Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective:

Deficits in social cognition consistently underlie functional disabilities in a wide range of psychiatric disorders. Neuroimaging studies have suggested that the anterior insula is a “common core” brain region that is impaired across neurological and psychiatric disorders, which include social cognition deficits. Nevertheless, neurobiological mechanisms of the anterior insula for social cognition remain elusive. This study aims to fill this knowledge gap.

Methods:

To determine the role of the anterior insula in social cognition, the authors manipulated expression of Cyp26B1, an anterior insula-enriched molecule that is crucial for retinoic acid degradation and is involved in the pathology of neuropsychiatric conditions. Social cognition was mainly assayed using the three-chamber social interaction test. Multimodal analyses were conducted at the molecular, cellular, circuitry, and behavioral levels.

Results:

At the molecular and cellular level, anterior insula–mediated social novelty recognition is maintained by proper activity of the layer 5 pyramidal neurons, for which retinoic acid–mediated gene transcription can play a role. The authors also demonstrate that oxytocin influences the anterior insula–mediated social novelty recognition, although not by direct projection of oxytocin neurons, nor by direct diffusion of oxytocin to the anterior insula, which contrasts with the modes of oxytocin regulation onto the posterior insula. Instead, oxytocin affects oxytocin receptor–expressing neurons in the dorsal raphe nucleus, where serotonergic neurons are projected to the anterior insula. Furthermore, the authors show that serotonin 5-HT2C receptor expressed in the anterior insula influences social novelty recognition.

Conclusions:

The anterior insula plays a pivotal role in social novelty recognition that is partly regulated by a local retinoic acid cascade but also remotely regulated by oxytocin via a long-range circuit mechanism.
Social cognition is critical for human behavior in complex social environments (1, 2), and deficits in social cognition consistently underlie functional disabilities in individuals suffering from a wide range of psychiatric disorders (37). Social novelty recognition (the ability to recognize social cues), a key subdomain of social cognition, can be assessed by performance-based measures of social cue (e.g., face or voice perception) in humans (8, 9) as well as by social novelty recognition/preference paradigms (three-chamber social interaction test and five-trial social novelty recognition test) in rodents (10, 11).
The insular cortex was one of the least understood brain regions until recently (1214). The advent of functional imaging techniques has unveiled its multiple functions in humans, which include interoception, emotion, cognition, and motivation (1517). These studies, including a recent meta-analysis, have suggested that the insular cortex is a “common core” brain region that is impaired across neurological and psychiatric disorders (2, 12, 15, 1820). The insular cortex can be divided into the anterior insula (AI) and the posterior insula (PI). They differ from each other in cytoarchitecture, connections, and functions, but they are also tightly interconnected. The PI shows a classical six-layer structure, whereas the AI lacks granular layer 4 (12). The PI receives multiple sensory inputs (12, 20), whereas the AI makes heavy reciprocal connections with the frontal areas and limbic system and participates in the salience network (20).
Oxytocin (OT) is a well-appreciated neuropeptide that influences social behavior, including social recognition, pair bonding, mating, and parental care (2123). OT modulates these behaviors by being released into the target brain regions, such as the amygdala, ventral tegmental area, and dorsal raphe, from the axonal fibers of oxytocinergic neurons located in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and supraoptic nucleus (SON) (24, 25). Animal studies for key molecules of the OT signaling pathway, such as OT, the OT receptor (OTR), and CD38 (cluster of differentiation 38, transmembrane glycoprotein with ADP-ribosyl cyclase activity that facilitates OT secretion), consistently support its critical role in social behaviors (22, 26, 27). When abundant OT release occurs in the brain, for instance during lactation, the AI is one of the most robustly activated brain regions (28, 29). Nevertheless, quantitative analyses of the distribution of OT fibers in multiple brain regions have found that the OT projections to the AI are negligible in multiple species (24, 30). In contrast, OT projection and rich expression of OTR binding are found in the PI (24, 31). Thus, OT signaling may be distinct between the PI and AI. Altogether, the mechanism that underlies the activation of the AI in response to OT in humans remains to be elucidated.
We and others have proposed the importance of using animal models in research to discover novel translatable mechanisms at the molecular and cellular levels in this understudied brain region (12). Outstanding studies with rodents that explore roles of the insular cortex, in particular focusing on the PI, have indeed emerged in the past several years: at least in rodents, the PI is involved in emotional processing, and the resultant social behavior in response to socially affective stimuli is mediated by OTR-dependent neural activity (32, 33). Here, we address the aforementioned enigma from human imaging studies and decipher a role of AI in social behavior in association with OT signaling. We used an AI-enriched molecule, Cyp26B1, which is crucial for retinoic acid (RA) degradation and is involved in the pathology of neuropsychiatric conditions, as a molecular lead to decipher the role of AI in social cognition.

Methods

The experimental methods used in the study are described in the supplementary methods section in the online supplement.

Statistical Analysis

Data were analyzed using the statistical methods stated in each figure legend, and all statistical analyses were performed using SPSS. Sample sizes were chosen on the basis of previous studies. When parametric tests such as t tests and analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed, the assumptions of normality (normal distribution) and homogeneity (similar variance among groups) were tested using Shapiro-Wilk’s test and Levene’s test, respectively. Data were mostly analyzed by one-way ANOVAs, two-way ANOVAs, and two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs followed by post hoc test. One-way ANOVAs followed by Tukey’s multiple comparisons were performed for testing statistical differences between means of three or more independent groups. Two-way ANOVAs followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests were used when examining the influence of two different factors on a dependent variable. However, if any repeated factor was present, two-way repeated-measures ANOVAs followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests were used. For simple pairwise comparisons, Student’s t tests were used.

