12, and 24 months. In short, American and Finnish mothers reporting a trough in stress during the middle and late stages of pregnancy gave birth to children with lower levels of dispositional fear, shyness, anger, and sadness as infants and toddlers, and this prospective association could not be explained by demographic confounders, the average severity of maternal stress experienced during pregnancy, or the intensity of maternal stress during the postpartum follow-up period. These observations—which capitalize on over 2,000 families, drawn from two continents and intensively studied for nearly 3 years—motivate the hypothesis that diminished levels of maternal stress in the later stages of pregnancy foster the development of children who are less likely to develop emotional disorders later in life. These findings reinforce the idea that “timing matters” when it comes to early-life stress and are well aligned with recent commentaries emphasizing the importance of parsing heterogeneity in both the nature and the developmental timing of stress for understanding psychopathology (15, 2730).
At first blush, Marr and colleagues’ findings seem to reinforce calls for an increased investment in interventions aimed at strengthening early-life caregiving and maternal well-being (15), but they also raise questions about whom to target. On the one hand, across the six perinatal stress patterns identified by Marr’s team, only trajectory 3—experienced by less than a quarter of the American mothers (22.6%)—was consistently associated with variation in offspring temperament, after controlling for ongoing levels of postpartum maternal stress. On the other hand, in analyses that did not control for postpartum maternal stress, the overall severity of perinatal stress was associated with significantly elevated infant negative emotionality. Taken together, these findings suggest that mothers who report chronically high levels of stress, either before or after giving birth, have children who are at greater risk for developing emotional disorders. The latter results are consistent with a wealth of other work and motivate the development and refinement of targeted, universal, and public-policy interventions (15, 19). Of course, as in nearly all prospective, longitudinal research studies, Marr and colleagues’ observations do not permit causal inferences. In addition to their immense practical value, well-designed intervention studies would afford a crucial opportunity to test whether reducing mothers’ perinatal stress is associated with positive consequences for infant temperament and, ultimately, mental health.
Marr and colleagues’ results raise a host of challenging questions for future research. Are the observed changes in early-life temperament associated with meaningful differences in the children’s emotional health in adolescence and emerging adulthood? Are the offspring of mothers who experience the middle- and late-pregnancy trough in stress, for instance, less likely to develop internalizing disorders? How exactly might subtle nuances in the dynamics of perinatal stress shape children’s early-life emotional development? Are similar associations evident for fathers? To what extent are these prospective phenotypic associations a consequence of genetic confounding (3133)? What exactly is meant by “stress” in this context? After all, the composite index of perinatal stress used in the American sample reflected a mixture of mood and anxiety symptoms; neuroticism; perceptions of uncertainty, uncontrollability, and overloading; difficulties coping with or regulating distress; and exposure to strains, hassles, and negative life events. Unpacking this conceptual complexity and demonstrating incremental validity over straightforward measures of maternal temperament and internalizing symptoms are important avenues for future research (34).
From a neurobiological angle, we might wonder how maternal stress “gets under the skin” to shape the development of the neural circuitry underlying negative emotionality in the offspring. Marr and colleagues provide a preliminary answer to this question. Leveraging fMRI data collected from a subset of the American infants at 1 month of age, they investigated potential differences in the intrinsic functional connectivity of the amygdala. The amygdala is particularly relevant because it is implicated in the development of anxiety and depression (3537), is thought to mediate the impact of early-life adversities on negative emotionality (38), and shows a protracted course of postnatal development (39, 40). Among other findings, Marr and colleagues’ results suggest that the infants of mothers experiencing a trough in stress during middle and late pregnancy (trajectory 3) showed stronger functional connectivity between the amygdala and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, a region thought to play a mechanistically important role in processing safety cues; biasing the amygdala; and regulating fear, anxiety, and other negative emotions (41). This prospective association between maternal stress and infant brain function remained significant while controlling for the overall severity of perinatal stress, the other three maternal stress trajectories, and nuisance variance in gestational age at birth and infant age at the time of scanning. Collecting usable fMRI data from children is notoriously difficult, and the authors are to be commended for what was, in all likelihood, a Herculean scientific exercise (42). Nevertheless, it is clear that these findings—which capitalize on just 5.5 minutes of imaging data acquired from 60 infants who were further categorized, on the basis of their mothers’ perinatal stress patterns, into six subgroups (N=9–32) (M. Marr, personal communication, July 16, 2023)—represent a provisional hypothesis that will need to be rigorously replicated in larger and more nationally representative samples (43, 44). Addressing this challenge is likely to be facilitated by pooling data via new or existing biobanks (e.g., Healthy Brain and Child Development Study [HBCD]) or research consortia (e.g., Enhancing Neuroimaging Genetics Through Meta-Analysis [ENIGMA]). It is also worth remembering that the amygdala is a heterogeneous collection of nuclei linked by a network of microcircuits (45). Understanding the relevance of the amygdala to early-life stress and psychiatric risk requires that future studies more fully embrace this neuroanatomical complexity. Adopting more precise definitions of stress (4648) and developing integrative cross-species models will also be crucial for understanding the molecular and cellular mechanisms that link variation in maternal experience to childhood mental health and disease (4952).
Internalizing symptoms, diagnoses, and treatment seeking are surging among youths and young adults (5356), drawing the attention of clinicians, scientists, insurers, and policymakers—from the Surgeon General to the President of the United States (5759). Although the underlying mechanisms remain elusive, Marr and colleagues’ study underscores the importance of developing a more complete understanding of the role that perinatal stress plays in the development of internalizing illness. Addressing this challenge will require a greater emphasis on mechanistically informative studies—including intervention, quasi-experimental, and genetic approaches—and the investment of resources commensurate with the established importance of children’s early emotional development for their lifelong health and well-being (1517, 33, 60).