Results

The AI Underlies Social Novelty Recognition

We first investigated whether the AI is a critical brain area for social cognition in mice. We introduced bilateral excitotoxic lesions in either the AI or its adjacent brain region, the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), in C57BL6/J wild-type male mice (see Figure S1 in the online supplement). In the three-chamber social interaction test, normal mice prefer the enclosure with a social subject (mouse) (trial 1: sociability) and show a selective preference to a novel mouse compared with a familiar mouse (trial 2: social novelty recognition) (Figure 1A). We observed a deficit only in trial 2 in AI-lesioned mice, whereas OFC-lesioned mice behaved normally in both trials (Figure 1A). Thus, AI-lesioned mice, but not OFC-lesioned mice, exhibited a significant deficit in social novelty recognition but not in sociability in this assay. To address whether the deficit was specific to the social context, we employed a five-trial social novelty recognition paradigm guided by social or nonsocial cues. Both OFC-lesioned and AI-lesioned mice displayed normal behaviors for nonsocial cue (odor): gradual reduction of exploration time to a banana scent during four consecutive trials, but substantially increased exploration when the scent was changed to a strawberry scent on the fifth trial (Figure 1B). In the context of social cue, the OFC-lesioned mice also displayed a normal response (Figure 1C). In contrast, AI-lesioned mice showed significant deficits in response to social cue: absence of habituation during the first four trials and no increase in exploration when exposed to a new conspecific cue in the fifth trial (Figure 1C). To avoid the possibility that these AI-specific deficits are associated with olfactory function or locomotor activity, we examined the hidden food test and open field test and showed that these are not confounding factors (see Figure S2A,B in the online supplement). In addition, AI-lesioned female mice behaved similarly to male mice, displaying a deficit in social novelty recognition (see Figure S2C in the online supplement). Together, these results indicate that the AI plays a role in social novelty recognition in mice, which clearly differentiates it from the OFC.
FIGURE 1. The anterior insula (AI) underlies social novelty recognitiona
aIn panel A, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)–lesioned mice show normal performance in both sociability (trial 1) and social novelty recognition (trial 2) in the three-chamber social interaction test. AI-lesioned mice show a deficit in social novelty recognition (trial 2: control mice [AI-Con], p<0.001, and AI-lesion, p=0.103; multiplicity-adjusted p values; Flesion×chamber=44.72, df=1, 72, p<0.001) but exhibited normal sociability (trial 1). (OFC: Con, N=12; lesion, N=15. AI: Con, N=19; lesion, N=19.) In panel B, both OFC-lesioned (left) and AI-lesioned (right) mice exhibit normal performance in the five-trial recognition test with nonsocial cues. (OFC-Con, N=5; OFC-lesion, N=4; AI-Con, N=8; AI-lesion, N=8.) In panel C, OFC-lesioned mice exhibit normal performance in the five-trial recognition test with social cues (left), while AI-lesioned mice exhibit abnormal performance in the five-trial recognition test with social cues (right). AI-lesioned mice were significantly different from AI-Con (Flesion×trial=4.71, df=4, 56, p<0.01). (OFC-Con, N=5; OFC-lesion, N=4; AI-Con, N=8; AI-lesion, N=8.) Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Bonferroni post hoc test for panel A; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test for panels B and C. Error bars indicate standard deviation.
*p<0.05. ***p<0.001.

Alteration of RA Signaling in the AI Leads to Abnormal Social Novelty Recognition

To study the molecular, cellular, and circuitry mechanisms that underlie this AI-mediated behavior, we looked for a lead molecule to build a useful mouse model at both the basic and translational science levels. We hypothesized that the target would be enriched in the layer 5 pyramidal neurons of the AI, whose gene is significantly highlighted in genome-wide association studies (GWASs) for brain disorders. We combined the information from our own microarray data of fluorescence-activated cell sorting (FACS)-isolated AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons (see the supplementary methods section in the online supplement for details) and an anatomic gene expression atlas (34), which were cross-referenced with the GWAS risk genes so that we highlighted 47 candidate molecules (see Figure S3A in the online supplement). Following successful precedents in which mouse models that genetically modify enzymes critical to the biosynthesis of key mediators (e.g., serine racemase for d-serine and nitric oxide synthase for nitric oxide) have been highly useful in exploring neurobiological mechanisms (35, 36), we further narrowed the candidates down to two molecules that fall in this category, Cyp26B1 (cytochrome P450 26B1, a retinoic acid [RA]–degrading enzyme) (37, 38) and Ckb (creatine kinase brain type) (see Figure S3A in the online supplement). Cyp26B1 has been highlighted by an analysis using a comprehensive transcriptome study of postmortem brains from patients with autism spectrum disorder, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder (see Figure S3B in the online supplement) (39). Additionally, RA signaling is one of the top four signaling cascades in which risk genes for brain disorders are enriched (Figure 2A). Furthermore, we observed that a chronic infusion of RA locally into the AI resulted in a phenocopy of the AI lesion, exhibiting a significant deficit in social novelty recognition (trial 2) but not in sociability (trial 1) in the three-chamber social interaction test (Figure 2B; see also Figure S4A in the online supplement). Finally, we experimentally confirmed that Cyp26B1 is almost exclusively expressed in the pyramidal neurons at the deep cortical layers (layers 5 and 6) of the AI (Figure 2C–E). We therefore decided to study the unique role of the AI in social novelty recognition by using RA signaling, in particular Cyp26B1, as a molecular lead.
FIGURE 2. Retinoic acid (RA) signaling in the anterior insula (AI) underlies social novelty recognitiona
aIn panel A, RA signaling is underscored as one of the top four biological pathways in which risk genes for brain disorders are overrepresented. The significance of a potential overrepresentation in each signal transduction pathway is evaluated by the p value of the chi-square test. The negative logarithmic form of the p value (−log[p]) for each signal transduction pathway is shown on the x-axis. In panel B, chronic intra-insular administration of RA leads to a deficit in social novelty recognition (vehicle [Veh], p<0.001, and RA, p=0.47; Ftreat×chamber=8.01, df=1, 20, p<0.05), but not in sociability. (Veh, N=6; RA, N=6.) Panel C is a representative image of the adult mouse prefrontal cortex. The dotted rectangle in the low-magnification images indicates the position in the high-magnification images. Cyp26B1 mRNA (red) is visualized by an RNAscope probe. Nuclei are counterstained by DAPI (blue). Cyp26B1 expression is prominent in the deep layers of the AI. Scale bar: 1 mm. In panel D, cortical distribution of mRNAs for Cyp26B1 (red), Cux2 (magenta), and Bcl11b (a.k.a. Ctip2) (green) are visualized by RNAscope probes. Numerals on the left indicate layer numbers. Cyp26B1 expression is prominent in deeper layers of the AI. Nuclei are counterstained by DAPI (blue). Scale bar: 100 µm. Panel E shows co-visualization of Cyp26B1 with cell-type markers. The dotted rectangles in the larger, low-magnification images indicate the positions of the high-magnification images. mRNA for Cyp26B1 (red), Camk2a (green in left two images), and Gad2 (green in right two images) were visualized by RNAscope probes. Nuclei are counterstained by DAPI (blue). Cyp26B1 is co-localized with the Camk2a-positive pyramidal neurons, but not with the Gad2-positive interneurons. Scale bar: 50 μm. In panel F, reduced Cyp26B1 expression in the AI by RNAi-mediated knockdown (KD) causes a significant deficit (control [Con], p<0.001, KD, p=0.502, and rescue, p<0.001; FAAV×chamber=11.38, df=2, 60, p<0.001) in social novelty recognition, but not in sociability. (Con, N=11; KD, N=13; KD+Cyp26B1R [Rescue], N=9.) In panel G, Cyp26B1 conditional knockout specifically in the AI (cKO) phenocopies (Con, p<0.001, and cKO, p=0.545; FAAV×chamber=18.59, df=1, 42, p<0.001) the deficits of social novelty recognition that were found in the AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice. (AAV-αCaMKII-EYFP [Con], N=11; AAV-αCaMKII-GFP-Cre [cKO], N=12.) Two-way analysis of variance with Bonferroni post hoc test for panels B, F, and G. Error bars indicate standard deviation.
***p<0.001.
To directly examine the role of Cyp26B1 in the AI on social behavior, we bilaterally knocked down the expression of Cyp26B1 in the AI by adeno-associated virus (AAV)–mediated RNAi (AAV-U6-shCyp26B1-CMV-mCherrry [AAV-Cyp26B1 KD]) (see Figure S4B in the online supplement). Efficient knockdown (KD) of Cyp26B1 and a reciprocal augmentation of RA signaling were confirmed in vitro and in vivo at mRNA and protein levels (see Figure S5A–D in the online supplement). Consistent with the effect of local RA infusion into the AI (Figure 2B), Cyp26B1 KD in the AI (AI-Cyp26B1 KD) strongly induced a deficit in social novelty recognition without affecting sociability in the three-chamber social interaction test, whereas control mice (AI-Con) injected with AAV-U6-shControl-CMV-mCherrry (AAV-Con) displayed typical social behaviors (Figure 2F). Restoring the expression of Cyp26B1 by introducing RNAi-resistant Cyp26B1 (Cyp26B1R) with AAV-hSyn-Cyp26B1R-HA (AAV-Cyp26B1R) in the AI rescued the deficit in social novelty recognition elicited by Cyp26B1 loss-of-function (Figure 2F). The AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice also displayed recognition deficits in the context of social but not nonsocial cues (see Figure S5E in the online supplement). These data indicate the direct involvement of Cyp26B1 in the AI in social novelty recognition. To confirm and extend the results obtained from the RNAi approach, we used Cyp26B1 conditional knockout mice (40): we selectively depleted Cyp26B1 in αCaMKII-positive cells in the AI by injecting AAV-αCaMKII::GFP-Cre in the AI of Cyp26B1-floxed (Cyp26B1flox/flox) mice (see Figure S4C in the online supplement). The genetic depletion of Cyp26B1 in αCaMKII-positive cells (pyramidal neurons) of the AI led to deficits in social novelty recognition (Figure 2G), which is consistent with the phenotype in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice.

AI-Mediated Social Novelty Recognition Is Maintained by Proper Activity of the Layer 5 Pyramidal Neurons for Which RA-Mediated Gene Transcription Plays a Role

Because RA signaling is primarily involved in transcriptional regulation, we hypothesized that Cyp26B1 KD/KO affected these behavioral phenotypes via altered gene transcription. Thus, we performed an unbiased molecular profiling analysis of FACS-enriched AI cortical layer 5 pyramidal neurons. There was a small subset of differentially expressed genes between the control and KD groups (false discovery rate <0.1), while the expression profiles of control and rescue groups were similar, with good reproducibility among biological triplicates (Figure 3A). The majority of the genes we identified (15 of 20) included a retinoic acid response element (41) and/or were known to bind with retinoic acid receptors (RARs) (42) (see Table S1 in the online supplement), suggesting that these genes are likely to be regulated by RA signaling. Results from quantitative real-time PCR analyses were consistent with the molecular profiling data (see Figure S5F in the online supplement). Bioinformatic analyses strongly suggested that chronically upregulated RA signaling might cause abnormal dendritic spine regulation in the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons (Figure 3B). Consistent with this prediction, we experimentally confirmed that AI-Cyp26B1 KD caused a significant reduction in spine density in the proximal apical dendrites, compared to the control group, while the expression of Cyp26B1R effectively normalized this deficit (Figure 3C). These results suggest that the decreased expression of Cyp26B1 was the sole cause for the spine deficit.
FIGURE 3. Anterior insula (AI)–associated social novelty recognition is mediated by proper activity of the AI layer 5 pyramidal neuronsa
aPanel A is a heat map for differentially expressed genes from a molecular profiling analysis of FACS-enriched AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Three brains per group were used for sampling after injection of AAV-Con (Con), AAV-Cyp26B1 KD (Cyp26B1 KD), and AAV-Cyp26B1 KD+AAV-Cyp26B1R (Rescue), respectively. Expression levels are normalized using z score transformation and represented as a color scale of the z score. Panel B lists the top 10 terms of gene ontology enrichment analyses. Groups associated with dendritic spine plasticity are highlighted in red. In panel C, spine densities of the YFP-expressing layer 5 pyramidal neurons in the AI of Thy1-YFP-H transgenic mice are reduced by Cyp26B1 KD (F=20.67, df=2, 41, p<0.001). At left are representative fluorescence images of the proximal apical dendritic spines. Scale bar: 10 µm. The graph at right shows quantitative assessment of averaged spine densities from three to four brains per group. (Con, N=13; KD, N=16; Rescue, N=15 neurons.) In panel D, the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons infected with AAV-Cyp26B1 KD (KD) show a decrease in spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic current (sEPSC) frequency (left: t=0.22, df=19, p<0.05), but not amplitude (right), compared with neurons infected with AAV-Con (Con). At the top are representative sEPSC traces recorded from AAV-infected neurons. (Con, N=10 neurons from five mice; KD, N=11 neurons from four mice.) In panel E, acute retinoic acid (RA) (1 µM) treatment for 30 minutes does not change the frequency (left) or amplitude (right) of sEPSCs in the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons compared with vehicle-treated cells (Veh) from wild-type mice. At top are representative sEPSC traces recorded from neurons in brain slices treated with Veh or RA. (N=10 neurons from five mice per group.) In panel F, optogenetic excitation of the AI pyramidal neurons during the three-chamber social interaction test does not affect sociability (trial 1) in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice. Optogenetic excitation restores proper social novelty recognition that was deficient in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice (KD+ChR2: laser-OFF, p=0.615, and laser-ON, p<0.001; Flaser×chamber=8.65, df=1, 36, p<0.01). (KD+EYFP, N=8; KD+ChR2, N=10.) One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post hoc Tukey’s multiple comparison test for panel C; two-tailed t test for panels D and E; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc test for panel F. Error bars indicate standard deviation.
*p<0.05. ***p<0.001.
We also validated the spine changes at the electrophysiological level. The AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons in acute slices from AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice exhibited a significantly lower frequency of spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic currents (sEPSCs) compared to those from control mice, whereas both groups of neurons showed no difference in amplitude (Figure 3D). The decrease in sEPSC frequency in the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons implies that there would be reduced activity in these neurons and their downstream targets. In contrast, acute bath application of RA to brain slices from wild-type mice did not affect the sEPSCs of the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons (Figure 3E). The results of electrophysiological experiments taken immediately after the acute RA application may reflect acute changes in intracellular signal transduction but are unrelated to chronic influences elicited by RA, such as gene transcription-mediated spine changes. Accordingly, the contrastive results in electrophysiology between acute and chronic applications of RA imply that electrophysiological changes in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice are likely to reflect a functional outcome associated with the spine changes. Taken together, these findings show that proper control of RA signaling is critically required for the maintenance of dendritic architecture and activity of the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons. Thus, we optogenetically activated the AI pyramidal neurons during the three-chamber social interaction test in mice infected with Cyp26B1 KD AAV and channel rhodopsin (ChR2)-EYFP AAV (see Figure S6 in the online supplement). Optogenetic excitation of the AI pyramidal neurons did not affect sociability (trial 1) in the AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice (Figure 3F). In contrast, optogenetic excitation in the AI restored proper social novelty recognition through a decrease in time spent with a familiar mouse (trial 2) (Figure 3F). These data suggest that modulation of spontaneous neuronal activity of the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons could affect social novelty recognition.