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge assistance and critical feedback from K. DeYoung, E. Fried, L. Friedman, H. Goldsmith, G. Hancock, N. Kalin, M. Marr, and J. Smith.

References

1.
Lee FS, Heimer H, Giedd JN, et al: Adolescent mental health: opportunity and obligation. Science 2014; 346:547–549
2.
Solmi M, Radua J, Olivola M, et al: Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol Psychiatry 2022; 27:281–295
3.
Global Burden of Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors Study 2019 Mental Disorders Collaborators: Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry 2022; 9:137–150
4.
Institute for Health Metrics and Evaluation: Global Burden of Disease Compare: Global Causes of Years Lived with Disability, 2019. Seattle, University of Washington, 2023. https://vizhub.healthdata.org/gbd-compare/
5.
National Comorbidity Study–Replication: Lifetime Prevalence of DSM-IV/WMH-CIDI Disorders by Sex and Cohort. 2007. https://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/ncs/ftpdir/NCS-R_Lifetime_Prevalence_Estimates.pdf
6.
Hasin DS, Sarvet AL, Meyers JL, et al: Epidemiology of adult DSM-5 major depressive disorder and its specifiers in the United States. JAMA Psychiatry 2018; 75:336–346
7.
Dieleman JL, Cao J, Chapin A, et al: US health care spending by payer and health condition, 1996-2016. JAMA 2020; 323:863–884
8.
Craske MG, Stein MB, Eley TC, et al: Anxiety disorders. Nat Rev Dis Primers 2017; 3:17024
9.
Otte C, Gold SM, Penninx BW, et al: Major depressive disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers 2016; 2:16065
10.
Garakani A, Murrough JW, Freire RC, et al: Pharmacotherapy of anxiety disorders: current and emerging treatment options. Front Psychiatry 2020; 11:595584
11.
Sartori SB, Singewald N: Novel pharmacological targets in drug development for the treatment of anxiety and anxiety-related disorders. Pharmacol Ther 2019; 204:107402
12.
James AC, Reardon T, Soler A, et al: Cognitive behavioural therapy for anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2020; 11:CD013162
13.
Ginsburg GS, Becker-Haimes EM, Keeton C, et al: Results from the Child/Adolescent Anxiety Multimodal Extended Long-Term Study (CAMELS): primary anxiety outcomes. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2018; 57:471–480
14.
Singewald N, Sartori SB, Reif A, et al: Alleviating anxiety and taming trauma: novel pharmacotherapeutics for anxiety disorders and posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropharmacology 2023; 226:109418
15.
Luby JL, Rogers C, McLaughlin KA: Environmental conditions to promote healthy childhood brain/behavioral development: informing early preventive interventions for delivery in routine care. Biol Psychiatry Glob Open Sci 2022; 2:233–241
16.
Rapee RM, Creswell C, Kendall PC, et al: Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: a summary and overview of the literature. Behav Res Ther 2023; 168:104376
17.
Gee DG, Cohodes EM: Caregiving influences on development: a sensitive period for biological embedding of predictability and safety cues. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2021; 30:376–383
18.
Kahhalé I, Barry KR, Hanson JL: Positive parenting moderates associations between childhood stress and corticolimbic structure. PNAS Nexus 2023; 2:pgad145
19.
Van den Bergh BRH, van den Heuvel MI, Lahti M, et al: Prenatal developmental origins of behavior and mental health: the influence of maternal stress in pregnancy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2020; 117:26–64
20.
Marr MC, Graham AM, Feczko E, et al: Maternal perinatal stress trajectories and negative affect and amygdala development in offspring. Am J Psychiatry 2023; 180:766–777
21.
Feczko E, Miranda-Dominguez O, Marr M, et al: The heterogeneity problem: approaches to identify psychiatric subtypes. Trends Cogn Sci 2019; 23:584–601
22.
Shiner RL: What are the dimensions and bases for lasting individual differences in emotion? in The Nature of Emotion, 2nd ed. Edited by Fox AS, Lapate RC, Shackman AJ, et al. New York, Oxford University Press, 2018, pp 61–64
23.