OT Influences AI-Mediated Social Novelty Recognition

Next, we looked for circuit mechanisms by which the activity of AI pyramidal neurons is properly maintained in social contexts. We examined plausible afferent projections to the AI by using a retrograde tracing method in which we injected cholera toxin subunit B-conjugated with Alexa 488 (CTB-Alexa 488) into the AI (Figure 4A). As expected, we observed a strong signal from the contralateral AI and basolateral amygdala, well-known brain regions that tightly connect with the AI (Figure 4B) (13, 43). Then we assessed possible projections to the AI from the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and supraoptic nucleus (SON), where OT neurons originate (44). A recent publication reported that OT could directly influence social affective behavior mediated by the posterior insula (PI) (33). Nevertheless, it remains elusive how OT affects the function of the AI. It is well known that the AI and PI (see Figure S1A in the online supplement) are quite distinct anatomically and functionally in many aspects (45, 46). Several reports have indicated that the projection of OT neurons to the AI is quite sparse or negligible (24, 30). Our experimental data were indeed in accordance with these reports, indicating that there are no detectable projections to the AI from the hypothalamic PVN and SON (Figure 4C).
FIGURE 4. Anterior insula (AI)–mediated social novelty recognition is regulated by oxytocin (OT) in a nonclassic mechanisma
aPanel A is a representative image of the unilateral injection of green fluorescence retrograde tracer (CTB-Alexa 488) into the right AI. OFC=orbitofrontal cortex. Scale bar: 500 µm. Panels B and C are representative images showing retrograde-labeled green fluorescence–positive cell bodies in AI-projecting brain areas. The two brain regions (the contralateral AI and basolateral amygdala [BLA]) that are known to have afferent projections to the AI are displayed as positive controls (panel B), together with other brain regions of interest (panel C). Among the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN), paraventricular nucleus (PVN), and supraoptic nucleus (SON), green fluorescence retrogradely transported from the AI was observed only in the DRN. LA=lateral amygdala; Aq=aqueduct. Scale bars: 100 µm. Panel D shows the effect of OT and OT receptor antagonist (OTA) on social novelty recognition. Veh=vehicle. At left, bilateral local infusion of OT or OTA into the AI shows no effect on AI-Cyp26B1 KD (KD) or control (Con) mice, respectively. (KD-Veh, N=13; KD-OT, N=11; Con-Veh, N=11; Con-OTA, N=12.) At right, intracerebroventricular (i.c.v.) administration of OT normalizes the social novelty recognition deficit in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice (KD) (Veh, p=0.463, and OT, p<0.01; Fdrug×chamber=2.12, df=1, 36, p=0.154), while OTA disrupts proper social novelty recognition in healthy control mice (Con) (Veh, p<0.001, and OTA, p=0.507; Fdrug×chamber=7.63, df=1, 32, p<0.01). (KD-Veh, N=10; KD-OT, N=10; Con-Veh, N=9; Con-OTA, N=9.) Panel E shows the effect of OT and OTA on sociability. At left, local infusion of OT or OTA into the left and right AI shows no effect on sociability in both AI-Cyp26B1 KD and control mice. (KD-Veh, N=13; KD-OT, N=11; Con-Veh, N=11; Con-OTA, N=12.) At right, i.c.v. administration of OT has no effect on sociability in the AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice, while OTA disrupts sociability in control mice (Veh, p<0.001, and OTA, p=0.355; Fdrug×chamber=19.28, df=1, 32, p<0.001). (KD-Veh, N=10; KD-OT, N=10; Con-Veh, N=9; Con-OTA, N=9.) Two-way analysis of variance with Bonferroni post hoc test for panels D and E. Error bars indicate standard deviation.
**p<0.005. ***p<0.001.
If OT does not influence the AI neurons via direct projection of OT neurons, another scenario is via diffusion of OT into the AI. To test this possibility, we locally infused OT into the AI of AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice and examined whether exogenous OT could ameliorate the AI-associated social novelty recognition deficits (see Figure S7A in the online supplement). Local infusion of OT into the AI of AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice did not alter their deficit in social novelty recognition (Figure 4D). We also locally infused desGly-NH2-d(CH2)5 [D-Tyr2, Thr4] OVT, a selective OTR antagonist (OTA), into the AI of control mice and did not observe any changes in social behavior (Figure 4D; see also Figure S7A in the online supplement). Nevertheless, intracerebroventricular (i.c.v.) administration of OT normalized the social novelty recognition deficit through a decrease in time spent with a familiar mouse in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice in which RA signaling is selectively perturbed only in the AI (Figure 4D; see also Figure S7B in the online supplement). The i.c.v. administration of OTA also influenced social novelty recognition in control mice (Figure 4D; see also Figure S7B in the online supplement). These effects were selective to social novelty recognition, except i.c.v. administration of OTA, which also altered sociability (Figure 4E). Together, these results suggest that although OT specifically participates in the regulation of AI-mediated social novelty recognition, this is unlikely to be via the influence of OT directly on the AI neurons. Instead, OT may influence AI-mediated social novelty recognition indirectly, by remotely affecting the neurons that project to the AI from different brain regions.