Klein DN, Dougherty LR, Kessel EM, et al: A transdiagnostic perspective on youth irritability. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2021; 30:437–443
24.
Tan E, Zeytinoglu S, Morales S, et al: Social versus non-social behavioral inhibition: differential prediction from early childhood of long-term psychosocial outcomes. Dev Sci (Online ahead of print, June 22, 2023)
25.
Fox AS, Kalin NH: A translational neuroscience approach to understanding the development of social anxiety disorder and its pathophysiology. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:1162–1173
26.
Beesdo K, Knappe S, Pine DS: Anxiety and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: developmental issues and implications for DSM-V. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2009; 32:483–524
27.
Gee DG: Early adversity and development: parsing heterogeneity and identifying pathways of risk and resilience. Am J Psychiatry 2021; 178:998–1013
28.
Cohodes EM, Kitt ER, Baskin-Sommers A, et al: Influences of early-life stress on frontolimbic circuitry: harnessing a dimensional approach to elucidate the effects of heterogeneity in stress exposure. Dev Psychobiol 2021; 63:153–172
29.
McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Humphreys KL, et al: The value of dimensional models of early experience: thinking clearly about concepts and categories. Perspect Psychol Sci 2021; 16:1463–1472
30.
Smith KE, Pollak SD: Rethinking concepts and categories for understanding the neurodevelopmental effects of childhood adversity. Perspect Psychol Sci 2021; 16:67–93
31.
Scarr S, McCartney K: How people make their own environments: a theory of genotype greater than environment effects. Child Dev 1983; 54:424–435
32.
Hart SA, Little C, van Bergen E: Nurture might be nature: cautionary tales and proposed solutions. NPJ Sci Learn 2021; 6:2
33.
Rice F: The intergenerational transmission of anxiety disorders and major depression. Am J Psychiatry 2022; 179:596–598
34.
Shackman AJ, Fox AS: Getting serious about variation: lessons for clinical neuroscience (a commentary on “The myth of optimality in clinical neuroscience”). Trends Cogn Sci 2018; 22:368–369
35.
Grogans SE, Fox AS, Shackman AJ: The amygdala and depression: a sober reconsideration. Am J Psychiatry 2022; 179:454–457
36.
Hur J, Stockbridge MD, Fox AS, et al: Dispositional negativity, cognition, and anxiety disorders: an integrative translational neuroscience framework. Prog Brain Res 2019; 247:375–436
37.
Shackman AJ, Fox AS: Two decades of anxiety neuroimaging research: new insights and a look to the future. Am J Psychiatry 2021; 178:106–109
38.
Fox AS, Oler JA, Shackman AJ, et al: Intergenerational neural mediators of early-life anxious temperament. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2015; 112:9118–9122
39.
Avino TA, Barger N, Vargas MV, et al: Neuron numbers increase in the human amygdala from birth to adulthood, but not in autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2018; 115:3710–3715
40.
Chareyron LJ, Banta Lavenex P, Amaral DG, et al: Life and death of immature neurons in the juvenile and adult primate amygdala. Int J Mol Sci 2021; 22:6691
41.
Tashjian SM, Zbozinek TD, Mobbs D: A decision architecture for safety computations. Trends Cogn Sci 2021; 25:342–354
42.
Pfeifer JH, Allen NB, Byrne ML, et al: Modeling developmental change: contemporary approaches to key methodological challenges in developmental neuroimaging. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2018; 33:1–4
43.
Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, et al: Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature 2022; 603:654–660
44.
LeWinn KZ, Sheridan MA, Keyes KM, et al: Sample composition alters associations between age and brain structure. Nat Commun 2017; 8:874
45.
Fox AS, Oler JA, Tromp DPM, et al: Extending the amygdala in theories of threat processing. Trends Neurosci 2015; 38:319–329
46.
Kagan J: Why stress remains an ambiguous concept: reply to McEwen and McEwen (2016) and Cohen et al (2016). Perspect Psychol Sci 2016; 11:464–465
47.
Kagan J: An overly permissive extension. Perspect Psychol Sci 2016; 11:442–450
48.