Serotonin Influences AI-Associated Social Novelty Recognition Via 5-HT2C Receptor

In the retrograde tracing experiments to look for neuronal projections to the AI, we examined several brain regions in addition to the PVN and SON. Among them, we observed a distinct signal in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) (Figure 4C), which is known to provide major serotonergic projections to the forebrain. Roles of serotonin receptors in the AI in higher brain functions have been suggested (47, 48). Thus, we examined how modulation of serotonergic inputs might affect AI-mediated social novelty recognition. Based on their high expression levels in the AI (http://mouse.brain-map.org), we focused on two serotonin receptors (5-HT1A and 5-HT2C). First we tested the effects of their agonists and antagonists by systemic injection. Only CP 809101 (a 5-HT2C receptor agonist) normalized the social novelty recognition deficit in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice, whereas SB 242084 (a 5-HT2C receptor antagonist) disrupted social novelty recognition in control mice (see Figure S8A in the online supplement). To further validate whether the impact of the agonist and antagonist on behavior is directly mediated via the AI, we conducted local infusion experiments. Stimulating the AI serotonergic system via 5-HT2C normalized the social novelty recognition deficit in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice (Figure 5A), while inhibition of 5-HT2C in the AI of control mice impaired social novelty recognition (Figure 5A). Treatment with the 5-HT2C receptor agonist increased both the amplitude and frequency of sEPSCs in AI-Cyp26B1 KD pyramidal neurons without affecting presynaptic features (Figure 5B; see also Figure S8B in the online supplement). These data suggest that the 5-HT2C receptor agonist ameliorated deficits in the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons of AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice.
FIGURE 5. Anterior insula (AI)–mediated social novelty recognition is regulated by serotonin via the 5-HT2C receptor expressed in the AIa
aPanel A shows the AI 5-HT2C receptor as an intervention for altered social novelty recognition. In the graph at left, bilateral local infusion of the 5-HT2C receptor antagonist SB 242084 (SB) into the AI disrupts social novelty recognition in control mice (Con) (vehicle [Veh], p<0.001, and SB, p=0.718; Fdrug×chamber=17.51, df=1, 38, p<0.001). (Con-Veh, N=11; and Con-SB, N=11.) In the graph at right, bilateral local infusion of the 5-HT2C receptor agonist CP 809101 (CP) into the AI normalizes the altered social novelty recognition in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice (KD) (Veh, p=0.534, and CP, p<0.001; Fdrug×chamber=5.74, df=1, 48, p<0.05). (KD-Veh, N=13; KD-CP, N=13.) In panel B, acute activation of 5-HT2C receptors increases both the frequency (left: t=5.66, df=27, p<0.001) and the amplitude (right: t=3.96, df=27, p<0.001) of spontaneous excitatory postsynaptic currents (sEPSCs) in the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons from AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice. (KD+Veh, N=14 neurons from five mice; KD+CP, N=15 neurons from four mice.) In panel C, oxytocin (OT) and OT antagonist (OTA) in the dorsal raphe nucleus (DRN) modulates AI-mediated social novelty recognition. In the graph at left, local infusion of OTA into the DRN disrupts normal social novelty recognition in control mice (Con) (Veh, p<0.01, and OTA, p=0.385; Fdrug×chamber=1.96, df=1, 40, p=0.170). (Con-Veh, N=12; and Con-OTA, N=10.) In the graph at right, local infusion of OT into the DRN restores proper social novelty recognition in AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice (Veh, p=0.881, and OT, p<0.05; Fdrug×chamber=2.90, df=1, 30, p=0.099). (KD-Veh, N=9; KD-OT, N=8.) Two-way analysis of variance with Bonferroni post hoc test for panels A and C; two-tailed t test for panel B. Error bars indicate standard deviation.
*p<0.05. **p<0.005. ***p<0.001.

Modulation of DRN Neurons by OT Influences AI-Associated Social Novelty Recognition

We have shown that AI-mediated social novelty recognition is properly maintained by serotonergic regulation via 5-HT2C receptors expressed on AI pyramidal neurons. Meanwhile, the same social behavior is also regulated by OT, which likely plays a role in the AI by remotely affecting the neurons that project to the AI from different brain regions. Given that serotonergic innervation in the AI is from the DRN, one possible hypothesis is that OT directly influences the neurons in the DRN, which in turn regulates the activity of the AI pyramidal neurons. Indeed, OT neurons from the PVN reportedly project to the DRN (25), where OT receptors are reportedly highly expressed (49). To test our hypothesis, first we infused OTA specifically into the DRN of control mice and tested its effects on social novelty recognition. We observed that this treatment selectively impaired social novelty recognition (Figure 5C; see also Figure S9 in the online supplement). To test whether interfering with OT signaling in the DRN is specifically associated with the AI-mediated social novelty recognition, we infused OT specifically into the DRN of AI-Cyp26B1 KD mice. OT infusion into the DRN significantly restored social novelty recognition (Figure 5C; see also Figure S9 in the online supplement).

Discussion

In the present study, by using mouse models, we report a pivotal role of the AI in social novelty recognition in which proper activity of the layer 5 pyramidal neurons is required. At the molecular and cellular level, AI-mediated social novelty recognition is maintained by proper activity of AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons, for which retinoic acid–mediated gene transcription plays a role. Social novelty recognition is also regulated by 5-HT2C receptor expressed in the AI. Furthermore, we demonstrate that OT influences AI-mediated social cognition not by direct projection of OT neurons, nor by direct diffusion of OT in the AI, but rather by affecting OTR-expressing neurons in the DRN, where serotonergic neurons are projected to the AI. Accordingly, the regulatory mechanism by OT on the AI is distinct from that on the PI, where direct projection of OT neurons plays an important role.
In the brain, OT exerts its control through long-range axonal release, and hence projection of OT fibers and the expression of OTR in the target brain region is crucial. In this context, several brain regions have been postulated to be involved in OT-associated social cognition, including the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and olfactory nucleus (5053), and, more recently, the PI (33). Unlike the PI, projection of OT neurons to the AI is sparse (33), and the expression of OTR is attenuated (54). In human functional connectivity studies, there are distinct subdivisions in the insular cortex for processing cognitive information and affective feeling (13). Recent studies in rodent models have highlighted a role of the PI in social behavior (32, 33). It is an important future question whether and how the involvement of the AI and PI in social cognition and behavior is similar and different, possibly with distinct regulatory mechanisms for each subregion, including those by OT.
We underscored the significance of the 5-HT2C receptor expressed in the AI for social novelty recognition. In the past, through systemic administration of a 5-HT2C receptor antagonist in wild-type animals (55) or the phenotype of 5-HT2C receptor genetic knockout (56), the involvement of this receptor in both social novelty recognition and sociability has been suggested. In contrast to a broad expression of 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors in the frontal cortex (57, 58), the expression of 5-HT2C receptor is more restricted, with a more prominent expression in the medial prefrontal cortex and AI (58, 59). Our data in the present study showed that local injection of a 5-HT2C receptor antagonist elicits the deficits only in social novelty recognition, but not in sociability, which is compatible with our mechanistic proposal. Thus, the impact of 5-HT2C receptor in sociability may be via the receptor in the medial prefrontal cortex, which is known to be involved in sociability (60, 61). Genetic manipulations of this receptor in the AI pyramidal neurons can further validate its specific role in social novelty recognition.
The present study has pinned down the mechanistic question for the next step of how OT can influence AI-mediated social novelty recognition in a nonclassic way (i.e., neither OT neuron direct projection, nor direct diffusion of OT to the AI). One of the most tempting working hypotheses may be a disynaptic model in which the OTR-expressing DRN serotonin neurons, receiving input from PVN/SON-originated OT neurons, directly project to the AI pyramidal neurons and exert their effect on AI-dependent social novelty recognition (see Figure S10 in the online supplement). This model is theoretically addressable, although experimental optimization may be very difficult, by depleting OTR specifically from the AI-projecting DRN neurons and further rescuing the AI projection by activating this projection specifically using optogenetic manipulation. Through this experiment, the necessity and sufficiency of the disynaptic mechanism may be shown in future studies to account for the influence of OT on AI-mediated social novelty recognition. Alternatively, the AI may be influenced by OT through an indirect mechanism via a brain area to which OTR-expressing nonserotonergic neurons project from the DRN.
Future investigation may also address local mechanisms to a greater extent, for instance how AI pyramidal neuron activity is regulated for AI-mediated social novelty recognition. These mechanisms may include modulation of neuronal excitability, possibly mediated via G protein–coupled potassium current downstream of 5-HT2C receptor (62, 63), and local synaptic regulation via RA signaling. Future studies should also address downstream executive mechanisms whereby the AI pyramidal neurons eventually influence behavior. The functional relationship of the AI with the amygdala and hippocampus, in particular the CA2 (64) and ventral CA1 (65), may be topics of future investigations.
Although recent human genetic research has suggested the significance of RA-associated genes in brain disorders (66, 67), the biological role of the RA-associated cascade in mental conditions has not yet been fully addressed. Mild elevation of RA signaling by RARβ2 expression or by chronic neuronal inactivation has been reported to show a positive effect on neurite regeneration and homeostatic synaptic scaling (6871). In contrast, in our experimental mouse model, we chronically disturbed RA signaling by suppressing Cyp26B1 in the AI layer 5 pyramidal neurons. The present model will be useful in deciphering further mechanisms of how OT and serotonin regulate AI-mediated social cognition. The model will also be useful for studying the pathological impact of chronic disturbance in RA signaling in the AI. Consistent with this notion, in multiple expression studies of postmortem brains from patients with mental disorders (39), Cyp26B1 has been underscored as a top molecule of interest. Together, the Cyp26B1 models used in the present study will be useful to explore mechanisms of social cognition deficits in mental disorders.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Ms. Yukiko Y. Lema for organizing the manuscript, Dr. Melissa Landek-Salgado and Ms. Lauren Guttman for critical reading and discussion of the manuscript, and Dr. Tyler Cash-Padgett for technical assistance. The authors thank Dr. Maria I. Morasso (National Institute of Arthritis and Musculoskeletal and Skin Diseases) and Dr. Hiroshi Hamada (Riken) for providing mice carrying the floxed Cyp26B1 allele (Cyp26b1f/f). The human synapsin1 (hSyn) promoter-containing AAV plasmid (AAV-6P-SEWB) was a gift from Dr. Sebastian Kügler (University of Göttingen).