Fox AS, Lapate RC, Davidson RJ, et al: The nature of emotion: a research agenda for the 21st century; in The Nature of Emotion Fundamental Questions, 2nd ed. Edited by Fox AS, Lapate RC, Shackman AJ, et al. New York, Oxford University Press, 2018, pp 403–417
49.
Fox AS, Shackman AJ: The central extended amygdala in fear and anxiety: closing the gap between mechanistic and neuroimaging research. Neurosci Lett 2019; 693:58–67
50.
Meyer HC, Odriozola P, Cohodes EM, et al: Ventral hippocampus interacts with prelimbic cortex during inhibition of threat response via learned safety in both mice and humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2019; 116:26970–26979
51.
Callaghan B, Meyer H, Opendak M, et al: Using a developmental ecology framework to align fear neurobiology across species. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2019; 15:345–369
52.
Meyer HC, Fields A, Vannucci A, et al: The added value of crosstalk between developmental circuit neuroscience and clinical practice to inform the treatment of adolescent anxiety. Biol Psychiatry Glob Open Sci 2022; 3:169–178
53.
Brody DJ, Gu Q: Antidepressant use among adults: United States, 2015–2018. National Center for Health Statistics Data Brief 2020; 377:1–8
54.
Binkley C, Fenn L: Colleges Struggle with Soaring Student Demand for Counseling. Associated Press, November 19, 2019. https://apnews.com/article/depression-anxiety-mental-health-25905a5c3d28454ba0d84dcb958fe32c
55.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration: Results from the 2018 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Key Substance Use and Mental Health Indicators in the United States:. Rockville, Md, Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, 2019
56.
Underwood JM, Brener N, Thornton J, et al: Overview and methods for the youth risk behavior surveillance system: United States, 2019. MMWR Suppl 2020; 69:1–10
57.
Office of the President of the United States: President Biden to Announce Strategy to Address Our National Mental Health Crisis, as Part of Unity Agenda in His First State of the Union. Washington, DC, White House, 2022. https://www.whitehouse.gov/briefing-room/statements-releases/2022/03/01/fact-sheet-president-biden-to-announce-strategy-to-address-our-national-mental-health-crisis-as-part-of-unity-agenda-in-his-first-state-of-the-union
58.
US Surgeon General: Protecting Youth Mental Health: The US Surgeon General’s Advisory. Washington, DC, US Department of Health and Human Services, 2021. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK575984/
59.
BlueCross BlueShield: New Research Underscores Mental Health Challenges Facing American Youth. Chicago, BlueCross BlueShield, 2023. https://www.bcbs.com/the-health-of-america/articles/new-research-underscores-mental-health-challenges-facing-american
60.
Ridley M, Rao G, Schilbach F, et al: Poverty, depression, and anxiety: causal evidence and mechanisms. Science 2020; 370:eaay0214

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 708 - 711

History

Received: 11 August 2023
Accepted: 14 August 2023
Published online: 1 October 2023
Published in print: October 1, 2023

Keywords

  1. Anxiety and Depression
  2. Developmental Psychopathology
  3. Intergenerational Transmission
  4. Negative Emotionality
  5. Neuroimaging
  6. Stress

Authors

Details

Alexander J. Shackman, Ph.D. [email protected]
Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, and Maryland Neuroimaging Center, University of Maryland, College Park (Shackman); Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn. (Gee).
Dylan G. Gee, Ph.D. [email protected]
Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, and Maryland Neuroimaging Center, University of Maryland, College Park (Shackman); Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, Conn. (Gee).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Shackman ([email protected]) and Dr. Gee ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

Supported in part by NIH grants AA030042, MH131264, MH121409, DA041174, and MH115113; National Science Foundation grants 2145372 and 2145372; and University of Maryland, College Park.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share