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.21010053.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Frith CD, Frith U: Mechanisms of social cognition. Annu Rev Psychol 2012; 63:287–313
2.
Rilling JK, Sanfey AG: The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu Rev Psychol 2011; 62:23–48
3.
Bora E, Pantelis C: Social cognition in schizophrenia in comparison to bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2016; 175:72–78
4.
Fett AKJ, Shergill SS, Krabbendam L: Social neuroscience in psychiatry: unravelling the neural mechanisms of social dysfunction. Psychol Med 2015; 45:1145–1165
5.
Meyer-Lindenberg A, Tost H: Neural mechanisms of social risk for psychiatric disorders. Nat Neurosci 2012; 15:663–668
6.
McCleery A, Lee J, Joshi A, et al: Meta-analysis of face processing event-related potentials in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2015; 77:116–126
7.
Weightman MJ, Air TM, Baune BT: A review of the role of social cognition in major depressive disorder. Front Psychiatry 2014; 5:179
8.
Gilboa-Schechtman E, Shachar-Lavie I: More than a face: a unified theoretical perspective on nonverbal social cue processing in social anxiety. Front Hum Neurosci 2013; 7:904
9.
Kohler CG, Walker JB, Martin EA, et al: Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr Bull 2010; 36:1009–1019
10.
Bicks LK, Koike H, Akbarian S, et al: Prefrontal cortex and social cognition in mouse and man. Front Psychol 2015; 6:1805
11.
Wilson CA, Koenig JI: Social interaction and social withdrawal in rodents as readouts for investigating the negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2014; 24:759–773
12.
Namkung H, Kim SH, Sawa A: The insula: an underestimated brain area in clinical neuroscience, psychiatry, and neurology. Trends Neurosci 2017; 40:200–207
13.
Uddin LQ, Nomi JS, Hebert-Seropian B, et al: Structure and function of the human insula. J Clin Neurophysiol 2017; 34:300–306
14.
Craig AD: Once an island, now the focus of attention. Brain Struct Funct 2010; 214:395–396
15.
Craig ADB: How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10:59–70
16.
Gogolla N: The insular cortex. Curr Biol 2017; 27:R580–R586
17.
Phelps EA, Lempert KM, Sokol-Hessner P: Emotion and decision making: multiple modulatory neural circuits. Annu Rev Neurosci 2014; 37:263–287
18.
Downar J, Blumberger DM, Daskalakis ZJ: The neural crossroads of psychiatric illness: an emerging target for brain stimulation. Trends Cogn Sci 2016; 20:107–120
19.
Singer T, Critchley HD, Preuschoff K: A common role of insula in feelings, empathy, and uncertainty. Trends Cogn Sci 2009; 13:334–340
20.
Uddin LQ: Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat Rev Neurosci 2015; 16:55–61
21.
Donaldson ZR, Young LJ: Oxytocin, vasopressin, and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science 2008; 322:900–904
22.
Ferguson JN, Young LJ, Hearn EF, et al: Social amnesia in mice lacking the oxytocin gene. Nat Genet 2000; 25:284–288
23.
Rilling JK, Young LJ: The biology of mammalian parenting and its effect on offspring social development. Science 2014; 345:771–776
24.
Knobloch HS, Charlet A, Hoffmann LC, et al: Evoked axonal oxytocin release in the central amygdala attenuates fear response. Neuron 2012; 73:553–566
25.
Weissbourd B, Ren J, DeLoach KE, et al: Presynaptic partners of dorsal raphe serotonergic and GABAergic neurons. Neuron 2014; 83:645–662
26.
Jin D, Liu HX, Hirai H, et al: CD38 is critical for social behaviour by regulating oxytocin secretion. Nature 2007; 446:41–45
27.
Cataldo I, Azhari A, Esposito G: A review of oxytocin and arginine-vasopressin receptors and their modulation of autism spectrum disorder. Front Mol Neurosci 2018; 11:27
28.
Febo M, Numan M, Ferris CF: Functional magnetic resonance imaging shows oxytocin activates brain regions associated with mother-pup bonding during suckling. J Neurosci 2005; 25:11637–11644
29.
Bartels A, Zeki S: The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage 2004; 21:1155–1166
30.
Rogers CN, Ross AP, Sahu SP, et al: Oxytocin- and arginine vasopressin-containing fibers in the cortex of humans, chimpanzees, and rhesus macaques. Am J Primatol 2018; 80:e22875
31.
Dumais KM, Bredewold R, Mayer TE, et al: Sex differences in oxytocin receptor binding in forebrain regions: correlations with social interest in brain region- and sex-specific ways. Horm Behav 2013; 64:693–701
32.
Gehrlach DA, Dolensek N, Klein AS, et al: Aversive state processing in the posterior insular cortex. Nat Neurosci 2019; 22:1424–1437
33.
Rogers-Carter MM, Varela JA, Gribbons KB, et al: Insular cortex mediates approach and avoidance responses to social affective stimuli. Nat Neurosci 2018; 21:404–414
34.
Ng L, Bernard A, Lau C, et al: An anatomic gene expression atlas of the adult mouse brain. Nat Neurosci 2009; 12:356–362
35.
Inoue R, Mori H: Serine racemase knockout mice: neurotoxicity, epilepsy, and schizophrenia, in D-Amino Acids: Physiology, Metabolism, and Application. Edited by Yoshimura T, Nishikawa T, Homma H. Tokyo, Springer, 2016, pp 119–136
36.
Steinert JR, Chernova T, Forsythe ID: Nitric oxide signaling in brain function, dysfunction, and dementia. Neuroscientist 2010; 16:435–452
37.
MacLean G, Abu-Abed S, Dolle P, et al: Cloning of a novel retinoic-acid metabolizing cytochrome P450, Cyp26B1, and comparative expression analysis with Cyp26A1 during early murine development. Mech Dev 2001; 107:195–201
38.
Ross AC, Zolfaghari R: Cytochrome P450s in the regulation of cellular retinoic acid metabolism. Annu Rev Nutr 2011; 31:65–87
39.
Gandal MJ, Zhang P, Hadjimichael E, et al: Transcriptome-wide isoform-level dysregulation in ASD, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder. Science 2018; 362:1265
40.
Okano J, Lichti U, Mamiya S, et al: Increased retinoic acid levels through ablation of Cyp26b1 determine the processes of embryonic skin barrier formation and peridermal development. J Cell Sci 2012; 125:1827–1836
41.
Lalevee S, Anno YN, Chatagnon A, et al: Genome-wide in silico identification of new conserved and functional retinoic acid receptor response elements (direct repeats separated by 5 bp). J Biol Chem 2011; 286:33322–33334
42.
Moutier E, Ye T, Choukrallah MA, et al: Retinoic acid receptors recognize the mouse genome through binding elements with diverse spacing and topology. J Biol Chem 2012; 287:26328–26341
43.
Saper CB: Convergence of autonomic and limbic connections in the insular cortex of the rat. J Comp Neurol 1982; 210:163–173
44.
Swanson LW, Sawchenko PE: Hypothalamic integration: organization of the paraventricular and supraoptic nuclei. Annu Rev Neurosci 1983; 6:269–324
45.
Chang LJ, Smith A, Dufwenberg M, et al: Triangulating the neural, psychological, and economic bases of guilt aversion. Neuron 2011; 70:560–572
46.
Nomi JS, Schettini E, Broce I, et al: Structural connections of functionally defined human insular subdivisions. Cereb Cortex 2018; 28:3445–3456
47.
Cavalcante LES, Zinn CG, Schmidt SD, et al: Modulation of the storage of social recognition memory by neurotransmitter systems in the insular cortex. Behav Brain Res 2017; 334:129–134
48.
Ju A, Fernandez-Arroyo B, Wu YF, et al: Expression of serotonin 1A and 2A receptors in molecular- and projection-defined neurons of the mouse insular cortex. Mol Brain 2020; 13:99
49.
Yoshida M, Takayanagi Y, Inoue K, et al: Evidence that oxytocin exerts anxiolytic effects via oxytocin receptor expressed in serotonergic neurons in mice. J Neurosci 2009; 29:2259–2271
50.
Takayanagi Y, Yoshida M, Takashima A, et al: Activation of supraoptic oxytocin neurons by secretin facilitates social recognition. Biol Psychiatry 2017; 81:243–251
51.
Oettl LL, Ravi N, Schneider M, et al: Oxytocin enhances social recognition by modulating cortical control of early olfactory processing. Neuron 2016; 90:609–621
52.
Tan Y, Singhal SM, Harden SW, et al: Oxytocin receptors are expressed by glutamatergic prefrontal cortical neurons that selectively modulate social recognition. J Neurosci 2019; 39:3249–3263
53.
Raam T, McAvoy KM, Besnard A, et al: Hippocampal oxytocin receptors are necessary for discrimination of social stimuli. Nat Commun 2017; 8:2001
54.
Yoshimura R, Kiyama H, Kimura T, et al: Localization of oxytocin receptor messenger-ribonucleic-acid in the rat-brain. Endocrinology 1993; 133:1239–1246
55.
Kennett GA, Wood MD, Bright F, et al: SB 242084, a selective and brain penetrant 5-HT2C receptor antagonist. Neuropharmacology 1997; 36:609–620
56.
Sejourne J, Llaneza D, Kuti OJ, et al: Social behavioral deficits coincide with the onset of seizure susceptibility in mice lacking serotonin receptor 2c. PLoS One 2015; 10:e0136494
57.
Li QH, Nakadate K, Tanaka-Nakadate S, et al: Unique expression patterns of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors in the rat brain during postnatal development: Western blot and immunohistochemical analyses. J Comp Neurol 2004; 469:128–140
58.
Santana N, Bortolozzi A, Serrats J, et al: Expression of serotonin1a and serotonin2a receptors in pyramidal and GABAergic neurons of the rat prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 2004; 14:1100–1109
59.
Santana N, Artigas F: Expression of serotonin2C receptors in pyramidal and GABAergic neurons of rat prefrontal cortex: a comparison with striatum. Cereb Cortex 2017; 27:3125–3139
60.
Paine TA, Swedlow N, Swetschinski L: Decreasing GABA function within the medial prefrontal cortex or basolateral amygdala decreases sociability. Behav Brain Res 2017; 317:542–552
61.
Yamamuro K, Bicks LK, Leventhal MB, et al: A prefrontal-paraventricular thalamus circuit requires juvenile social experience to regulate adult sociability in mice. Nat Neurosci 2020; 23:1240–1252
62.
Austgen JR, Dantzler HA, Barger BK, et al: 5-Hydroxytryptamine 2C receptors tonically augment synaptic currents in the nucleus tractus solitarii. J Neurophysiol 2012; 108:2292–2305
63.
Qiu J, Xue CH, Bosch MA, et al: Serotonin 5-hydroxytryptamine2C receptor signaling in hypothalamic proopiomelanocortin neurons: role in energy homeostasis in females. Mol Pharmacol 2007; 72:885–896
64.
Hitti FL, Siegelbaum SA: The hippocampal CA2 region is essential for social memory. Nature 2014; 508:88–92
65.
Okuyama T, Kitamura T, Roy DS, et al: Ventral CA1 neurons store social memory. Science 2016; 353:1536–1541
66.
Reay WR, Atkins JR, Quide Y, et al: Polygenic disruption of retinoid signalling in schizophrenia and a severe cognitive deficit subtype. Mol Psychiatry 2020; 25:719–731
67.
Reay WR, Cairns MJ: The role of the retinoids in schizophrenia: genomic and clinical perspectives. Mol Psychiatry 2020; 25:706–718
68.
Maden M: Retinoic acid in the development, regeneration, and maintenance of the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007; 8:755–765
69.
Aoto J, Nam CI, Poon MM, et al: Synaptic signaling by all-trans retinoic acid in homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neuron 2008; 60:308–320
70.
Chen L, Lau AG, Sarti F: Synaptic retinoic acid signaling and homeostatic synaptic plasticity. Neuropharmacology 2014; 78:3–12
71.
Yip PK, Wong LF, Pattinson D, et al: Lentiviral vector expressing retinoic acid receptor beta2 promotes recovery of function after corticospinal tract injury in the adult rat spinal cord. Hum Mol Genet 2006; 15:3107–3118

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 305 - 317
PubMed: 36128683

History

Received: 14 January 2021
Revision received: 8 April 2022
Accepted: 17 May 2022
Published online: 21 September 2022
Published in print: April 01, 2023

Keywords

  1. Anterior Insula
  2. Social Cognition
  3. Oxytocin
  4. Serotonin
  5. Retinoic Acid

Authors

Details

Sun-Hong Kim, Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Kyongman An, Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Ho Namkung, Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Atsushi Saito, M.D., Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Matthew D. Rannals, Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
James R. Moore, B.S.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Marina Mihaljevic, M.D., Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Sneha Saha, M.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Seyun Oh, M.S.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Mari A. Kondo, Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Koko Ishizuka, M.D., Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Kun Yang, Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Brady J. Maher, Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Minae Niwa, Ph.D.
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).
Akira Sawa, M.D. [email protected]
Departments of Psychiatry (Kim, An, Namkung, Saito, Moore, Mihaljevic, Saha, Oh, Kondo, Ishizuka, Yang, Maher, Niwa, Sawa), Neuroscience (Maher, Sawa), Biomedical Engineering (Namkung, Sawa), Pharmacology (Sawa), and Genetic Medicine (Sawa), Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore; Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore (Sawa); Lieber Institute for Brain Development, Johns Hopkins Medical Campus, Baltimore (Rannals, Oh, Maher); Neuroscience Research Australia, Sydney (Kondo); Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurobiology, University of Alabama at Birmingham School of Medicine, Birmingham (Niwa).

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Sawa ([email protected]).

Author Contributions

Drs. Kim and An contributed equally to this study; Drs. Maher and Niwa contributed equally to this study.

Competing Interests

The authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

Supported by NIMH grants MH-094268 and MH-105660 (to Dr. Sawa), as well as foundation grants from Stanley (to Dr. Sawa), RUSK/S-R (to Dr. Sawa), and NARSAD (to Dr. Sawa and Dr. Kim).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share